Running Head: EFFECT OF UTILITY MESSAGES ON PERCEIVED TASK VALUE. The Effect of Utility Messages During Science Instruction on

Similar documents
An Empirical Analysis of the Effects of Mexican American Studies Participation on Student Achievement within Tucson Unified School District

Hierarchical Linear Models I: Introduction ICPSR 2015

Understanding Games for Teaching Reflections on Empirical Approaches in Team Sports Research

Greek Teachers Attitudes toward the Inclusion of Students with Special Educational Needs

Gender and socioeconomic differences in science achievement in Australia: From SISS to TIMSS

American Journal of Business Education October 2009 Volume 2, Number 7

Comparing Teachers Adaptations of an Inquiry-Oriented Curriculum Unit with Student Learning. Jay Fogleman and Katherine L. McNeill

MOTIVATION FOR READING AND UPPER PRIMARY SCHOOL STUDENTS ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT IN READING IN KENYA

Sheila M. Smith is Assistant Professor, Department of Business Information Technology, College of Business, Ball State University, Muncie, Indiana.

Effective practices of peer mentors in an undergraduate writing intensive course

Effective Recruitment and Retention Strategies for Underrepresented Minority Students: Perspectives from Dental Students

Iowa School District Profiles. Le Mars

Positive turning points for girls in mathematics classrooms: Do they stand the test of time?

STEM Academy Workshops Evaluation

Process Evaluations for a Multisite Nutrition Education Program

BENCHMARK TREND COMPARISON REPORT:

Shelters Elementary School

IS FINANCIAL LITERACY IMPROVED BY PARTICIPATING IN A STOCK MARKET GAME?

5 Programmatic. The second component area of the equity audit is programmatic. Equity

Study Abroad Housing and Cultural Intelligence: Does Housing Influence the Gaining of Cultural Intelligence?

Paper presented at the ERA-AARE Joint Conference, Singapore, November, 1996.

PSIWORLD Keywords: self-directed learning; personality traits; academic achievement; learning strategies; learning activties.

Institution of Higher Education Demographic Survey

File Print Created 11/17/2017 6:16 PM 1 of 10

Data Glossary. Summa Cum Laude: the top 2% of each college's distribution of cumulative GPAs for the graduating cohort. Academic Honors (Latin Honors)

NATIONAL SURVEY OF STUDENT ENGAGEMENT (NSSE)

Cooper Upper Elementary School

On-the-Fly Customization of Automated Essay Scoring

The Relationship of Grade Span in 9 th Grade to Math Achievement in High School

Post-intervention multi-informant survey on knowledge, attitudes and practices (KAP) on disability and inclusive education

National Survey of Student Engagement Spring University of Kansas. Executive Summary

National Survey of Student Engagement (NSSE) Temple University 2016 Results

Shyness and Technology Use in High School Students. Lynne Henderson, Ph. D., Visiting Scholar, Stanford

Enhancing Students Understanding Statistics with TinkerPlots: Problem-Based Learning Approach

The Effect of Income on Educational Attainment: Evidence from State Earned Income Tax Credit Expansions

TCH_LRN 531 Frameworks for Research in Mathematics and Science Education (3 Credits)

Psychometric Research Brief Office of Shared Accountability

Status of Women of Color in Science, Engineering, and Medicine

The Condition of College & Career Readiness 2016

Student Support Services Evaluation Readiness Report. By Mandalyn R. Swanson, Ph.D., Program Evaluation Specialist. and Evaluation

Evaluation of a College Freshman Diversity Research Program

Effective Pre-school and Primary Education 3-11 Project (EPPE 3-11)

teacher, peer, or school) on each page, and a package of stickers on which

Educational Attainment

A Diverse Student Body

Strategy for teaching communication skills in dentistry

Practices Worthy of Attention Step Up to High School Chicago Public Schools Chicago, Illinois

Strategies for Solving Fraction Tasks and Their Link to Algebraic Thinking

The My Class Activities Instrument as Used in Saturday Enrichment Program Evaluation

NATIONAL SURVEY OF STUDENT ENGAGEMENT

2012 ACT RESULTS BACKGROUND

Sex Differences in Self-Efficacy and Attributions: Influence of Performance Feedback

Legacy of NAACP Salary equalization suits.

MEASURING GENDER EQUALITY IN EDUCATION: LESSONS FROM 43 COUNTRIES

Race, Class, and the Selective College Experience

Multiple regression as a practical tool for teacher preparation program evaluation

National Survey of Student Engagement The College Student Report

self-regulated learning Boekaerts, 1997, 1999; Pintrich, 1999a, 2000; Wolters, 1998; Zimmerman, 2000

PREDISPOSING FACTORS TOWARDS EXAMINATION MALPRACTICE AMONG STUDENTS IN LAGOS UNIVERSITIES: IMPLICATIONS FOR COUNSELLING

EFFECTS OF MATHEMATICS ACCELERATION ON ACHIEVEMENT, PERCEPTION, AND BEHAVIOR IN LOW- PERFORMING SECONDARY STUDENTS

Serving Country and Community: A Study of Service in AmeriCorps. A Profile of AmeriCorps Members at Baseline. June 2001

VIEW: An Assessment of Problem Solving Style

Higher education is becoming a major driver of economic competitiveness

It s not me, it s you : An Analysis of Factors that Influence the Departure of First-Year Students of Color

STUDENT PERCEPTION SURVEYS ACTIONABLE STUDENT FEEDBACK PROMOTING EXCELLENCE IN TEACHING AND LEARNING

Principal vacancies and appointments

ILLINOIS DISTRICT REPORT CARD

success. It will place emphasis on:

What effect does science club have on pupil attitudes, engagement and attainment? Dr S.J. Nolan, The Perse School, June 2014

ILLINOIS DISTRICT REPORT CARD

A Game-based Assessment of Children s Choices to Seek Feedback and to Revise

Updated: December Educational Attainment

Effectiveness of McGraw-Hill s Treasures Reading Program in Grades 3 5. October 21, Research Conducted by Empirical Education Inc.

2005 National Survey of Student Engagement: Freshman and Senior Students at. St. Cloud State University. Preliminary Report.

Inside the mind of a learner

Cooper Upper Elementary School

U VA THE CHANGING FACE OF UVA STUDENTS: SSESSMENT. About The Study

Peer Influence on Academic Achievement: Mean, Variance, and Network Effects under School Choice

Redirected Inbound Call Sampling An Example of Fit for Purpose Non-probability Sample Design

The Diversity of STEM Majors and a Strategy for Improved STEM Retention

University of Utah. 1. Graduation-Rates Data a. All Students. b. Student-Athletes

Applying Motivation Theories to the Design of Educational Technology

Math Pathways Task Force Recommendations February Background

READY OR NOT? CALIFORNIA'S EARLY ASSESSMENT PROGRAM AND THE TRANSITION TO COLLEGE

NATIONAL SURVEY OF STUDENT ENGAGEMENT

Running head: DEVELOPING MULTIPLICATION AUTOMATICTY 1. Examining the Impact of Frustration Levels on Multiplication Automaticity.

THE PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIVERSITY SCHREYER HONORS COLLEGE DEPARTMENT OF MATHEMATICS ASSESSING THE EFFECTIVENESS OF MULTIPLE CHOICE MATH TESTS

The KAM project: Mathematics in vocational subjects*

Global Seminar Quito, Ecuador Language, Culture & Child Development. EDS 115 GS Cognitive Development & Education Summer Session I, 2016

Title: Comparison Between Teachers Efficacy Beliefs and Students Academic Performance from Highly Vulnerable Areas (ICSEI 2011 no.

INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY

ScienceDirect. Noorminshah A Iahad a *, Marva Mirabolghasemi a, Noorfa Haszlinna Mustaffa a, Muhammad Shafie Abd. Latif a, Yahya Buntat b

Monitoring Metacognitive abilities in children: A comparison of children between the ages of 5 to 7 years and 8 to 11 years

Segmentation Study of Tulsa Area Higher Education Needs Ages 36+ March Prepared for: Conducted by:

Accessing Higher Education in Developing Countries: panel data analysis from India, Peru and Vietnam

The number of involuntary part-time workers,

Observing Teachers: The Mathematics Pedagogy of Quebec Francophone and Anglophone Teachers

PROMOTING QUALITY AND EQUITY IN EDUCATION: THE IMPACT OF SCHOOL LEARNING ENVIRONMENT

- COURSE DESCRIPTIONS - (*From Online Graduate Catalog )

Executive Summary. Belle Terre Elementary School

Transcription:

Running Head: EFFECT OF UTILITY MESSAGES ON PERCEIVED TASK VALUE The Effect of Utility Messages During Science Instruction on Students Perceptions of Task Value Stephen Kafkas, Hayal Kackar-Cam, Jennifer A. Schmidt, & Lee Shumow Northern Illinois University This material is based upon work supported by the National Science Foundation under Grant No: HRD-1136143. Any opinions, findings, conclusions, or recommendations expressed in this material are those of the author(s) and do not reflect the views of the National Science Foundation. DRAFT: PLEASE DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE WITHOUT AUTHORS PERMISSION

EFFECT OF UTILITY MESSAGES ON PERCEIVED TASK VALUE 2 Abstract Using data collected through surveys, classroom observations, and a variant of Experience Sampling Method, we investigated whether the use of utility value statements during science instruction is related to seventh grade students perceptions about the usefulness and importance of their daily science activities. We also examined whether gender, race, interest, grades and perceived competence moderate these relationships. Hierarchical Linear Modeling revealed that the number of utility statements made during instruction was positively associated with students ratings of usefulness and importance of the content. Students with high perceived competence expressed greater valuing of science content. Gender and race played a complex role in the relationship between teacher statements and students beliefs. Implications for motivation research and science teacher practice are discussed.

EFFECT OF UTILITY MESSAGES ON PERCEIVED TASK VALUE 3 The Effect of Utility Messages During Science Instruction on Students Perceptions of Task Value The purpose of this study is two-fold. First, we investigate whether emphasizing the utility of science content is related to seventh grade students perceptions of the usefulness and importance of science tasks, while also taking into account students interest in these tasks. Second, we examine whether these relationships are moderated by factors like gender, race and perceived competence, while controlling for students science grades. Science class can be challenging for students; learning science often requires that students be actively engaged in learning. The expectancy-value model (Eccles et al., 1983) identifies student beliefs about the subjective value of tasks as an important factor in their engagement and motivation. Subjective task values are defined as how a task might meet different needs of students. Students may find tasks valuable for a wide range of reasons. For example, research has shown that high school students see science as more valuable than middle school students which is largely due to the belief that doing well in science class is important for college admissions (Baker & Leary, 1995; Greenfield, 1996). Research has also shown that student perceptions of high value in their schoolwork supports achievement-related behaviors in their lives outside of school. For example, students who do not find their schoolwork useful, do not tend to associate their academic behaviors with their personal lives, whereas students who find their schoolwork useful tend to find their academic behaviors beneficial for their lives (Simons, DeWitte, & Lens, 2004). The extent to which students perceive value in their academic coursework has been shown to impact students decisions to pursue those domains in both the short-term and long-term (i.e., Durik, Vida, & Eccles, 2006).

EFFECT OF UTILITY MESSAGES ON PERCEIVED TASK VALUE 4 In this study we focus on three components of students task value beliefs in science class (Eccles & Wigfield, 1995): usefulness of the task outside of school, the personal importance of the task, and students interest in the task. While these components have been shown to be positively and moderately correlated, research has provided empirical support to the theoretical distinctions between these three components of task value (e.g., Eccles & Wigfield, 1995). At the same time, research has also shown that competence beliefs are important in successful performance in various domains (Bandura, 1997; Csikszentmihalyi, 1990; Eccles et al., 1998; Meece et al., 1990; Wigfield & Eccles, 1992). Students who feel competent in a certain domain, however may not choose to engage in that domain if they find no value in it (Wigfield & Eccles, 2002). Competence and task value beliefs, then, complement each other in determining students task choice and engagement. Given the importance of considering competence beliefs in relation to students task value beliefs, in this study we investigated whether students who feel they are good at science will be more likely to see it as useful and important to them. Additionally, we examine whether those with high competence beliefs are more likely to internalize statements made during instruction about the utility of science. There is evidence that adolescent boys and girls differ in their perceptions of the value of science. For example, middle school girls were found to report that science has less value than do boys (Catsambis, 1995; Lee & Burkam, 1996), and utility beliefs as well as ability beliefs played an important role in 7th and 9th grade girls course-taking (James, 2002). It has been well documented that Hispanic students have lower achievement in STEM courses, and Hispanic adults are severely underrepresented in STEM fields compared to non-hispanic Whites (Taningco et al., 2008). There could be ethnic differences in students perceptions of the value for science that may help explain observed ethnicity gaps in STEM areas. Consequently Hispanic

EFFECT OF UTILITY MESSAGES ON PERCEIVED TASK VALUE 5 students may draw particular benefit from instructional approaches that involve the use of utility statements in classroom. In light of race and gender differences in perceived task value in the science classroom, in the present study we investigate whether the relationship between the use of utility statements in class and students perception of the usefulness of and importance of science is moderated by race and gender. The research questions that guide this study are: 1-What is the association of utility statements and students' interest during science instruction with students perceptions of the usefulness and importance of science tasks? 2-How do the associations tested in question 1 vary by gender, race, and perceived competence, while controlling for grades? Method Data used in this paper were collected as part of a larger, federally-funded study designed to examine classroom factors that impact students interest, engagement and achievement in science. Data were collected in 2011-2012 from two middle schools (n=14 classrooms) serving students from a diverse community located on the fringe of a large metropolitan area. Participants Student Sample. Three hundred and seventy-nine 7 th grade students participated in the study (mean age=12.24). The sample was 45% male and 55% female. The sample was 57% Hispanic (regardless of race), 22% non-hispanic White, 8% multi-racial (non-hispanic), 11% African American, 3% Asian, and less than 1% Native American. According to school records, 61% of the participants were eligible to receive free or reduced lunch. Forty-four percent of the students in the sample reported that neither of their parents had attained a college degree. Twelve percent said that at least 1 parent had graduated from college, and 11% indicated that at least one parent had earned an advanced degree.

EFFECT OF UTILITY MESSAGES ON PERCEIVED TASK VALUE 6 Teacher Sample. The 7 th grade teacher sample was comprised of four White females; two in their twenties and two in their fifties. They had between four and 20 years of teaching experience, with the vast majority of each teacher s experience being in their present school. The two older teachers had each taught briefly in another school. The two older teachers were tenured; the two younger were not. Both of the older teachers had earned master s degrees. For each teacher, we observed in either 3 or 4 of their 7 th grade science classrooms. Instruments and Measures Data were collected using traditional surveys, classroom observations, and a variant of the Experience Sampling Method (ESM, Csikszentmihalyi & Larson, 1987). Each classroom was observed 11 times during the school year by at least 2 trained observers. Following each observed lesson, students completed an Experience Sampling Form (ESF) in which they were prompted to think about their work in class today and report on several dimensions of their experience using 4 point Likert scales. The ESFs took approximately 3 minutes to complete. Turner and colleagues have used procedures very similar to those used in the current study, combining ESM and observational data to demonstrate relationships among specific instructional practices and students subjective experience (Turner, et al., 1998; Schweinle, Turner & Meyer, 2006). The method has a high degree of external or ecological validity, capturing participants responses in everyday life. There are indications that the internal validity of the ESM is stronger than one-time questionnaires as well. Zuzanek (1999) has shown that the immediacy of the questions reduces the potential for failure of recall and the tendency to choose responses on the basis of social desirability (see Csikszentmihalyi & Larson, 1987, and Hektner, Schmidt, & Csikszentmihalyi, 2007 for extensive evidence on ESM s validity and reliability) Outcome Measures. Perceived usefulness was measured on the ESM form. Students

EFFECT OF UTILITY MESSAGES ON PERCEIVED TASK VALUE 7 were asked to rate whether they think what you learned today could be useful outside of school? (-1=no, 22%; 0=not sure, 33%, and 1= yes, 45%, M=0.23, SD= 0.788). Perceived importance was also measured on the ESM form. Students were asked to rate the importance of today's work to you? (0=not at all, 3=very much; M=1.85, SD=1.063). Time-varying Predictors. The number of utility statements made during science instruction was gathered from classroom observations. Two to three trained observers conducted classroom observations and recorded instances during the class which pertained to any type of value statement that was made about science (utility, attainment, intrinsic, cost values). The initiator (i.e., teacher, student) of the utility statement was also noted. In the current paper, we focused on any utility value statement made by either teachers or students to capture the full range of utility statements that the students were exposed to. Statements were coded as referencing utility value if the speaker pointed out how what the class was doing or learning related to everyday life (e.g., it s important to understand acceleration when you drive a car ), to students future educational plans (e.g., this will help you understand chemistry when you get to high school ), to students occupational plans (e.g., you said you wanted to be an engineer: engineers use this all the time ), or to careers in general (e.g., Dieticians use their knowledge of food chemistry to help people learn how to eat right. ). Utility value statements were coded on multiple dimensions, but for the purpose of this study we focused simply on the number of times these statements were made in each class period. This way, the total number of utility value statements made in class can be matched to students ratings of the perceived usefulness of the content they covered in class that day. A total of 287 utility statements were recorded during the observations, with 77% of those being made by the teacher and 23% by students. Situational Interest was another time-varying predictor of the two outcomes, and was

EFFECT OF UTILITY MESSAGES ON PERCEIVED TASK VALUE 8 measured on the ESM form. Students were asked to rate the extent to which they thought today s class work was interesting. (0=not at all, 3=very much; M=1.70, SD=1.076). Time-invariant Predictors. Perceived competence was measured in a survey administered to students prior to the classroom observation period. In the survey students rated these three items (from Wigfield & Eccles, 2000) about their science ability: How good in science are you? (1=not at all good, 7= very good), If you were to list all the students in this class from the worst to the best in science, where would you put yourself? 1=one of the worst, 4=in the middle, 7=one of the best), and Compared to most of your other school subjects, how good are you in science? (1= a lot worse in science, 4=about the same, 7= a lot better in science).these ratings were combined to create a composite variable of perceived competence (M=4.26, SD=1.22, α=.80). Gender, Ethnicity and Grades. Students reported their gender and ethnicity in surveys. Students first quarter science grades were gathered from school records and were used as a control in analysis (0=F, 1=D, 2=C, 3=B, 4=A, M=2.67, SD=1.17). Analyses Because the data are nested (ESM observations nested within students) Hierarchical Linear Modeling (HLM, Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002) was used to analyze the data. The aggregated measures of students perceived usefulness and importance were used as outcome variables. The number of utility statements made during science instruction and students perceived interest were included as time varying level-1 variables. Gender (female=1, male=0), ethnicity (Hispanic, regardless of race=1, non-hispanic=0) and students ratings of perceived competence were included at level-2. Students science grades were entered as a control variable. Analyses explored interactions between ethnicity and gender as well. As an illustration of the

EFFECT OF UTILITY MESSAGES ON PERCEIVED TASK VALUE 9 models used in our analyses, we present the level 1 and level 2 equations built for the Usefulness outcome. Level-1 Model Usefulness ti = π 0i + π 1i (Number of Utility Statements ti ) + π 1i (Interest ti ) + e ti Level-2 Model π 0i = β 00 + β 01 (Female i ) + β 02(Competence i ) + β 03(Hispanic i ) + β 04(Hispanic*Female i ) + β 05(Grades i ) + r 0i π 1i = β 10 + β 11(Female i ) + β 12(Competence i ) + β 13 (Hispanic i ) + β 14(Hispanic*Female i ) + β 15(Grades i ) + r 1i π 2i = β 20 + β 21(Female i ) + β 22(Competence i ) + β 23(Hispanic i ) + β 24(Hispanic*Female i ) + β 25(Grades i ) + r 2i Results The Effect of Utility Statements and Interest on Task Usefulness As seen in Table 1, regardless of gender, ethnicity and science achievement, average student usefulness ratings indicate that in general students were not sure whether the day s content was useful (β 00 = 0.17 p<.05). There was, however, variation in perceived usefulness that can be attributed to perceived competence. Students with higher perceived competence tended to rate the science topic as more useful (β 02 = 0.09, p <.05). The number of utility statements made during instruction was positively associated with non-hispanic males ratings of usefulness of the content for that day (β 10 = 0. 062002, p<.001), such that for every utility statement made, non-hispanic males ratings of usefulness increased by.06. Non-Hispanic females did not differ significantly from non-hispanic males in this relationship, suggesting that the relationship between utility statements and usefulness ratings

EFFECT OF UTILITY MESSAGES ON PERCEIVED TASK VALUE 10 were similar across these two groups (β 11 = -0.02, p=ns). While the positive effect of utility statements on usefulness ratings was weaker for Hispanic males (β 13 = -0.07), it was stronger for Hispanic females (β 14 = 0.06, marginal significance), such that for every utility statement made, Hispanic females ratings of usefulness increased by.12 (.06 +.06). Students competence beliefs and science grades did not play a significant role on the positive effect of utility statements on perceived usefulness. Higher ratings of interest during science instruction was associated with higher ratings of usefulness of the content for that day (β 20 = 0.18, p<.001) for all students regardless of gender, ethnicity and competence beliefs. This positive relationship was stronger for students with higher levels of science achievement (β 25 = 0.03 p<.05). The Effect of Utility Statements and Interest on Task Importance As seen in Table 2, regardless of gender and ethnicity, average student usefulness ratings indicate that in general students thought that the day s content was somewhat important (β 00 = 1.79, p<.001). As with the task usefulness outcome, there was variation in task importance that can be attributed to perceived competence. Students with higher perceived competence tended to rate the science topic as more important (β 02 = 0.06, p=.05). Higher science achievement was related to lower ratings of importance (β 05 = -0.07, p<.05). The number of utility statements made during instruction was positively associated with non-hispanic males ratings of task importance (β 10 = 0.04, p<.05), such that for every utility statement made, non-hispanic males ratings of the importance of the content for that day increased by.036. This relationship was weaker for non-hispanic females (β 11 = -0.06, p<.05), and Hispanic males (β 13 = -0.07, p<.01). This suggests that in comparison to non-hispanic male

EFFECT OF UTILITY MESSAGES ON PERCEIVED TASK VALUE 11 students, non-hispanic female and Hispanic male students would need more frequent exposure to utility statements in class in order to find their class content more important. The positive effect of utility statements on importance ratings was remarkably stronger for Hispanic females (β 14 =0.12, p<.001) than any other group of students, such that for every utility statement made, Hispanic females ratings of the importance of the content for that day increased by.15 (0.03 + 0.12). Higher ratings of interest during science instruction were associated with higher ratings of importance of the content for that day (β 20 = 0.39, p<.001) for all students regardless of gender, ethnicity, competence beliefs or science achievement. Discussion Results of this study contribute to motivation research and science teacher practice in several ways. Results suggest that when teachers emphasize the utility of science content during instruction, this does become internalized by students and is reflected in their own perceptions of the usefulness and importance of that content. As finding value in an activity is critical to persistence in that activity, this teacher behavior could have important implications for students achievement and persistence in science. Our results also emphasize the crucial role of students ability beliefs in finding science content useful in that students with high ability beliefs expressed higher usefulness and importance of the science content. On the other hand, competence was not a significant moderator of the positive relationship between the use of utility statements during science instruction and students ratings of task usefulness and importance. This suggests that even students with low competence beliefs benefit from the positive effects of emphasizing the utility of science content during instruction.

EFFECT OF UTILITY MESSAGES ON PERCEIVED TASK VALUE 12 Results suggest that gender and ethnicity play a complex role in moderating the relationship between utility statements and students task value beliefs. Emphasizing the utility of science content during instruction had the strongest positive impact on Hispanic females beliefs about task usefulness and importance. The second strongest impact of utility statements on positive task value beliefs was for non-hispanic males. This suggested that in order to positively influence the task value beliefs of the other two groups of students, non-hispanic females and Hispanic males, teachers would need to highlight the utility of science content more frequently. Results also provided evidence that emphasizing utility of science during instruction had positive effects on task value beliefs, regardless of students interest in the class content. The same independent effect was observed for interest, such that students interest in the science content was positively reflected in their ratings of usefulness and importance, regardless of whether or not utility messages were made in class. This suggests that emphasizing the utility of science and supporting students interest in tasks can both promote positive task values independently of one another. Therefore, science instruction may benefit from both of these teacher behaviors. Recent multi-method research in science classrooms suggests that teachers miss many opportunities to emphasize the value of science to students in the course of everyday science instruction. Teachers may erroneously assume that their students are independently making more frequent connections between science content and their lives, or teachers may lack the deep content knowledge that is necessary to make spontaneous connections during instruction. Moreover, this same research suggests that students report relatively low levels of interest during science instruction (Shumow & Schmidt, 2014). Thus, these are two areas that are very promising for improving the value students place on science.

EFFECT OF UTILITY MESSAGES ON PERCEIVED TASK VALUE 13 A finding that merits further investigation was that higher science grades were related to lower ratings of task importance. There was a moderate correlation between competence and science grades (r=.42) indicating that there was no multicollinearity between the two measures. A possible explanation is that students may have interpreted task importance as having high stakes for future education (e.g., I need to do well in this course), and thus students who are already doing well in class may have felt less pressure to do well and therefore may have reported lower importance ratings.

EFFECT OF UTILITY MESSAGES ON PERCEIVED TASK VALUE 14 References Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of self-control. Gordonsville, VA: WH Freeman & Co. Catsambis, S. (1995). Gender, race, ethnicity, and science education in the middle grades. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 32(3), 243-257. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal performance. New York: Harper and Row. Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Larson, R. (1987). Validity and reliability of the experience sampling method. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 175, 526-536. Durik, A. M., Vida, M., & Eccles, J. S. (2006). Task values and ability beliefs as predictors of high school literacy choices: A developmental analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(2), 382. Eccles, J. S., Adler, T. F., Futterman, R., Goff, S. B., Kaczala, C. M., Meece, J., et al. (1983). Expectancies, values and academic behaviors. In J. T. Spence (Ed.), Achievement and Achievement Motives (pp. 75-146). San Francisco, CA: Freeman. Eccles, J.S., Wigfield, A., & Schiefele, U. (1998). Motivation to succeed. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Volume 3 -- Social, emotional, and personality development (5th ed.). New York: Wiley. Hektner, J.M., Schmidt, J.A., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2007). Experience sampling method: Measuring the quality of everyday life. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Hulleman, C. S., Godes, O., Hendricks, B. L., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2010). Enhancing interest and performance with a utility value intervention. Journal of Educational Psychology,

EFFECT OF UTILITY MESSAGES ON PERCEIVED TASK VALUE 15 102(4), 880. James, H. M. (2002). Why do Girls Persist in Science? A Qualitative Study of the Decision- Making Processes of Pre-Adolescent and Adolescent Girls (Doctoral dissertation, Harvard University). Lee, V. E., & Burkam, D. T. (1996). Gender differences in middle grade science achievement: Subject domain, ability level, and course emphasis. Science Education, 80(6), 613-650. Meece, J. L., Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (1990). Predictors of math anxiety and its influence on young adolescents' course enrollment intentions and performance in mathematics. Journal of educational psychology, 82(1), 60. Raudenbush, S. W., Bryk, A. S., & Congdon, R. T. (2002). Hierarchical linear modeling. Thousands Oaks: Sage. Schweinle, A., Turner, J. C., & Meyer, D. K. (2006). Striking the right balance: Students' motivation and affect in upper elementary mathematics classes. Journal of Educational Research, 995, 271-293. Simmons, J., Dewitte, S., & Lens, W. (2004). The Role of Different Types of Instrumentality in Motivation, Study Strategies, and Performance: Know Why You Learn, So You ll Know What You Learn! British Journal of Educational Psychology. 74, 343-360. Taningco, M. T., Mathew, A. B., & Pachon, H. P. (2008). Computer use, parental expectations & Latino academic achievement. The Tomas Rivera Policy Institute. Retrieved from http://www.ibm.com/ibm/ibmgives/downloads/quant_study_04-16- 08.pdf Turner, J. C., Meyer, D. K., Cox, K. E., Logan, C., DiCintio, M., & Thomas, C. T. (1998). Creating contexts for involvement in mathematics. Journal of Educational Psychology, 904, 730-745.

EFFECT OF UTILITY MESSAGES ON PERCEIVED TASK VALUE 16 Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (1992). The development of achievement task values: A theoretical analysis. Developmental review, 12(3), 265-310. Wigfield, A. & Eccles, J. (2000). Expectancy-value theory of achievement motivation. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 68-81. Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (2002). The development of competence beliefs, expectancies for success, and achievement values from childhood through adolescence. Development of achievement motivation, 91-120. Zuzanek, J. (1999). Experience sampling method: Current and potential research applications. Paper presented at the workshop on time-use measurement and research, National Research Council, Washington, DC.

EFFECT OF UTILITY MESSAGES ON PERCEIVED TASK VALUE 17 Tables Table 1. Two-level Hierarchical Linear Model Analysis Predicting Usefulness Fixed Effect Coefficient Standard error t-ratio d.f. p-value For Intercept1, π 0 Intercept2, β 00 0.168545 0.074347 2.267 270 0.024 Female, β 01 0.071508 0.090125 0.793 270 0.428 Competence, β 02 0.090370 0.029824 3.030 270 0.003 Hispanic, β 03 0.001456 0.099453 0.015 270 0.988 Hispanic*Female, β 04-0.008963 0.121862-0.074 270 0.941 Science Grades, β 05-0.041353 0.030357-1.362 270 0.174 For Utility Messages slope, π 1 Intercept2, β 10 0.062002 0.017075 3.631 270 <0.001 Female, β 11-0.022878 0.020245-1.130 270 0.259 Competence, β 12 0.002183 0.006893 0.317 270 0.752 Hispanic, β 13-0.065778 0.023712-2.774 270 0.006 Hispanic*Female, β 14 0.058617 0.030881 1.898 270 0.059 Science Grades, β 15 0.007357 0.007094 1.037 270 0.301 For Situational Interest slope, π 2 Intercept2, β 20 0.178844 0.031409 5.694 270 <0.001 Female, β 21-0.016014 0.043767-0.366 270 0.715 Competence, β 22-0.002057 0.013538-0.152 270 0.879 Hispanic, β 23 0.030706 0.042637 0.720 270 0.472 Hispanic*Female, β 24-0.024607 0.058658-0.419 270 0.675 Science Grades, β 25 0.031178 0.012919 2.413 270 0.016 Notes: N= 276 Level-1 units (students), 2402 Level-2 units (responses)

EFFECT OF UTILITY MESSAGES ON PERCEIVED TASK VALUE 18 Table 2. Two-level Hierarchical Linear Model Analysis Predicting Task Importance Fixed Effect Coefficient Standard error t-ratio Approx. d.f. p-value For Intercept1, π 0 Intercept2, β 00 1.791267 0.079166 22.627 270 <0.001 Female, β 01 0.055967 0.107998 0.518 270 0.605 Competence, β 02 0.064537 0.032803 1.967 270 0.050 Hispanic, β 03 0.047096 0.099071 0.475 270 0.635 Hispanic*Female, β 04 0.020529 0.137024 0.150 270 0.881 Science Grades, β 05-0.074835 0.033967-2.203 270 0.028 For Utility Messages slope, π 1 Intercept2, β 10 0.036435 0.018441 1.976 270 0.049 Female, β 11-0.063350 0.025467-2.488 270 0.013 Competence, β 12 0.002733 0.006961 0.393 270 0.695 Hispanic, β 13-0.074560 0.025417-2.933 270 0.004 Hispanic*Female, β 14 0.118568 0.034840 3.403 270 <0.001 Science Grades, β 15-0.001359 0.008754-0.155 270 0.877 For Situational Interest slope, π 2 Intercept2, β 20 0.387695 0.063039 6.150 270 <0.001 Female, β 21 0.049872 0.070763 0.705 270 0.482 Competence, β 22 0.018899 0.024516 0.771 270 0.441 Hispanic, β 23 0.085854 0.079684 1.077 270 0.282 Hispanic*Female, β 24-0.118952 0.090341-1.317 270 0.189 Science Grades, β 25-0.022321 0.022633-0.986 270 0.325 Notes: N= 276 Level-1 units (students), 2402 Level-2 units (responses)