Canadian English as a Window To the Rise of Like In Discourse

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Canadian English as a Window To the Rise of Like In Discourse"

Transcription

1 1 Canadian English as a Window To the Rise of Like In Discourse 1. Introduction Both the uses and the users of like in discourse have received a growing amount of attention over the past two and half decades. This attention has not been restricted to linguistics, but has appeared in an array of contexts, from mainstream media sources such as internet, television, and radio, to usage guides. What many of these discussions have in common is a finger pointed steadfastedly on younger speakers and an insistence that like is used fairly haphazardly (e.g. Chapman 1986; Wilson 1987; Diamond 2000; Fought 2006; see also citations in Levey 2003). The question that must be asked is whether it makes sense, linguistically-speaking, to assume that today's "unpliant young" (Wilson 1987, 92) should bear sole responsibility for this putatively random feature of the spoken vernacular? Presumably those who do use like did not invent it ex nihilo. The issue is all the more interesting when we consider that the discourse functions of like are ubiquitous across varieties of English. But why focus on like in Canadian English when the lens is generally aimed at its neighbour to the south? The answer is quite simple. The vernacular forms of like are shared by both varieties and comparison of past research reveals regular trends across American and Canadian Englishes (e.g. Schourup 1983; Underhill 1988; Blyth, Recktenwald, and Wang 1990; Meehan 1991; Romaine and Lange 1991; Dailey-O'Cain 2000; Buchstaller 2001; Cukor-Avila 2002; Tagliamonte and D'Arcy 2004, 2007; D'Arcy 2005, 2006). Furthermore, the Toronto English Archive, housed in the Sociolinguistics Laboratory at the University of Toronto, allows for apparent-time analysis of like across the full age spectrum. This enables detailed examination of the rise of like within a well-defined community, one which is representative of contemporary, urban Canadian English. What we find is that like has been developing systematically in the vernacular for as long as can be ascertained using apparent-time data, giving us a view of how it has come to its current state of use. In other words, Canadian English provides a window to the rise of like in discourse, one that we can use to extrapolate to other varieties and locales. 2. The Forest For the Trees A central tenet of the variationist paradigm is that heterogeneity is an inherent aspect of the linguistic system, and, like other aspects of the grammar, this variability is structured (Weinreich, Labov, and Herzog 1968). This notwithstanding, when used in the ways demonstrated in (1), like has eluded traditional linguistic description, appearing more ad hoc than systematic. (1) a. Like if you're doing your undergrad, no big deal. Like it's not that bad, but like I'm in a professional school. (N/f/26) 1 1 All examples are drawn from the Toronto English Archive, housed in the Sociolinguistics Laboratory at the University of Toronto. The parenthetical information following each example marks the subcorpus from which the datum was extracted, followed by the speaker's sex and age.

2 2 b. Like the first hour I was like totally fine, like I wasn't like drunk. (3/m/18) c. As long as they like try to like merge with Canadian culture [ ]. (I/m/22) d. I remember there being like a solar eclipse. (I/f/29) Not included in (1) is quotative be like, as in (2), or adverbial like, as in (3). The former serves the well-defined role of introducing constructed dialogue and can (more than likely) be traced to American English during the late 1970s or early 1980s (D'Arcy 2007; Tagliamonte and D'Arcy 2007). The latter represents a lexical replacement for about, functioning as an approximative adverb in the spoken vernacular (D'Arcy 2006; see also Schourup 1983, 30 and Underhill 1988, 234). These two functions do not form part of the following discussion. The functions in (1), comprising the discourse marker (1a) and the discourse particle (1b-d), are the focus of this chapter and are referred to here collectively as (discourse) like. (2) a. I was like "Where do you find these people?" (I/f/19) b. When you go there, you're like "Hi, how are you?" and talk to them and stuff like that. (2/f/16) c. And when I came home I'd be like "Yeah, I can't wait to go home." And she's like "You are home, this is your home." (N/f/26) (3) a. And usually it takes like fifteen minutes to get there. (3/f/12) b. They're like sixteen feet long. (I/m/32) c. He got to high-school when he was like twelve. (N/f/72) Part of the difficulty in assessing possible constraints on like is the wide range of surface constructions in which it appears. Media characterizations typically reference discourse uses as random and 'anti-verbal': "[t]o the common ear, the word seems just flung in" (Diamond 2000, 2; see also Fought 2006). In the linguistic literature, like has been discussed in terms of "syntactic detachability and positional mobility" (Romaine and Lange 1991, 261) and its ability to "occur grammatically anywhere in a sentence" (Siegel 2002, 64). This is not to suggest that concerted efforts to pin down possible conditions have not been made. Quite the contrary is true, and I am thinking in particular of work by Ross and Cooper (1979), Underhill (1988), and Andersen (2001). Focusing on its functions as focus particle and pausal interjection, Ross and Cooper argue that like opens a constituent that dominates the focused element (1979, 349). In a similar vein, Underhill suggests that like is "closely rule-governed" because "it always or nearly always introduces a constituent" (1988, 243). Taking a somewhat different approach, Andersen (2001, 275) argues that the position of like is dependent on phrase type. For example, like is more likely to occur before a determiner than a head noun, but in the case of verb phrases, it typically appears immediately before the head. What all three analyses hold in common is the view that like is associated with the left periphery of the element over which it scopes. I return to this point below. However, as a syntactic category, the constituent is generic and vague (see, e.g., Adger 2003, 64), while Andersen's Principle of Lexical Attraction seems stipulative rather than systematic. Moreover, in the case of Underhill (1988) and Andersen (2001), no consideration is given to the places where like does not occur; the focus is strictly on those in which it appears. The end result is a compilation of syntagmatic combinatorial possibilities (e.g. like can appear before or within a noun phrase, before or within a verb phrase, before or within a prepositional phrase, before an adverb, etc.). Such an

3 CANADIAN ENGLISH AS A WINDOW TO THE RISE OF LIKE IN DISCOURSE 3 approach places emphasis on token types, but provides neither a motivation nor an explanation for the patterns attested, unattested, and even unattestable 2 found in natural language data. At the same time, it brings descriptions of like full circle in that we are left with a snapshot of the places like appears in sentence structure, the same apparently unsystematic view with which we began. The possibility of order and systematicity in the discursive uses of like has been further obfuscated by concentrated attention on specific subsections of the population, generally preadolescents and adolescents, or, in the rare case, young adults (e.g. Underhill 1988). Miller and Weinert (1995), for example, considered the use of like by preadolescents in the 8 to 13 year old age-bracket, Siegel (2002) used her 15 year old daughter's high-school friends as informants, and Andersen (1997, 1998, 2000, 2001) relied on the Bergen Corpus of London Teenage Language (COLT), which consists of data from teenagers aged 13 to 17. Thus, while a number of researchers have considered quotative be like (2) from a generational perspective (e.g. Ferrara and Bell 1995; Cukor-Avila 2002; Buchstaller 2004, 2006; Barbieri 2007; Tagliamonte and D'Arcy 2007) and D'Arcy (2006) examined approximative adverbial like in apparent-time, analyses of like as a discourse marker and particle have largely ignored older speakers. A notable exception to this trend is Dailey-O'Cain (2000), though her focus was attitudinal rather than distributional. Even Buchstaller (2001), which provides a perspective on the use of discourse like within a single family, is limited to one college-age woman and three middle-aged adults (late 40s to mid 50s). In sum, the investigative focal point has been speakers who are believed to be the primary if not the sole users of like. The underlying assumption seems to be that like is simply age-graded slang that disappears from the vernacular when it is no longer age-appropriate. The drawback to this perspective is that until adolescents are seen in relation to older segments of the population, patterns of use among those who are purported to use like cannot be contextualized. This is a point that bears directly on issues of language change, stability, and age grading, as well as those surrounding our understanding of the genesis of like in discourse. Discourse like is not, in fact, a recent innovation. Grammarians and language commentators have been remarking on like since at least the first half of the nineteenth century (e.g. De Quincey , 224; Robinson 1876, 76; Jespersen 1942, 417) and it was used by the first generation of native New Zealanders born in the period from 1851 to 1919 (D'Arcy 2007). Today it is used by septa- and octagenarians living in isolated, rural towns and villages in England, Ireland, and Scotland (D'Arcy 2005, 2007; Tagliamonte to appear). In North America, like has been associated with both the jazz, cool, and Beat groups of New York City during the 1950s and 1960s (see Andersen 2001, 216, and references therein; also Chapman 1986, 259) and the California Valley Girls of the 1980s (Dailey-O'Cain 2000). Thus, despite the perception of newness, like is anything but. 3 Contemporary youth have not invented discourse like; they got it from somewhere and they learned to use it from someone. 2 For this distinction, see Hyman See Buchstaller and Traugott (2006) for discussion of adverbial all (e.g. John is all wet), another longstanding feature of English that is considered new due to its association with the genuinely innovative form quotative be all. The same process is likely at work here, with quotative be like drawing a perceptual generalization of recent innovation across all discourse uses of like (see D'Arcy 2007).

4 4 As such, let us assume orderly heterogeneity (Weinreich et al. 1968, 100). There is a variable grammar for like and speakers who use like have acquired that grammar. In short, it is time to stop looking at the trees and figure out where the forest came from. How did like, in its present state, come to be? 3. Methodological Considerations 3.1. Data and Method The current discussion draws on corpus data from a large archive of informal, spoken English. The materials were collected in Toronto, Canada in the period between 2002 and 2004, using a combination of quota-based random sampling and social networking. 4 The full archive comprises over 350 hours of casual conversational data with speakers between the ages of 9 and 92, all of whom were born and raised in the city; the sample used for the current analysis is outlined in Table 1. Table 1: The Toronto English Sample Age Male Female Total total Toronto presents an ideal context in which to examine contemporary urban vernacular usage. The city has the largest metropolitan population in Canada. In the broader North American context, it is fourth largest: in the U.S. only New York, Los Angeles, and Chicago have larger populations (WorldAtlas.Com 2006). Although the national varieties of Canada and the U.S. differ in a number of respects, the Toronto English Archive is taken here to represent North American English more generally on the basis of two factors. One, as noted in the introduction, there is striking consistency across observations about discourse like regardless of geographic locale (e.g. Schourup 1983; Underhill 1988; Meehan 1991; Romaine and Lange 1991; Dailey-O'Cain 2000; D'Arcy 2005, 2006). Two, models of spatial diffusion highlight the crucial role of cities in the spread of linguistic features (Trudgill 1974; Bailey, Wilke, Tillery, and Sand 1994; Labov 2003). It is typically the case that new forms spread hierarchically from an originating centre. Although some changes are seemingly arrested by national boundaries (e.g. the Northern Cities Shift), others are not (e.g. uvular (r) in Europe; 4 For discussion of the design and construction of the individual corpora within the Toronto English Archive, see Tagliamonte and D'Arcy (2004) and Tagliamonte (2006).

5 CANADIAN ENGLISH AS A WINDOW TO THE RISE OF LIKE IN DISCOURSE 5 Trudgill 1974). Discourse like belongs to this second category. Methodologically, the analytical approach of the current discussion differs from previous research on like in three crucial respects. First, the age range in Table 1 enables an apparent-time perspective of like use in which not just younger speakers are accounted for, but rather, all age cohorts from age 10 through 87 are included. Second, I carefully circumscribe the variable context according to structural diagnostics within functional domains. That is, rather than looking only at those places where like appears (e.g. before a noun phrase), the structure of individual syntactic complexes becomes the key heuristic. The question then becomes: What does it mean for like to be before a noun phrase as opposed to being within one? As will be shown, examining like in this way has important ramifications for uncovering its developmental trajectory in the vernacular. The third methodological difference falls out from the third: both contexts where like does and does not appear are considered. The net result was analysis of more than 20,000 tokens comprising seven syntactic contexts; the details are provided in Table 2. Table 2: Summary Of Data (D'Arcy 2005, 205) Syntactic Domain Projection N clausal: clause-initial CP, TP 5,737 nominal: determiner phrase DP, NP 4,408 adjectival: (predicate) DegP, AP 4,298 verbal: (light) verb phrase νp 5,843 total 20,286 This methodology has been described in detail in D'Arcy (2005) and is too elaborate to repeat in full here. The crucial point is that the delimitation of the envelope of variation along syntactic parameters allows for objective analysis of like following the principle of accountability (Labov 1972, 72). Occurrences of like are contrasted with whatever form with which they may alternate in any given context, including other discourse markers (you know, well) as well as nothing. Exemplification is provided in (4), which illustrates the matrix level, clause-initial context. (4) a. Ø Nobody said a word. Like my first experience with death was this Italian family. (N/f/82) b. Ø I love Carrie. Like Carrie's like a little like out-of-it [ ] Like she's a space-cadet. (3/f/18) c. You know, like the people were very, very friendly. You know, Ø we'd sit out in the park [...]. (N/f/60) d. Ø We used to go to these crazy after-hour bars. Like you know, transvestites and all the strippers were there. (N/m/26) e. And Ø my other cat always sleeps, and like we almost never see him. (3/m/11) f. Like it's not that bad, but like I'm in a professional school. (N/f/26) This approach offers a unique perspective on the use of like in the community and exposes details previously unavailable for consideration.

6 Discourse Markers vs. Discourse Particles Before continuing, it is important to clarify what is both a theoretical and terminological distinction between discourse markers, as in (1a) and (4), and discourse particles, as in (1b-d). Markers tend to occur in clause-initial position where they bracket elements of talk (e.g. Schiffrin 1987, 31). Such bracketing may be local, linking contiguous utterances (1a), but discourse markers can also function globally, linking non-contiguous stretches of discourse (see Schiffrin 1992). Their function is thus textual, as they relate the current utterance to prior discourse by signaling exemplification, clarification, elaboration, etc. (see Fraser 1988, 1990; Traugott 1997 [1995]; Brinton 1996, 2006). In this respect discourse markers are "essential to the rhetorical shape of any argument or narrative" (Traugott and Dasher 2005, 154) and because they are interactional signposts, Schiffrin (1987) refers to them as discourse deictics. Other English markers include so, then, and well, as well as parentheticals such as I/you know, I guess, and I think (Traugott and Dasher 1995; Brinton 1996). In fact, as exemplified in (5), these last can often be felicitously substituted for like without affecting the epistemic stance of the utterance. (5) a. Like one of my cats meows so much, 'cause like he's really picky and everything. (3/m/11) b. I mean one of my cats meows so much, 'cause you know he's really picky and everything. Structurally, both Kiparsky (1995, ) and Traugott (1997 [1995], 6) have argued that markers occupy a specific syntactic slot: they adjoin to the left periphery of CP, the functional projection that dominates the clause (i.e. clause-initial position, (1a), (4) and (5)). In other words, discourse markers are considered to be adjuncts. Treating them in this way (as opposed to, for example, the head of their own functional category) has repercussions for the analysis presented here. In the architecture of the Minimalist framework (Chomsky 1995, 2000, 2001), adjunction occurs at the phrasal level (i.e. XP). By defining the variable context for like along structural parameters, for example AP or NP, the adjunction site is implicationally assumed to be the maximal projection heading each structure. Particles have functions that are primarily interpersonal, such as marking focus or the speaker's epistemic stance toward the utterance, aiding cooperative aspects of communication (e.g. checking or expressing understanding), and generating a sense of sharing or intimacy between interlocutors (Östman 1982; Schourup 1983, 1999; Schiffrin 1987; Andersen 1997, 2001). Indeed, given that interactions in which particles are not used can be perceived as unnatural, awkward, dogmatic, or even unfriendly (Brinton 1996, 35), their discourse saliency is quite high. 5 Among the pragmatic functions proposed for like, the most frequent include pausal interjection (Schourup 1983), focus (Underhill 1988), and non-equivalence between form and intention (Schourup 1983; Andersen 1997, 1998, 2001). Structurally, like as a particle occurs in clause- 5 See also Dailey-O'Cain (2000) for positive solidarity ratings for like guises as opposed to non-like guises.

7 CANADIAN ENGLISH AS A WINDOW TO THE RISE OF LIKE IN DISCOURSE 7 internal positions. In sum, discourse markers and discourse particles are differentiated here with regard to both pragmatic function and syntactic position: markers have textual functions and occur in clause-initial position, particles have interpersonal functions and occur in positions other than clause-initial. A similar distinction is made by Andersen (2001, 275), who differentiates between clause-external contexts (i.e. the marker) and clauseinternal contexts (i.e. the particle). Why is this important? That is, why not collapse the marker and the particle as a monolithic entity? There are two reasons. One, a growing body of work concentrates on the development of discourse markers (e.g. Traugott 1997 [1995]; Brinton 2005, 2006, and references therein), and two, as will be seen here, there is evidence that long before like functioned as a particle, it was a marker (see D'Arcy 2007 for more elaborated evidence to this end). The basic argument is that the starting point for discourse uses of like was the left edge of CP, where like functions as a discourse marker, and lower projections, where like functions as a discourse particle, gradually came available through generalization. If we follow this through, we find that the current state of like, where it appears in a variety of syntactic positions, has arisen systematically. In other words, there is nothing haphazard about like use at all. 4. Looking Through the Apparent-Time Window As just discussed, there has been a recent focus on discourse markers as well as claims that they adjoin to syntactic structure in a specific (and historically long-standing) 6 position. Moreover, a number of authors have noted that like is highly frequent in clause-initial position (e.g. Underhill 1988; Romaine and Lange 1991; Andersen 2001). In Andersen (2001, 273), for example, one third of all tokens occur in this position. These points combine to make the marker, and therefore the CP context, an appropriate point of departure for an accountable, and structurally delimited, analysis of discourse like. An analytical advantage of examining like from a syntactic perspective is that it allows for distinctions between different levels of structure. In the case of the CP, this means that matrix and subordinate clauses can be treated separately. The examples in (4) illustrate matrix structures. There are two types of subordinate structures: embedded complement clauses, as in (6), and various types of adjunct clauses, such as those in (7) and (8). (6) a. He couldn't believe [that like he's held all this animosity]... (I/m/22) b. I think [that like there's been a desire instilled in me]. (I/f/21) c. But then I was really lucky [that like it was starting to go down]. (3/m/15) (7) a. Like one of my cats meows so much ['cause like he's really picky and everything]. (3/m/11) b. I'm going to go buy Canadian stuff ['cause like I'm proud to be Canadian]. (2/m/15) c. It's weird [because like you didn't really fit in the Black group]. (I/f/21) (8) a. [Like when I first heard that] I was still teaching. (N/m/64) b. So I get it all done [like when I get home]. (3/f/17) 6 See Kiparsky (1995).

8 8 c. [Like if you drive up Elgin or Arnold or whatever] there was just always these big monster homes. (I/f/21) d. There's still some [like if you go up on Kipling]. (I/m/40) Note the position of like relative to the left periphery of the clause in these examples. In (4) and (8), like is adjoined immediately on the left edge of the clause. This is canonical CP-adjunction. 7 In (6) and (7), however, a different pattern is exhibited: like cannot be adjoined to CP because it appears to the right of the element in the head of CP (i.e. (be)cause and that), on the immediate left of the subject. That such should be the case at first seems odd: because and that occupy the same slot as do if and when (also after, while, until, etc.). Moreover, these are categorical patterns in the data. This is where the perspective afforded by apparent-time is particularly helpful, as it helps shed light on what has happened. Figure 1 tracks the frequency of like as a discourse marker in the three contexts illustrated above: on the left periphery of matrix CPs (4), on the left periphery of subordinate CPs (8), and in the position illustrated in (6) and (7), which I suggest is the left periphery of TP, the functional projection that hosts the subject and which dominates all verbal projections (Pollock 1989). The results in figure 1 reveal that like is already established on matrix clauses among the oldest generation of Torontonians sampled here, speakers in their eighties. In contrast, neither the subordinate CP context nor the other subordinate context is established among this same group of speakers: like never occurs in these positions. In fact, like does not occur on subordinate CPs until the 60 year olds, while the first time like is used in the other subordinate position, what I suggest is TP, is among the 40 year olds. In all cases, the apparent-time trajectory is one of regular, incremental increases in frequency among subsequent age groups. 8 In sum, figure 1 suggests that as a discourse marker, the use of like with subordinate clauses is a later development, one that follows its use with matrix clauses by a number of decades. Among eighty year olds, like occurs on matrix CPs at a rate of 8% overall (N = 299). To put this in context, the overall distribution of all other discourse markers combined in this age group is just 11%. In other words, like accounts for nearly half of discourse marker use by the octogenarians in this data set, providing compelling evidence that it is already fairly robust on matrix CPs among the oldest speakers in the community. That like should first target matrix clauses is consistent with what is already known about the development of discourse markers more generally (see, e.g., Traugott 1997 [1995]; Brinton 2005, 2006): the first place they appear is on the left edge of sentences, adjoined to CP. Since most matrix clauses are initial, it 7 Adjunction can apply iteratively, but unlike adverb interpolation, it is not required to follow a fixed hierarchy. As such, when like co-occurs with other discourse markers, as in (4c,d), the linear relationship of the markers vis-à-vis one another is not predicted to follow any particular order. That is, you know like (4c) is no more probable than like you know (4d), a fact that is reflected by the marker collocations in the current data set (e.g. I mean like/like I mean, etc.). In contrast, the syntagmatic order of like and conjunctions is fixed: like categorically follows the conjunction (see also Andersen 2001, 285). This too is predictable from structure, since in coordinate constructions, the conjunction is situated above the projection it conjoins. When CPs are conjoined and like marks the lower clause, as in (4e,f), like will necessarily occur after the conjunction because the conjunction is hierarchically located above the CP level. 8 In contrast, the frequency of other discourse markers is constant across the sample (see D'Arcy 2005, 85-86).

9 CANADIAN ENGLISH AS A WINDOW TO THE RISE OF LIKE IN DISCOURSE 9 follows that the earliest and most established context for like as a marker will be matrix-level CPs. From here, like subsequently generalized to subordinate clauses, but it did not target all embedded clauses evenly. It first spread to temporal and conditional clauses of the type in (8), where it continues to adjoin to CP, only later spreading to clauses of the type illustrated in (6) and (7), where it targets the lower functional projection, TP. In short, the evidence suggests a developmental trajectory of matrix CP to subordinate CP to TP. Fig. 1: Like In Apparent-Time: Discourse Marker The question is, how and where do the clause-internal positions fit in, where like functions interpersonally as a discourse particle? Figure 2 displays the apparent-time distribution of like in four representative positions in the clause-internal context. DP, exemplified in (9), is the functional projection that dominates noun phrases. NP, seen in (10), is the lexical projection that contains the head noun. The light verb, νp, seen in (11), is the functional projection that is located hierarchically between the tense phrase (TP, which hosts auxiliaries and other functional categories in the verb phrase; the subject is located in SpecTP) and the lexical verb projection. DegP, as in (12), is the functional projection that subcategorizes for an adjective phrase (Abney 1987). This latter context, AP, is not included in figure 2 due to the extremely low frequency at which like occurs unambiguously on the left edge of the adjective. Nonetheless, a few examples are given in (13), which can be contrasted with those in (12). (9) a. Well you just cut out like a girl figure and a boy figure and then you'd cut out like a dress or a skirt or a coat, and like you'd colour it. (N/f/75) b. We stayed at like a motel. (N/f/76) c. They didn't have windows. They had like a box. (N/m/62) d. Then there's like a guy with like a bell and like a hood. (2/m/18) (10) a. They didn't want to go to like a horrible crappy little like Firkin Bar or something like that. (N/m/26) b. They have this like energy you-know? (I/f/21) c. Like there s a lot of like house-boaters. (I/f/21) d. We um bought some like treats for ourselves. (3/f/12) (11) a. They were like living like dogs. (N/m/52) b. And they had like scraped her. (I/m/35) c. I've like grown into that. (3/m/12) d. Everyone is like calling stuff out. (I/m/22)

10 10 e. They like to like intervene a lot. (3/m/18) f. As long as they like try to like merge with Canadian culture. (I/m/22) (12) a. I've discovered new reading material which is like so interesting. (N/f/52) b. Like I just thought it was like so small. (I/m/40) c. They're like really quiet. (2/f/16) d. Like my computer's like really damn slow. (2/m/15) (13) a. I remember certain things that certain staff have done because they were just so incredibly like oblivious to their role. (I/f/29) b. I get really like flabbergasted that they know the things that they know. (I/f/24) c. My whole mouth was getting incredibly like dry. (3/m/18) d. He's in rehab now and he's all like humble. (2/f/16) e. Like the person who teaches it is really like nice. (2/f/12) Fig. 2: Like In Apparent-Time: Discourse Particle Figure 2 reveals that none of these contexts are attested among the eighty year olds. However, beginning with speakers in their seventies, the first clause-internal position to host like is the DP. The next projection to host like is DegP, where it first occurs in the speech of the 65 year olds, followed shortly thereafter by the light verb with the 50 year olds. Finally, among the 30 year olds, like begins to adjoin to NP. For all contexts, however, regardless of the entry point in the apparent-time trajectory, the frequency of like rises consistently across the generations. This same pattern was also evident for like as a discourse marker in figure 1. The perspective provided by apparent-time is thus one that shows quite clearly that like did not come to its present state as a spontaneous innovation in the speech of adolescents and young adults. Nor would it be fair to say that it is a strictly age-graded feature of the vernacular: it is not restricted to any particular life-stage. It is true that in all contexts, like is more frequent among younger speakers than it is among older speakers, but crucially, its use is not isolated to any particular age group. Instead, we see that once like begins to occur in any given context, it continues to do so: there is continuity across apparent-time. In other words, there is a stability here that might not have been expected. Were like a fairly ad hoc and random feature of discourse, there would be no reason to suspect the kind of systematicity unveiled by the apparent-time windows in figures 1 and 2, let alone the monotonic relationship between use and age that is evident for each context tracked in this data set. The apparent-time data therefore reveal that although like may seem random, there has been nothing random about its development as a pragmatic device. It has risen to its current state through regular, step-wise development, consistent with change in progress.

11 CANADIAN ENGLISH AS A WINDOW TO THE RISE OF LIKE IN DISCOURSE The Rise Of Like In Discourse Figure 3 abstracts away from issues of frequency, isolating the point at which each context is first attested in the Toronto materials and after which it continues to occur in the speech of subsequent generations. This layering of adjunction sites, which has developed systematically in Canadian English over the past sixty-five years or so, graphically demonstrates how like has arrived at its current state of use. AP NP TP νp DegP CP sub DP CP matrix > c.1930s c.1940s c.1950s c.1960s c.1970s c.1980s c.1990s Fig. 3: Generalization of like across maximal projections: darker shading = marker; lighter shading = particle (based on D'Arcy 2005, 209) As discussed above, like is firmly entrenched as a marker on matrix CPs among the oldest speakers in the sample, suggesting that this was the entry point for the discourse uses of like more generally. From here, the marker generalized to the periphery of subordinate CPs before later spreading to the edge of TP. In the interim, however, like began to work its way into the syntactic hierarchy as a particle, adjoining to maximal projections below the clausal level. Its first target was the DP and from this position it continued to spread deeper into the syntax, generalizing across other levels of structure: DegP, νp, NP, AP. These pathways are interesting in and of themselves, since it is clear that from clause-external positions like generalized to clause-internal ones, and it did so incrementally rather than all at once. This suggests that there are grammatical imperatives at work. However, the story is more complex than this. The ordering of the adjunction sites is not random. Note the types of maximal projections targeted by like. CP, TP, DP, DegP, and νp are all functional categories. NP and AP are lexical categories. There are at least two distinct developmental pathways evidenced in the data. On the one hand, there is generalization from higher functional projections to lower functional projections in the syntactic hierarchy. In the case of the marker, for example, we see generalization from CP to TP. In the case of the particle, we see generalization from DP to DegP to νp. On the other hand, there is generalization from the higher functional projection to the lower lexical projection within a domain. In other words, the lexical projections signify later stages of development. It is not a coincidence that of the contexts examined here, NP and AP are the last two to appear (cf. figure 3). Consider, for example, the nominal domain. DP first appears as a target for like adjunction among speakers in their seventies. Unambiguous evidence for adjunction on the left edge of NP, however, does not appear until the thirty year olds. In other words,

12 12 there is approximately a 40-year gap between adjunction on the functional projection and adjunction on the lower lexical projection it dominates. Similarly, the difference between DegP-adjunction and AP-adjunction is one of approximately 35 years: like adjoins to DegP among speakers aged 65 years and below, but does not unambiguously occur on the edge of AP until speakers aged 29 and under. This regularity, demonstrated so unequivocally by the pathways that emerge from the apparent-time perspective, provide dramatic evidence that like was not a feature that simply appeared ex nihilo in the vernacular. Quite the contrary is true. It has been developing gradually and systematically as a feature of Canadian discourse for minimally the period we can track with this corpus, and it has done so following clear grammatical imperatives. Add to this the observation that once like shows up in a given context, that particular projection is firmly established as an adjunction site. In other words, after like generalizes to a new projection, it not only continues to appear in that position among successive generations, but its frequency steadily increases across apparent-time. What all of this points to is linguistic change, not age grading, not slang, and not some trendy marker of youthfulness. In short, like is not a passing fad of the adolescent years. It is incontrovertible that younger speakers use like more frequently than older speakers do (see also Dailey-O'Cain 2000, 66). It is also incontrovertible, however, that adolescents are not the sole users of like. Other age groups use it as well, they simply do so at lower rates. In fact, while like does not occur in every context for all age cohorts, it is nonetheless frequency rather than position that primarily differentiates a 15 year old from, for example, a 45 year old. This suggests that the someone from whom younger speakers acquired like consists of older speakers and that the somewhere from which they learned to use it was the community's variable grammar. 6. Concluding Remarks At the outset of this chapter, I had asked if it made good linguistic sense not only to attribute like to a narrowly circumscribed sector of the population, but also to assume that its use is haphazard and unconstrained. A fundamental principle of language change is that incoming forms will be most frequent among younger speakers, while the variationist paradigm is based on the assumption that linguistic variability is structured. The apparent-time window of the present study has provided persuasive evidence that the answer to both aspects of this question is a resolute "no". Discourse like is a relatively long-standing feature of Canadian vernacular speech. 9 Despite appearances, it is not an ad hoc feature of the spoken language. The problem is that when we take a snapshot of younger speakers alone, we literally cannot see the forest for the trees. To get a true picture of what is going on in speech, it is necessary to step back and take in a more panoramic view. The full community must be considered, oldest speakers to youngest speakers. When we do that with a feature such as like, we discover that the current state of affairs represents the results of long-standing developments within the grammar. The synchronic state of like, where it surfaces in a broad range of syntactic positions, did not emerge all at once as a fully-fledged system. 9 For more elaborated discussions of the history of like as a vernacular form in English, see Romaine and Lange (1991, 270) and D'Arcy (2007).

13 CANADIAN ENGLISH AS A WINDOW TO THE RISE OF LIKE IN DISCOURSE 13 Quite the contrary: it has developed systematically, projection by projection. In sum, like has reached its current pattern of use in Canadian English via regular, step-wise development; it is not a random discourse device of the inarticulate. That this is so for this variety suggests that it is also the case for others, which may be at different stages along the trajectory and which may, as a consequence, evidence fewer or possibly more adjunction sites in vernacular use. Works Cited Abney, Steven. The English Noun Phrase in its Sentential Aspect. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Massachusetts: MIT, Adger, David. Core Syntax: A Minimalist Approach. Oxford: Oxford University Press, Andersen, Gisle. "They gave us these yeah, and they like wanna see like how we talk and all that: The Use of 'like' and Other Discourse Markers in London Teenage Speech." Eds. Ulla- Britt Kostinas, Anna-Brita Stenström and Anna-Malin Karlsson, Ungdomsspråk i Norden. MINS 43. Stockholm: Stockholms universitet, Institutionene für nordiska språk, "The Pragmatic Marker like from a Relevance-Theoretic Perspective." Eds. Andreas Jucker and Yael Ziv, Discourse Markers: Descriptions and Theory. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, "The Role of Pragmatic Marker like in Utterance Interpretation." Eds. Gisle Andersen and Thorstein Fretheim, Pragmatic Markers and Propositional Attitude. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, Pragmatic Markers and Sociolinguistic Variation. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, Bailey, Guy, Tom Wilke, Jan Tillery, and Lori Sand. "Some Patterns of Linguistic Diffusion." Language Variation and Change 5 (1994): Barbieri, Federica. "Older Men and Younger Women: A Corpus-Based Study of Quotative Use in American English." English World-Wide 28 (2007): Blyth, Carl, Sigrid Recktenwald, and Jenny Wang. "I'm like, 'say what?!': A New Quotative in American Oral Narrative." American Speech 65 (1990): Brinton, Laurel. Pragmatic Markers in English: Grammaticalization and Discourse Functions. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, "Processes Underlying the Development of Pragmatic Markers: The Case of '(I) say'. Eds. J. Skaffari, M. Peikola, R. Carroll, R. Hiltunen, and B. Wårvik, Opening Windows on Texts and Discourses of the Past. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins, "Pathways in the Development of Pragmatic Markers in English." Eds. A. van Kemenade and B. Los, Handbook on the History of the English Language. London: Blackwell, Buchstaller, Isa. "An Alternative View of like: Its Grammaticalization in Conversational American English and Beyond." Edinburgh Working Papers in Applied Linguistics 11 (2001): "The Sociolinguistic Constraints on the Quotative System: British English and US English Compared." Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Edinburgh: University of Edinburgh, "Diagnostics of Age-Graded Behavior: The Case of the Quotative System." Journal of Sociolinguistics 10 (2006): Chapman, Robert. New Dictionary of American Slang. New York: Harper and Row, Chomsky, Noam. The Minimalist Program. Cambridge: MIT Press, "Minimalist Inquiries: The Framework." Step by Step: Essays on Minimalist Syntax in Honour of Howard Lasnik. Eds. R. Martin, D. Michaels and J. Uriagereka. Cambridge: MIT Press, Cukor-Avila, Patricia. "She say, She go, She be like: Verbs of Quotation over Time in African

14 14 American Vernacular English." American Speech 77 (2002): Dailey-O'Cain, Jennifer. "The Sociolinguistic Distribution of and Attitudes Toward Focuser like and Quotative like." Journal of Sociolinguistics 4 (2000): D'Arcy, Alexandra. "Like: Syntax and Development." Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Toronto: University of Toronto, "Lexical Replacement and the Likes." American Speech 81 (2006): "Like and Language Ideology: Disentangling Fact from Fiction." American Speech 82 (2007). De Quincey, Thomas. "Style." Blackwood's Magazine, ; reprinted in De Quincey's Works, vol. 10, Style and Rhetoric and Other Essays. Edinburgh: Black, Diamond, S.J. "Like It or Not, like is Probably Here to Stay." The Los Angeles Times, 21 August 2000: 2. Ferrara, Kathleen and Barbara Bell. "Sociolinguistic Variation and Discourse Function of Constructed Dialogue Introducers: The Case of be + like." American Speech 70 (1995): Fought, Carmen. "Like It or Not, a Discourse Marker Making its Mark on a Wider Stage." Voice of American News. Accessed 28 September Fraser, Bruce. "Types of English Discourse Markers." Acta Linguistica Hungaria 38 (1988): "An Approach to Discourse Markers." Journal of Pragmatics 14 (1990): Hyman, Larry. "The limits of phonetic determinism in phonology. *NC revisited". The Role of Speech Perception in Phonology. Eds. Elizabeth Hume and Keith Johnson. San Diego: Academic Press, Jespersen, Otto. A Modern English Grammar on Historical Principles, Part VI, Morphology. Copenhagen: Ejnar Munksgaard, Kiparsky, Paul. "Indo-European Origins of Germanic Syntax." Clause Structure and Language Change. Eds. Adrian Battye and Ian Roberts. Oxford: Oxford University Press, Labov, William. Sociolinguistic Patterns. Philadelphia: University of Philadelphia Press, Principles of Linguistic Change: Social Factors. Cambridge: Blackwell, "Pursuing the cascade model." Social Dialectology: In Honour of Peter Trudgill. Eds. David Britain and Jenny Cheshire. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, Levey, Stephen. "He's like 'Do it now!' and I'm like 'No!': Some Innovative Quotative Usage among Young People in London." English Today 19 (2003): Meehan, Teresa. "It's like, 'What's happening in the evolution of like?': A Theory of Grammaticalization." Kansas Working Papers in Linguistics 16 (1991): Miller, Jim and Regina Weinert. "The Function of like in Dialogue." Journal of Pragmatics 23 (1995): Östman, Jan-Ola. "The Symbiotic Relationship between Pragmatic Particles and Impromptu Speech." Impromptu Speech: A Symposium. Ed. N.E. Enkvist. Åbo: Åbo Akademi, Pollock, Jean-Yves. "Verb Movement, Universal Grammar, and the Structure of IP." Linguistic Inquiry 20 (1989): Robinson, C. Clough. A Glossary of Words Pertaining to the Dialect of Mid-Yorkshire, English Dialect Society no. 14. London: Trübner, Romaine Suzanne and Deborah Lange. "The Use of like as a Marker of Reported Speech and Thought: A case of Grammaticalization in Progress." American Speech 66 (1991):

15 CANADIAN ENGLISH AS A WINDOW TO THE RISE OF LIKE IN DISCOURSE 15 Ross, J.R. and W.E. Cooper. "Like, Syntax." Sentence Processing: Psycholinguistic Studies Presented to Merrill Garrett. Eds. W.E. Cooper and E.C.T. Walker. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, Schiffrin, Deborah. Discourse Markers. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, "Anaphoric then: Aspectual, Textual, and Epistemic Meaning. Linguistics 30 (1992): Schourup, Lawrence. Common Discourse Particles in English Conversation. Ohio State Working Papers in Linguistics 28, "Discourse Markers." Lingua 107 (1999): Siegel, Muffy. "'Like': The Discourse Particle and Semantics. Journal of Semantics 19 (2002): Tagliamonte, Sali A. "So cool, right: Canadian English Entering the 21st Century." Canadian English in the Global Context. Eds. Peter Avery, Alexandra D Arcy, Elaine Gold, and Keren Rice. Theme issue of Canadian Journal of Linguistics 51 (2006): Roots of English: Exploring the History of Dialects. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, to appear. and Alexandra D'Arcy. "He's like, she's like: The Quotative System in Canadian Youth." Journal of Sociolinguistics 8 (2004): "Frequency and Variation in the Community Grammar: Tracking a New Change through the Generations." Language Variation and Change 19 (2007): Traugott, Elizabeth C. "The Role of the Development of Discourse Markers in a Theory of Grammaticalization." Paper presented at the Twelfth International Conference on Historical Linguistics, University of Manchester, 1997 [1995]. Available online at: < edu/~traugott/ect-papersonline.html.>. and Richard B. Dasher. Regularity in Semantic Change. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, Trudgill, Peter. "Linguistic Change and Diffusion: Description and Explanation in Sociolinguistic Dialect Geography." Language in Society 3 (1974): Underhill, Robert. "Like is Like, Focus." American Speech 63 (1988): Weinreich, Uriel, William Labov and Marvin Herzog. "Empirical Foundations for a Theory of Language Change." Eds. W.P. Lehmann & Y. Malkiel (eds.), Directions for Historial Linguistics. Austin: University of Texas Press, WorldAtlas.Com. "Largest Cities of the World: By Population." < (27 September 2006). Wilson, Kenneth G. Van Winkle's Return: Change in American English, Hanover: University Press of New England, 1987.

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many Schmidt 1 Eric Schmidt Prof. Suzanne Flynn Linguistic Study of Bilingualism December 13, 2013 A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one.

More information

Possessive have and (have) got in New Zealand English Heidi Quinn, University of Canterbury, New Zealand

Possessive have and (have) got in New Zealand English Heidi Quinn, University of Canterbury, New Zealand 1 Introduction Possessive have and (have) got in New Zealand English Heidi Quinn, University of Canterbury, New Zealand heidi.quinn@canterbury.ac.nz NWAV 33, Ann Arbor 1 October 24 This paper looks at

More information

Discourse markers and grammaticalization

Discourse markers and grammaticalization Universidade Federal Fluminense Niterói Mini curso, Part 2: 08.05.14, 17:30 Discourse markers and grammaticalization Bernd Heine 1 bernd.heine@uni-keln.de What is a discourse marker? 2 ... the status of

More information

The Effect of Discourse Markers on the Speaking Production of EFL Students. Iman Moradimanesh

The Effect of Discourse Markers on the Speaking Production of EFL Students. Iman Moradimanesh The Effect of Discourse Markers on the Speaking Production of EFL Students Iman Moradimanesh Abstract The research aimed at investigating the relationship between discourse markers (DMs) and a special

More information

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm Kansas Working Papers in Linguistics, Vol. 0 (008), p. 8 Abstract Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm Yuwen Lai and Jie Zhang University of Kansas Research on spoken word recognition

More information

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first Minimalism Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first introduced by Chomsky in his work The Minimalist Program (1995) and has seen several developments

More information

Linguistic Variation across Sports Category of Press Reportage from British Newspapers: a Diachronic Multidimensional Analysis

Linguistic Variation across Sports Category of Press Reportage from British Newspapers: a Diachronic Multidimensional Analysis International Journal of Arts Humanities and Social Sciences (IJAHSS) Volume 1 Issue 1 ǁ August 216. www.ijahss.com Linguistic Variation across Sports Category of Press Reportage from British Newspapers:

More information

AN INTRODUCTION (2 ND ED.) (LONDON, BLOOMSBURY ACADEMIC PP. VI, 282)

AN INTRODUCTION (2 ND ED.) (LONDON, BLOOMSBURY ACADEMIC PP. VI, 282) B. PALTRIDGE, DISCOURSE ANALYSIS: AN INTRODUCTION (2 ND ED.) (LONDON, BLOOMSBURY ACADEMIC. 2012. PP. VI, 282) Review by Glenda Shopen _ This book is a revised edition of the author s 2006 introductory

More information

SOME MINIMAL NOTES ON MINIMALISM *

SOME MINIMAL NOTES ON MINIMALISM * In Linguistic Society of Hong Kong Newsletter 36, 7-10. (2000) SOME MINIMAL NOTES ON MINIMALISM * Sze-Wing Tang The Hong Kong Polytechnic University 1 Introduction Based on the framework outlined in chapter

More information

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections Tyler Perrachione LING 451-0 Proseminar in Sound Structure Prof. A. Bradlow 17 March 2006 Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections Abstract Although the acoustic and

More information

Multiple case assignment and the English pseudo-passive *

Multiple case assignment and the English pseudo-passive * Multiple case assignment and the English pseudo-passive * Norvin Richards Massachusetts Institute of Technology Previous literature on pseudo-passives (see van Riemsdijk 1978, Chomsky 1981, Hornstein &

More information

Progressive Aspect in Nigerian English

Progressive Aspect in Nigerian English ISLE 2011 17 June 2011 1 New Englishes Empirical Studies Aspect in Nigerian Languages 2 3 Nigerian English Other New Englishes Explanations Progressive Aspect in New Englishes New Englishes Empirical Studies

More information

What Women are Saying About Coaching Needs and Practices in Masters Sport

What Women are Saying About Coaching Needs and Practices in Masters Sport 2016 Coaching Association of Canada, ISSN 1496-1539 July 2016, Vol. 16, No. 3 What Women are Saying About Coaching Needs and Practices in Masters Sport As the Coaching Association of Canada notes*, Masters

More information

Ch VI- SENTENCE PATTERNS.

Ch VI- SENTENCE PATTERNS. Ch VI- SENTENCE PATTERNS faizrisd@gmail.com www.pakfaizal.com It is a common fact that in the making of well-formed sentences we badly need several syntactic devices used to link together words by means

More information

Basic Syntax. Doug Arnold We review some basic grammatical ideas and terminology, and look at some common constructions in English.

Basic Syntax. Doug Arnold We review some basic grammatical ideas and terminology, and look at some common constructions in English. Basic Syntax Doug Arnold doug@essex.ac.uk We review some basic grammatical ideas and terminology, and look at some common constructions in English. 1 Categories 1.1 Word level (lexical and functional)

More information

An Introduction to the Minimalist Program

An Introduction to the Minimalist Program An Introduction to the Minimalist Program Luke Smith University of Arizona Summer 2016 Some findings of traditional syntax Human languages vary greatly, but digging deeper, they all have distinct commonalities:

More information

The Structure of Relative Clauses in Maay Maay By Elly Zimmer

The Structure of Relative Clauses in Maay Maay By Elly Zimmer I Introduction A. Goals of this study The Structure of Relative Clauses in Maay Maay By Elly Zimmer 1. Provide a basic documentation of Maay Maay relative clauses First time this structure has ever been

More information

Agree or Move? On Partial Control Anna Snarska, Adam Mickiewicz University

Agree or Move? On Partial Control Anna Snarska, Adam Mickiewicz University PLM, 14 September 2007 Agree or Move? On Partial Control Anna Snarska, Adam Mickiewicz University 1. Introduction While in the history of generative grammar the distinction between Obligatory Control (OC)

More information

Argument structure and theta roles

Argument structure and theta roles Argument structure and theta roles Introduction to Syntax, EGG Summer School 2017 András Bárány ab155@soas.ac.uk 26 July 2017 Overview Where we left off Arguments and theta roles Some consequences of theta

More information

Pragmatic Functions of Discourse Markers: A Review of Related Literature

Pragmatic Functions of Discourse Markers: A Review of Related Literature International Journal on Studies in English Language and Literature (IJSELL) Volume 3, Issue 3, March 2015, PP 1-10 ISSN 2347-3126 (Print) & ISSN 2347-3134 (Online) www.arcjournals.org Pragmatic Functions

More information

Constraining X-Bar: Theta Theory

Constraining X-Bar: Theta Theory Constraining X-Bar: Theta Theory Carnie, 2013, chapter 8 Kofi K. Saah 1 Learning objectives Distinguish between thematic relation and theta role. Identify the thematic relations agent, theme, goal, source,

More information

English Language and Applied Linguistics. Module Descriptions 2017/18

English Language and Applied Linguistics. Module Descriptions 2017/18 English Language and Applied Linguistics Module Descriptions 2017/18 Level I (i.e. 2 nd Yr.) Modules Please be aware that all modules are subject to availability. If you have any questions about the modules,

More information

Introduction to HPSG. Introduction. Historical Overview. The HPSG architecture. Signature. Linguistic Objects. Descriptions.

Introduction to HPSG. Introduction. Historical Overview. The HPSG architecture. Signature. Linguistic Objects. Descriptions. to as a linguistic theory to to a member of the family of linguistic frameworks that are called generative grammars a grammar which is formalized to a high degree and thus makes exact predictions about

More information

Formulaic Language and Fluency: ESL Teaching Applications

Formulaic Language and Fluency: ESL Teaching Applications Formulaic Language and Fluency: ESL Teaching Applications Formulaic Language Terminology Formulaic sequence One such item Formulaic language Non-count noun referring to these items Phraseology The study

More information

Underlying and Surface Grammatical Relations in Greek consider

Underlying and Surface Grammatical Relations in Greek consider 0 Underlying and Surface Grammatical Relations in Greek consider Sentences Brian D. Joseph The Ohio State University Abbreviated Title Grammatical Relations in Greek consider Sentences Brian D. Joseph

More information

Approaches to control phenomena handout Obligatory control and morphological case: Icelandic and Basque

Approaches to control phenomena handout Obligatory control and morphological case: Icelandic and Basque Approaches to control phenomena handout 6 5.4 Obligatory control and morphological case: Icelandic and Basque Icelandinc quirky case (displaying properties of both structural and inherent case: lexically

More information

Listening and Speaking Skills of English Language of Adolescents of Government and Private Schools

Listening and Speaking Skills of English Language of Adolescents of Government and Private Schools Listening and Speaking Skills of English Language of Adolescents of Government and Private Schools Dr. Amardeep Kaur Professor, Babe Ke College of Education, Mudki, Ferozepur, Punjab Abstract The present

More information

Derivational and Inflectional Morphemes in Pak-Pak Language

Derivational and Inflectional Morphemes in Pak-Pak Language Derivational and Inflectional Morphemes in Pak-Pak Language Agustina Situmorang and Tima Mariany Arifin ABSTRACT The objectives of this study are to find out the derivational and inflectional morphemes

More information

Literature and the Language Arts Experiencing Literature

Literature and the Language Arts Experiencing Literature Correlation of Literature and the Language Arts Experiencing Literature Grade 9 2 nd edition to the Nebraska Reading/Writing Standards EMC/Paradigm Publishing 875 Montreal Way St. Paul, Minnesota 55102

More information

AN EXPERIMENTAL APPROACH TO NEW AND OLD INFORMATION IN TURKISH LOCATIVES AND EXISTENTIALS

AN EXPERIMENTAL APPROACH TO NEW AND OLD INFORMATION IN TURKISH LOCATIVES AND EXISTENTIALS AN EXPERIMENTAL APPROACH TO NEW AND OLD INFORMATION IN TURKISH LOCATIVES AND EXISTENTIALS Engin ARIK 1, Pınar ÖZTOP 2, and Esen BÜYÜKSÖKMEN 1 Doguş University, 2 Plymouth University enginarik@enginarik.com

More information

The College Board Redesigned SAT Grade 12

The College Board Redesigned SAT Grade 12 A Correlation of, 2017 To the Redesigned SAT Introduction This document demonstrates how myperspectives English Language Arts meets the Reading, Writing and Language and Essay Domains of Redesigned SAT.

More information

Chapter 3: Semi-lexical categories. nor truly functional. As Corver and van Riemsdijk rightly point out, There is more

Chapter 3: Semi-lexical categories. nor truly functional. As Corver and van Riemsdijk rightly point out, There is more Chapter 3: Semi-lexical categories 0 Introduction While lexical and functional categories are central to current approaches to syntax, it has been noticed that not all categories fit perfectly into this

More information

CEFR Overall Illustrative English Proficiency Scales

CEFR Overall Illustrative English Proficiency Scales CEFR Overall Illustrative English Proficiency s CEFR CEFR OVERALL ORAL PRODUCTION Has a good command of idiomatic expressions and colloquialisms with awareness of connotative levels of meaning. Can convey

More information

Advanced Grammar in Use

Advanced Grammar in Use Advanced Grammar in Use A self-study reference and practice book for advanced learners of English Third Edition with answers and CD-ROM cambridge university press cambridge, new york, melbourne, madrid,

More information

Review in ICAME Journal, Volume 38, 2014, DOI: /icame

Review in ICAME Journal, Volume 38, 2014, DOI: /icame Review in ICAME Journal, Volume 38, 2014, DOI: 10.2478/icame-2014-0012 Gaëtanelle Gilquin and Sylvie De Cock (eds.). Errors and disfluencies in spoken corpora. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 2013. 172 pp.

More information

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form Orthographic Form 1 Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form The development and testing of word-retrieval treatments for aphasia has generally focused

More information

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 141 ( 2014 ) WCLTA Using Corpus Linguistics in the Development of Writing

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 141 ( 2014 ) WCLTA Using Corpus Linguistics in the Development of Writing Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 141 ( 2014 ) 124 128 WCLTA 2013 Using Corpus Linguistics in the Development of Writing Blanka Frydrychova

More information

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 154 ( 2014 )

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 154 ( 2014 ) Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 154 ( 2014 ) 263 267 THE XXV ANNUAL INTERNATIONAL ACADEMIC CONFERENCE, LANGUAGE AND CULTURE, 20-22 October

More information

Writing a composition

Writing a composition A good composition has three elements: Writing a composition an introduction: A topic sentence which contains the main idea of the paragraph. a body : Supporting sentences that develop the main idea. a

More information

A cautionary note is research still caught up in an implementer approach to the teacher?

A cautionary note is research still caught up in an implementer approach to the teacher? A cautionary note is research still caught up in an implementer approach to the teacher? Jeppe Skott Växjö University, Sweden & the University of Aarhus, Denmark Abstract: In this paper I outline two historically

More information

Developing a TT-MCTAG for German with an RCG-based Parser

Developing a TT-MCTAG for German with an RCG-based Parser Developing a TT-MCTAG for German with an RCG-based Parser Laura Kallmeyer, Timm Lichte, Wolfgang Maier, Yannick Parmentier, Johannes Dellert University of Tübingen, Germany CNRS-LORIA, France LREC 2008,

More information

Age Effects on Syntactic Control in. Second Language Learning

Age Effects on Syntactic Control in. Second Language Learning Age Effects on Syntactic Control in Second Language Learning Miriam Tullgren Loyola University Chicago Abstract 1 This paper explores the effects of age on second language acquisition in adolescents, ages

More information

Specification and Evaluation of Machine Translation Toy Systems - Criteria for laboratory assignments

Specification and Evaluation of Machine Translation Toy Systems - Criteria for laboratory assignments Specification and Evaluation of Machine Translation Toy Systems - Criteria for laboratory assignments Cristina Vertan, Walther v. Hahn University of Hamburg, Natural Language Systems Division Hamburg,

More information

The Acquisition of Person and Number Morphology Within the Verbal Domain in Early Greek

The Acquisition of Person and Number Morphology Within the Verbal Domain in Early Greek Vol. 4 (2012) 15-25 University of Reading ISSN 2040-3461 LANGUAGE STUDIES WORKING PAPERS Editors: C. Ciarlo and D.S. Giannoni The Acquisition of Person and Number Morphology Within the Verbal Domain in

More information

Case government vs Case agreement: modelling Modern Greek case attraction phenomena in LFG

Case government vs Case agreement: modelling Modern Greek case attraction phenomena in LFG Case government vs Case agreement: modelling Modern Greek case attraction phenomena in LFG Dr. Kakia Chatsiou, University of Essex achats at essex.ac.uk Explorations in Syntactic Government and Subcategorisation,

More information

Universal Grammar 2. Universal Grammar 1. Forms and functions 1. Universal Grammar 3. Conceptual and surface structure of complex clauses

Universal Grammar 2. Universal Grammar 1. Forms and functions 1. Universal Grammar 3. Conceptual and surface structure of complex clauses Universal Grammar 1 evidence : 1. crosslinguistic investigation of properties of languages 2. evidence from language acquisition 3. general cognitive abilities 1. Properties can be reflected in a.) structural

More information

- «Crede Experto:,,,». 2 (09) (http://ce.if-mstuca.ru) '36

- «Crede Experto:,,,». 2 (09) (http://ce.if-mstuca.ru) '36 - «Crede Experto:,,,». 2 (09). 2016 (http://ce.if-mstuca.ru) 811.512.122'36 Ш163.24-2 505.. е е ы, Қ х Ц Ь ғ ғ ғ,,, ғ ғ ғ, ғ ғ,,, ғ че ые :,,,, -, ғ ғ ғ, 2016 D. A. Alkebaeva Almaty, Kazakhstan NOUTIONS

More information

The Language of Football England vs. Germany (working title) by Elmar Thalhammer. Abstract

The Language of Football England vs. Germany (working title) by Elmar Thalhammer. Abstract The Language of Football England vs. Germany (working title) by Elmar Thalhammer Abstract As opposed to about fifteen years ago, football has now become a socially acceptable phenomenon in both Germany

More information

Assessing speaking skills:. a workshop for teacher development. Ben Knight

Assessing speaking skills:. a workshop for teacher development. Ben Knight Assessing speaking skills:. a workshop for teacher development Ben Knight Speaking skills are often considered the most important part of an EFL course, and yet the difficulties in testing oral skills

More information

Principal vacancies and appointments

Principal vacancies and appointments Principal vacancies and appointments 2009 10 Sally Robertson New Zealand Council for Educational Research NEW ZEALAND COUNCIL FOR EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH TE RŪNANGA O AOTEAROA MŌ TE RANGAHAU I TE MĀTAURANGA

More information

Describing Motion Events in Adult L2 Spanish Narratives

Describing Motion Events in Adult L2 Spanish Narratives Describing Motion Events in Adult L2 Spanish Narratives Samuel Navarro and Elena Nicoladis University of Alberta 1. Introduction When learning a second language (L2), learners are faced with the challenge

More information

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist Meeting 2 Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Today s agenda Repetition of meeting 1 Mini-lecture on morphology Seminar on chapter 7, worksheet Mini-lecture on syntax Seminar on chapter 9, worksheet

More information

CS 598 Natural Language Processing

CS 598 Natural Language Processing CS 598 Natural Language Processing Natural language is everywhere Natural language is everywhere Natural language is everywhere Natural language is everywhere!"#$%&'&()*+,-./012 34*5665756638/9:;< =>?@ABCDEFGHIJ5KL@

More information

Second Language Acquisition in Adults: From Research to Practice

Second Language Acquisition in Adults: From Research to Practice Second Language Acquisition in Adults: From Research to Practice Donna Moss, National Center for ESL Literacy Education Lauren Ross-Feldman, Georgetown University Second language acquisition (SLA) is the

More information

Prentice Hall Literature: Timeless Voices, Timeless Themes Gold 2000 Correlated to Nebraska Reading/Writing Standards, (Grade 9)

Prentice Hall Literature: Timeless Voices, Timeless Themes Gold 2000 Correlated to Nebraska Reading/Writing Standards, (Grade 9) Nebraska Reading/Writing Standards, (Grade 9) 12.1 Reading The standards for grade 1 presume that basic skills in reading have been taught before grade 4 and that students are independent readers. For

More information

Degree Qualification Profiles Intellectual Skills

Degree Qualification Profiles Intellectual Skills Degree Qualification Profiles Intellectual Skills Intellectual Skills: These are cross-cutting skills that should transcend disciplinary boundaries. Students need all of these Intellectual Skills to acquire

More information

Language Acquisition Fall 2010/Winter Lexical Categories. Afra Alishahi, Heiner Drenhaus

Language Acquisition Fall 2010/Winter Lexical Categories. Afra Alishahi, Heiner Drenhaus Language Acquisition Fall 2010/Winter 2011 Lexical Categories Afra Alishahi, Heiner Drenhaus Computational Linguistics and Phonetics Saarland University Children s Sensitivity to Lexical Categories Look,

More information

U VA THE CHANGING FACE OF UVA STUDENTS: SSESSMENT. About The Study

U VA THE CHANGING FACE OF UVA STUDENTS: SSESSMENT. About The Study About The Study U VA SSESSMENT In 6, the University of Virginia Office of Institutional Assessment and Studies undertook a study to describe how first-year students have changed over the past four decades.

More information

Modal Verbs for the Advice Move in Advice Columns

Modal Verbs for the Advice Move in Advice Columns Modal Verbs for the Advice Move in Advice Columns Ying-shu Liao a and Ting-gen Liao b a Department of English, National Chengchi University, No. 64, Sec. 2, ZhiNan Rd., Wensgan District, Taipei City, 11605,

More information

Intensive English Program Southwest College

Intensive English Program Southwest College Intensive English Program Southwest College ESOL 0352 Advanced Intermediate Grammar for Foreign Speakers CRN 55661-- Summer 2015 Gulfton Center Room 114 11:00 2:45 Mon. Fri. 3 hours lecture / 2 hours lab

More information

Language and Tourism in Sabah, Malaysia and Edinburgh, Scotland

Language and Tourism in Sabah, Malaysia and Edinburgh, Scotland Language and Tourism in Sabah, Malaysia and Edinburgh, Scotland Alan A. Lew a, Lauren Hall-Lew b, Amie Fairs b Northern Arizona University a, University of Edinburgh b alan.lew@nau.edu, lauren.hall-lew@ed.ac.uk,

More information

First Grade Curriculum Highlights: In alignment with the Common Core Standards

First Grade Curriculum Highlights: In alignment with the Common Core Standards First Grade Curriculum Highlights: In alignment with the Common Core Standards ENGLISH LANGUAGE ARTS Foundational Skills Print Concepts Demonstrate understanding of the organization and basic features

More information

Chapter 9 Banked gap-filling

Chapter 9 Banked gap-filling Chapter 9 Banked gap-filling This testing technique is known as banked gap-filling, because you have to choose the appropriate word from a bank of alternatives. In a banked gap-filling task, similarly

More information

FOREWORD.. 5 THE PROPER RUSSIAN PRONUNCIATION. 8. УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) 4 80.

FOREWORD.. 5 THE PROPER RUSSIAN PRONUNCIATION. 8. УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) 4 80. CONTENTS FOREWORD.. 5 THE PROPER RUSSIAN PRONUNCIATION. 8 УРОК (Unit) 1 25 1.1. QUESTIONS WITH КТО AND ЧТО 27 1.2. GENDER OF NOUNS 29 1.3. PERSONAL PRONOUNS 31 УРОК (Unit) 2 38 2.1. PRESENT TENSE OF THE

More information

Politics and Society Curriculum Specification

Politics and Society Curriculum Specification Leaving Certificate Politics and Society Curriculum Specification Ordinary and Higher Level 1 September 2015 2 Contents Senior cycle 5 The experience of senior cycle 6 Politics and Society 9 Introduction

More information

Reading Grammar Section and Lesson Writing Chapter and Lesson Identify a purpose for reading W1-LO; W2- LO; W3- LO; W4- LO; W5-

Reading Grammar Section and Lesson Writing Chapter and Lesson Identify a purpose for reading W1-LO; W2- LO; W3- LO; W4- LO; W5- New York Grade 7 Core Performance Indicators Grades 7 8: common to all four ELA standards Throughout grades 7 and 8, students demonstrate the following core performance indicators in the key ideas of reading,

More information

Inleiding Taalkunde. Docent: Paola Monachesi. Blok 4, 2001/ Syntax 2. 2 Phrases and constituent structure 2. 3 A minigrammar of Italian 3

Inleiding Taalkunde. Docent: Paola Monachesi. Blok 4, 2001/ Syntax 2. 2 Phrases and constituent structure 2. 3 A minigrammar of Italian 3 Inleiding Taalkunde Docent: Paola Monachesi Blok 4, 2001/2002 Contents 1 Syntax 2 2 Phrases and constituent structure 2 3 A minigrammar of Italian 3 4 Trees 3 5 Developing an Italian lexicon 4 6 S(emantic)-selection

More information

The Discourse Anaphoric Properties of Connectives

The Discourse Anaphoric Properties of Connectives The Discourse Anaphoric Properties of Connectives Cassandre Creswell, Kate Forbes, Eleni Miltsakaki, Rashmi Prasad, Aravind Joshi Λ, Bonnie Webber y Λ University of Pennsylvania 3401 Walnut Street Philadelphia,

More information

LEXICAL COHESION ANALYSIS OF THE ARTICLE WHAT IS A GOOD RESEARCH PROJECT? BY BRIAN PALTRIDGE A JOURNAL ARTICLE

LEXICAL COHESION ANALYSIS OF THE ARTICLE WHAT IS A GOOD RESEARCH PROJECT? BY BRIAN PALTRIDGE A JOURNAL ARTICLE LEXICAL COHESION ANALYSIS OF THE ARTICLE WHAT IS A GOOD RESEARCH PROJECT? BY BRIAN PALTRIDGE A JOURNAL ARTICLE Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Sarjana Sastra (S.S.)

More information

ELD CELDT 5 EDGE Level C Curriculum Guide LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT VOCABULARY COMMON WRITING PROJECT. ToolKit

ELD CELDT 5 EDGE Level C Curriculum Guide LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT VOCABULARY COMMON WRITING PROJECT. ToolKit Unit 1 Language Development Express Ideas and Opinions Ask for and Give Information Engage in Discussion ELD CELDT 5 EDGE Level C Curriculum Guide 20132014 Sentences Reflective Essay August 12 th September

More information

BULATS A2 WORDLIST 2

BULATS A2 WORDLIST 2 BULATS A2 WORDLIST 2 INTRODUCTION TO THE BULATS A2 WORDLIST 2 The BULATS A2 WORDLIST 21 is a list of approximately 750 words to help candidates aiming at an A2 pass in the Cambridge BULATS exam. It is

More information

Pseudo-Passives as Adjectival Passives

Pseudo-Passives as Adjectival Passives Pseudo-Passives as Adjectival Passives Kwang-sup Kim Hankuk University of Foreign Studies English Department 81 Oedae-lo Cheoin-Gu Yongin-City 449-791 Republic of Korea kwangsup@hufs.ac.kr Abstract The

More information

Heritage Korean Stage 6 Syllabus Preliminary and HSC Courses

Heritage Korean Stage 6 Syllabus Preliminary and HSC Courses Heritage Korean Stage 6 Syllabus Preliminary and HSC Courses 2010 Board of Studies NSW for and on behalf of the Crown in right of the State of New South Wales This document contains Material prepared by

More information

IN THIS UNIT YOU LEARN HOW TO: SPEAKING 1 Work in pairs. Discuss the questions. 2 Work with a new partner. Discuss the questions.

IN THIS UNIT YOU LEARN HOW TO: SPEAKING 1 Work in pairs. Discuss the questions. 2 Work with a new partner. Discuss the questions. 6 1 IN THIS UNIT YOU LEARN HOW TO: ask and answer common questions about jobs talk about what you re doing at work at the moment talk about arrangements and appointments recognise and use collocations

More information

Research Update. Educational Migration and Non-return in Northern Ireland May 2008

Research Update. Educational Migration and Non-return in Northern Ireland May 2008 Research Update Educational Migration and Non-return in Northern Ireland May 2008 The Equality Commission for Northern Ireland (hereafter the Commission ) in 2007 contracted the Employment Research Institute

More information

National Literacy and Numeracy Framework for years 3/4

National Literacy and Numeracy Framework for years 3/4 1. Oracy National Literacy and Numeracy Framework for years 3/4 Speaking Listening Collaboration and discussion Year 3 - Explain information and ideas using relevant vocabulary - Organise what they say

More information

NAME: East Carolina University PSYC Developmental Psychology Dr. Eppler & Dr. Ironsmith

NAME: East Carolina University PSYC Developmental Psychology Dr. Eppler & Dr. Ironsmith Module 10 1 NAME: East Carolina University PSYC 3206 -- Developmental Psychology Dr. Eppler & Dr. Ironsmith Study Questions for Chapter 10: Language and Education Sigelman & Rider (2009). Life-span human

More information

Ontologies vs. classification systems

Ontologies vs. classification systems Ontologies vs. classification systems Bodil Nistrup Madsen Copenhagen Business School Copenhagen, Denmark bnm.isv@cbs.dk Hanne Erdman Thomsen Copenhagen Business School Copenhagen, Denmark het.isv@cbs.dk

More information

Maximizing Learning Through Course Alignment and Experience with Different Types of Knowledge

Maximizing Learning Through Course Alignment and Experience with Different Types of Knowledge Innov High Educ (2009) 34:93 103 DOI 10.1007/s10755-009-9095-2 Maximizing Learning Through Course Alignment and Experience with Different Types of Knowledge Phyllis Blumberg Published online: 3 February

More information

CORPUS ANALYSIS CORPUS ANALYSIS QUANTITATIVE ANALYSIS

CORPUS ANALYSIS CORPUS ANALYSIS QUANTITATIVE ANALYSIS CORPUS ANALYSIS Antonella Serra CORPUS ANALYSIS ITINEARIES ON LINE: SARDINIA, CAPRI AND CORSICA TOTAL NUMBER OF WORD TOKENS 13.260 TOTAL NUMBER OF WORD TYPES 3188 QUANTITATIVE ANALYSIS THE MOST SIGNIFICATIVE

More information

Linguistics. Undergraduate. Departmental Honors. Graduate. Faculty. Linguistics 1

Linguistics. Undergraduate. Departmental Honors. Graduate. Faculty. Linguistics 1 Linguistics 1 Linguistics Matthew Gordon, Chair Interdepartmental Program in the College of Arts and Science 223 Tate Hall (573) 882-6421 gordonmj@missouri.edu Kibby Smith, Advisor Office of Multidisciplinary

More information

Control and Boundedness

Control and Boundedness Control and Boundedness Having eliminated rules, we would expect constructions to follow from the lexical categories (of heads and specifiers of syntactic constructions) alone. Combinatory syntax simply

More information

The presence of interpretable but ungrammatical sentences corresponds to mismatches between interpretive and productive parsing.

The presence of interpretable but ungrammatical sentences corresponds to mismatches between interpretive and productive parsing. Lecture 4: OT Syntax Sources: Kager 1999, Section 8; Legendre et al. 1998; Grimshaw 1997; Barbosa et al. 1998, Introduction; Bresnan 1998; Fanselow et al. 1999; Gibson & Broihier 1998. OT is not a theory

More information

Ideology and corpora in two languages. Rachelle Freake Queen Mary, University of London

Ideology and corpora in two languages. Rachelle Freake Queen Mary, University of London Ideology and corpora in two languages Rachelle Freake Queen Mary, University of London 1 Outline Cross-linguistic corpus-assisted discourse studies (C-CADS) Ideology: a latent construct Using C-CADS to

More information

Text Type Purpose Structure Language Features Article

Text Type Purpose Structure Language Features Article Page1 Text Types - Purpose, Structure, and Language Features The context, purpose and audience of the text, and whether the text will be spoken or written, will determine the chosen. Levels of, features,

More information

SCHEMA ACTIVATION IN MEMORY FOR PROSE 1. Michael A. R. Townsend State University of New York at Albany

SCHEMA ACTIVATION IN MEMORY FOR PROSE 1. Michael A. R. Townsend State University of New York at Albany Journal of Reading Behavior 1980, Vol. II, No. 1 SCHEMA ACTIVATION IN MEMORY FOR PROSE 1 Michael A. R. Townsend State University of New York at Albany Abstract. Forty-eight college students listened to

More information

Using dialogue context to improve parsing performance in dialogue systems

Using dialogue context to improve parsing performance in dialogue systems Using dialogue context to improve parsing performance in dialogue systems Ivan Meza-Ruiz and Oliver Lemon School of Informatics, Edinburgh University 2 Buccleuch Place, Edinburgh I.V.Meza-Ruiz@sms.ed.ac.uk,

More information

Proof Theory for Syntacticians

Proof Theory for Syntacticians Department of Linguistics Ohio State University Syntax 2 (Linguistics 602.02) January 5, 2012 Logics for Linguistics Many different kinds of logic are directly applicable to formalizing theories in syntax

More information

Written by: YULI AMRIA (RRA1B210085) ABSTRACT. Key words: ability, possessive pronouns, and possessive adjectives INTRODUCTION

Written by: YULI AMRIA (RRA1B210085) ABSTRACT. Key words: ability, possessive pronouns, and possessive adjectives INTRODUCTION STUDYING GRAMMAR OF ENGLISH AS A FOREIGN LANGUAGE: STUDENTS ABILITY IN USING POSSESSIVE PRONOUNS AND POSSESSIVE ADJECTIVES IN ONE JUNIOR HIGH SCHOOL IN JAMBI CITY Written by: YULI AMRIA (RRA1B210085) ABSTRACT

More information

The Inclusiveness Condition in Survive-minimalism

The Inclusiveness Condition in Survive-minimalism The Inclusiveness Condition in Survive-minimalism Minoru Fukuda Miyazaki Municipal University fukuda@miyazaki-mu.ac.jp March 2013 1. Introduction Given a phonetic form (PF) representation! and a logical

More information

Grade 11 Language Arts (2 Semester Course) CURRICULUM. Course Description ENGLISH 11 (2 Semester Course) Duration: 2 Semesters Prerequisite: None

Grade 11 Language Arts (2 Semester Course) CURRICULUM. Course Description ENGLISH 11 (2 Semester Course) Duration: 2 Semesters Prerequisite: None Grade 11 Language Arts (2 Semester Course) CURRICULUM Course Description ENGLISH 11 (2 Semester Course) Duration: 2 Semesters Prerequisite: None Through the integrated study of literature, composition,

More information

Understanding the Relationship between Comprehension and Production

Understanding the Relationship between Comprehension and Production Carnegie Mellon University Research Showcase @ CMU Department of Psychology Dietrich College of Humanities and Social Sciences 1-1987 Understanding the Relationship between Comprehension and Production

More information

LANGUAGE IN INDIA Strength for Today and Bright Hope for Tomorrow Volume 11 : 3 March 2011 ISSN

LANGUAGE IN INDIA Strength for Today and Bright Hope for Tomorrow Volume 11 : 3 March 2011 ISSN LANGUAGE IN INDIA Strength for Today and Bright Hope for Tomorrow Volume ISSN 1930-2940 Managing Editor: M. S. Thirumalai, Ph.D. Editors: B. Mallikarjun, Ph.D. Sam Mohanlal, Ph.D. B. A. Sharada, Ph.D.

More information

California Department of Education English Language Development Standards for Grade 8

California Department of Education English Language Development Standards for Grade 8 Section 1: Goal, Critical Principles, and Overview Goal: English learners read, analyze, interpret, and create a variety of literary and informational text types. They develop an understanding of how language

More information

Prentice Hall Literature: Timeless Voices, Timeless Themes, Platinum 2000 Correlated to Nebraska Reading/Writing Standards (Grade 10)

Prentice Hall Literature: Timeless Voices, Timeless Themes, Platinum 2000 Correlated to Nebraska Reading/Writing Standards (Grade 10) Prentice Hall Literature: Timeless Voices, Timeless Themes, Platinum 2000 Nebraska Reading/Writing Standards (Grade 10) 12.1 Reading The standards for grade 1 presume that basic skills in reading have

More information

Common Core State Standards for English Language Arts

Common Core State Standards for English Language Arts Reading Standards for Literature 6-12 Grade 9-10 Students: 1. Cite strong and thorough textual evidence to support analysis of what the text says explicitly as well as inferences drawn from the text. 2.

More information

The subject of adjectives: Syntactic position and semantic interpretation

The subject of adjectives: Syntactic position and semantic interpretation The subject of adjectives: Syntactic position and semantic interpretation Aya Meltzer-ASSCHER Abstract It is widely accepted that subjects of verbs are base-generated within the (extended) verbal projection.

More information

To appear in The TESOL encyclopedia of ELT (Wiley-Blackwell) 1 RECASTING. Kazuya Saito. Birkbeck, University of London

To appear in The TESOL encyclopedia of ELT (Wiley-Blackwell) 1 RECASTING. Kazuya Saito. Birkbeck, University of London To appear in The TESOL encyclopedia of ELT (Wiley-Blackwell) 1 RECASTING Kazuya Saito Birkbeck, University of London Abstract Among the many corrective feedback techniques at ESL/EFL teachers' disposal,

More information

Som and Optimality Theory

Som and Optimality Theory Som and Optimality Theory This article argues that the difference between English and Norwegian with respect to the presence of a complementizer in embedded subject questions is attributable to a larger

More information

Developing Grammar in Context

Developing Grammar in Context Developing Grammar in Context intermediate with answers Mark Nettle and Diana Hopkins PUBLISHED BY THE PRESS SYNDICATE OF THE UNIVERSITY OF CAMBRIDGE The Pitt Building, Trumpington Street, Cambridge, United

More information