A new approach for quantifying the effects of response elaboration training

Similar documents
Discussion Data reported here confirm and extend the findings of Antonucci (2009) which provided preliminary evidence that SFA treatment can result

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

Presentation Summary. Methods. Qualitative Approach

Beeson, P. M. (1999). Treating acquired writing impairment. Aphasiology, 13,

Index. Language Test (ANELT), 29, 235 auditory comprehension, 4,58, 100 Blissymbolics, 305

Clinical Review Criteria Related to Speech Therapy 1

DIBELS Next BENCHMARK ASSESSMENTS

More ESL Teaching Ideas

Supported Reading Comprehension for People with Aphasia: Visual and Linguistic Supports

Student Support Services Evaluation Readiness Report. By Mandalyn R. Swanson, Ph.D., Program Evaluation Specialist. and Evaluation

African American Male Achievement Update

Cooper Upper Elementary School

Exemplar 6 th Grade Math Unit: Prime Factorization, Greatest Common Factor, and Least Common Multiple

10 Tips For Using Your Ipad as An AAC Device. A practical guide for parents and professionals

Adults with traumatic brain injury (TBI) often have word retrieval problems (Barrow, et al., 2003; 2006; King, et al., 2006a; 2006b; Levin et al.

Formulaic Language and Fluency: ESL Teaching Applications

YMCA SCHOOL AGE CHILD CARE PROGRAM PLAN

Tun your everyday simulation activity into research

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

Review in ICAME Journal, Volume 38, 2014, DOI: /icame

MATH Study Skills Workshop

Shelters Elementary School

MERRY CHRISTMAS Level: 5th year of Primary Education Grammar:

Past, Present, and Future Tenses. Language Presentation by Mark

Summary / Response. Karl Smith, Accelerations Educational Software. Page 1 of 8

Sight Word Assessment

Writing Functional Dysphagia Goals

ANGLAIS LANGUE SECONDE

The Effects of Super Speed 100 on Reading Fluency. Jennifer Thorne. University of New England

Aspectual Classes of Verb Phrases

Language Acquisition Chart

The Effect of Written Corrective Feedback on the Accuracy of English Article Usage in L2 Writing

Facts and Figures Office of Institutional Research and Planning

Ohio s Learning Standards-Clear Learning Targets

Organizing Comprehensive Literacy Assessment: How to Get Started

Improving recruitment, hiring, and retention practices for VA psychologists: An analysis of the benefits of Title 38

Cooper Upper Elementary School

A Guide to Adequate Yearly Progress Analyses in Nevada 2007 Nevada Department of Education

Standard 5: The Faculty. Martha Ross James Madison University Patty Garvin

ILLINOIS DISTRICT REPORT CARD

Maximizing Learning Through Course Alignment and Experience with Different Types of Knowledge

Unraveling symbolic number processing and the implications for its association with mathematics. Delphine Sasanguie

ILLINOIS DISTRICT REPORT CARD

CEFR Overall Illustrative English Proficiency Scales

Junior (61-90 semester hours or quarter hours) Two-year Colleges Number of Students Tested at Each Institution July 2008 through June 2013

Guidelines for the Use of the Continuing Education Unit (CEU)

THE HEAD START CHILD OUTCOMES FRAMEWORK

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

SOFTWARE EVALUATION TOOL

The Four Principal Parts of Verbs. The building blocks of all verb tenses.

SURVIVING ON MARS WITH GEOGEBRA

12- A whirlwind tour of statistics

Cambridgeshire Community Services NHS Trust: delivering excellence in children and young people s health services

(I couldn t find a Smartie Book) NEW Grade 5/6 Mathematics: (Number, Statistics and Probability) Title Smartie Mathematics

OVERVIEW OF CURRICULUM-BASED MEASUREMENT AS A GENERAL OUTCOME MEASURE

Lancaster Lane CP School. The Importance of Motor Skills

South Carolina English Language Arts

Document number: 2013/ Programs Committee 6/2014 (July) Agenda Item 42.0 Bachelor of Engineering with Honours in Software Engineering

INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY

PROGRAM REQUIREMENTS FOR RESIDENCY EDUCATION IN DEVELOPMENTAL-BEHAVIORAL PEDIATRICS

Writing Functional Ot Goals In Snf

Construction Grammar. University of Jena.

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

Part I. Figuring out how English works

Language Acquisition by Identical vs. Fraternal SLI Twins * Karin Stromswold & Jay I. Rifkin

On May 3, 2013 at 9:30 a.m., Miss Dixon and I co-taught a ballet lesson to twenty

MARY MCLEOD BETHUNE. A Dedicated Teacher

First Grade Curriculum Highlights: In alignment with the Common Core Standards

PART 1. A. Safer Keyboarding Introduction. B. Fifteen Principles of Safer Keyboarding Instruction

ELA/ELD Standards Correlation Matrix for ELD Materials Grade 1 Reading

Language acquisition: acquiring some aspects of syntax.

First Grade Standards

Tracy Dudek & Jenifer Russell Trinity Services, Inc. *Copyright 2008, Mark L. Sundberg

The College Board Redesigned SAT Grade 12

Communication around Interactive Tables

South Carolina College- and Career-Ready Standards for Mathematics. Standards Unpacking Documents Grade 5

Author: Justyna Kowalczys Stowarzyszenie Angielski w Medycynie (PL) Feb 2015

The Acquisition of English Grammatical Morphemes: A Case of Iranian EFL Learners

2,1 .,,, , %, ,,,,,,. . %., Butterworth,)?.(1989; Levelt, 1989; Levelt et al., 1991; Levelt, Roelofs & Meyer, 1999

Process Evaluations for a Multisite Nutrition Education Program

Which verb classes and why? Research questions: Semantic Basis Hypothesis (SBH) What verb classes? Why the truth of the SBH matters

The Effect of Extensive Reading on Developing the Grammatical. Accuracy of the EFL Freshmen at Al Al-Bayt University

Think A F R I C A when assessing speaking. C.E.F.R. Oral Assessment Criteria. Think A F R I C A - 1 -

Word Stress and Intonation: Introduction

English Nexus Offender Learning

Assessing speaking skills:. a workshop for teacher development. Ben Knight

Further, Robert W. Lissitz, University of Maryland Huynh Huynh, University of South Carolina ADEQUATE YEARLY PROGRESS

Laporan Penelitian Unggulan Prodi

Pathways to Health Professions of the Future

Nicole M. Rosa, PhD. Department of Psychology Worcester State University 486 Chandler Street Worcester, MA

Effectiveness of McGraw-Hill s Treasures Reading Program in Grades 3 5. October 21, Research Conducted by Empirical Education Inc.

How To: Structure Classroom Data Collection for Individual Students

Occupational Therapy and Increasing independence

Developing a College-level Speed and Accuracy Test

An Empirical and Computational Test of Linguistic Relativity

IMPROVING SPEAKING SKILL OF THE TENTH GRADE STUDENTS OF SMK 17 AGUSTUS 1945 MUNCAR THROUGH DIRECT PRACTICE WITH THE NATIVE SPEAKER

Section 7, Unit 4: Sample Student Book Activities for Teaching Listening

UNIVERSITY OF SOUTHERN MISSISSIPPI Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences SHS 726 Auditory Processing Disorders Spring 2016

2016 Match List. Residency Program Distribution by Specialty. Anesthesiology. Barnes-Jewish Hospital, St. Louis MO

Transcription:

Introduction A new approach for quantifying the effects of response elaboration training Response Elaboration Training (RET; Kearns, 1985, 1986) is a content-driven treatment for individuals with aphasia. Several studies by Kearns and colleagues indicate RET s unique loose training paradigm increases the amount of verbal information produced by individuals with non-fluent Broca s aphasia in response to picture stimuli (Gaddie, Kearns, & Yedor, 1991; Kearns, 1985, 1986; Kearns & Scher, 1989; Kearns & Yedor, 1991; Nessler, 2009), and that in some cases, RET effects generalize to other speaking partners, stimuli, and settings (Bennett, Wambaugh, & Nesslar, 2005; Gaddie et al., 1991; Kearns & Yedor, 1991). In most RET research, training effects are quantified by having the participant describe the same pictures used in RET. This provision of pictorial support limits conclusions that can be drawn about the effectiveness of RET and its generalizability. The present study examined the effectiveness and generalizability of RET on speaking performance of individuals with non-fluent Broca s aphasia with a sentence production task (SPT) that did not provide picture support. Methods Subjects Three adults with chronic non-fluent Broca s aphasia participated in the study. All were Native speakers of English who incurred a left-hemisphere ischemic stroke with resulting right hemiparesis, aphasia, and apraxia of speech. Participant characteristics are provided in Table 1. Treatment Subjects received 12 sessions of RET following procedures described by Kearns (1986). Twenty treatment stimuli were randomly selected for each participant from 30 color action photographs depicting common activities (Webber, 2001). The remaining 10 photographs were not used in treatment, but the action verbs associated with these photographs were used to assess generalization. Participants were seen for treatment three times per week for four weeks. For each treatment session, RET was administered two to three times using the 20 picture stimuli. Dependent Variable Effects of RET and generalization were assessed with a sentence production task (SPT). For the SPT, the participant was instructed to use each of the 30 action verbs depicted in the photographs used for training or to measure generalization in a sentence (e.g., put the word cooking in a sentence). The SPT was administered on four occasions before treatment, four occasions during treatment, and one month after treatment. The 30 verbs of the SPT, shown in Table 2, were randomized for each of the nine administrations of the SPT and participants responses were audio-recorded.

Measurement and Reliability Responses to the SPT were transcribed verbatim. Each of the 30 responses was evaluated in terms of (a) type of syntactic structure produced, (b) grammatical completeness, and (c) semantic appropriateness based on criteria listed in Table 3. To calculate inter-observer reliability, twenty-five percent of the sentence productions were randomly selected and evaluated by two independent observers. Their percentage of agreements was compared on point-to-point bases. The average agreement between the examiners and observers judgment was 88%. Results Figures 1-6 display subjects data for the nine SPT administrations. For each participant, SPT responses, based on verbs associated with the 20 trained photographs and 10 untrained photographs, are graphed separately, and data for type of syntactic structure, grammatical completeness, and semantic accuracy are shown in separate segments of the graph. Space limitations preclude full discussion of the performance of each participant. Figures 1, 3, and 5 indicate that all participants (1) decreased their production of non-sentences and increased their production of SV and SV+ sentences, (2) increased their use of grammatically complete sentences, and (3) increased their use of semantically appropriate responses from the Before Tx to the Treatment SPTs on verbs associated with treated photographs. Participants 1 and 2 reflected greater improvements on the SPT than participant 3, but participant 3 did markedly decrease his non-sentence productions. Figures 2, 4, and 6 indicate that all the participants improved their performance on the items of the SPT that required them to use a verb associated with a photograph not treated using RET. To provide further information on the effects of RET on sentence production, four verbs associated with the trained photographs were randomly selected for each subject from the first, fifth, and either the seventh or eighth SPT. Verbatim transcriptions of these utterances for each participant are shown in Table 4. These data further support the benefits of RET on the SPT. From these transcriptions, it can be seen that subjects increased the length and complexity of their sentence productions, and decreased the number of false starts, interjections, and other disfluencies associated with their sentence productions across the repeated administrations of the SPT. Table 5 shows the pre- and post-treatment Western Aphasia Battery (WAB; Kertesa, 2006) scores and picture description task data for subjects 1 and 3. Subject 2 was not available for post-treatment assessment or for his final SPT during treatment. Table 5 shows that participants 1 and 3 improved on the oral-language portion of the WAB and both improved on the three metrics of the picture description task: (1) time, (2) number of correct information units (CIU; Nicholas & Brookshire, 1993), and (3) mean length of utterance (MLU; Florance, 1981). Discussion

Most studies of RET have employed multiple baseline single-subject designs and provided subjects with far more treatment than received by the subjects of this study. Geographical and other issues of subject availability restricted this study to use of a case series design and provision of treatment to 12 sessions. Nevertheless, results of this study were positive with some generalization to other tasks and stimuli and confirmed the viability of RET as a treatment that is beneficial for persons with non-fluent Broca s aphasia. For this study, the effects of RET were quantified with a task that was not worked on in treatment, a sentence production task in which the subject had to put a specified verb in a sentence. Although the SPT was not worked on in treatment, subjects improved their performance on the SPT in three ways: (1) they produced successively fewer non-sentence productions and more sentences with an SV or SV+ construction, (2) their sentence productions reflected increased grammatical completeness, and (3) their sentence productions reflected increased semantic appropriateness. These results suggest RET has potential for indirectly affecting syntactic performance in spite of the fact that it is a content-driven approach. In addition, results of this study provide additional support for the generalizability of RET and its loose training procedure. Not only did the subjects improve the syntactic accuracy, grammatical completeness, and other aspects of performance on the SPT, but two of the three subjects improved their pre- and post-treatment performance on the WAB and two picture description tasks. Finally, as seen in Table 4, all subjects reflected improvements in sentence production as evinced by the transcriptions of the randomly selected sentence productions. These were largely qualitative and suggested that with treatment subject s sentences were longer, more complex, and more fluent. In sum, the sentence production task seems to be a viable alternative to quantifying the benefits of RET and eliminates possible contaminants associated with visual support from pictures used in training. The SPT could, of course, be improved upon. One means of doing this might be that of balancing the verbs used for the task and/or controlling for verb argument structure.

References Bennett, J., Wambaugh, J. & Nesslar, C. (2005). Stimulus generalization effects of response elaboration training. Presented at the Clinical Aphasiology Conference, Sanibel Island, FL. Florance, C. L. (1981). Methods of communication analysis used in family interaction therapy. In R. H. Brookshire (Ed.), Clinical aphasiology conference proceeding (pp. 204-211). Minneapolis, MN: BRK. Gaddie, A, Kearns, K. P., & Yedor, K. (1991) A Qualitative analysis of response elaboration training effects. Clinical Aphasiology, 19, 171-183. Goodglass, H. & Kaplan, E. (1983). The Boston diagnostic aphasia examination. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams and Wilkins. Kearns, K. P. (1985). Response elaboration training for patient initiated utterances. In R. H. Brookshire (Ed.), Clinical Aphasiology Conference Proceedings (pp. 196-204). Minneapolis: BRK. Kearns, K. P. (1986). Systematic programming of verbal elaboration skills in chronic Broca s aphasia. In R. C. Marshall (Ed.), Case studies in aphasia rehabilitation: For clinicians by clinicians (pp. 225-244). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. Kearn, K. P. & Scher, G. (1989). The generalization of response elaboration training effects. Clinical Aphasiology, 18, 223-238. Kearns, K. P. & Yedor, K. (1991). An alternating treatments comparison of loose training and a convergent treatment strategy. Clinical Aphasiology, 20, 223-238. Kertesa, A. (2006). Western Aphasia Battery-Revised. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment. Nicholas, L. E. & Brookshire, R. H. (1993). A system for quantifying the informativeness and efficiency of the connected speech of adults with aphasia. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 338-350. Nessler, C., Wambaugh, J. and Wright, S. (2009) Effects of response elaboration training on increased length and complexity of utterances with two participants with fluent aphasia. Presented at the Clinical Aphasiology Conference, Keystone, CO. Webber, S. G. (2001). Webber Photo Cards: Verbs. Greenville, S. C.: Super Duper Publications.

Table 1 Participant Characteristics Characteristics Participant 1 Participant 2 Participant 3 Age 63 66 64 Gender Female Male Male Race Caucasian African American Caucasian Months of post-onset 95 73 36 Years of education 12 12 14 Former occupation Office Manager Material Handler Robotics Technician Pre-morbid handedness Right Right Left Table 2 Action Verbs used in the SPT eating skiing Selling smelling running reading blowing throwing raking mopping vacuuming swimming swinging walking drinking rinsing mowing shouting hugging cooking exercising crying feeding teaching talking hanging sleeping painting jumping shaving

Table 3 Classifications of SPT Responses Parameter Classification Definition Example Syntactic Structure S-V Response has a subjectnoun and a verb (main or auxiliary) Person is cooking S-V+ Response has a subjectnoun, verb, plus additional information Person is cooking vegetables NS Response is nonsentential Cooking Grammatical Completeness Grammatical (credit is given for this parameter) Response is acceptable according to the grammar of Standard English The person is cooking Ungrammatical (no credit is given for this parameter) Response is not acceptable according to the grammar of Standard English Person cooking is good Semantic Appropriateness Appropriate (credit is given for this parameter) Response (a) contains target word or semantically related alternative, (b) is logically plausible, (c) is propositionally meaningful, and (d) has an SV or SV+ construction Man is cooking on the grill Non-appropriate (no credit is given for this parameter) Response does not meet the criteria for appropriate The man is cooking the mouse Participants production errors (e.g., paraphasias, articulation errors), extraneous words (e.g., fillers, repetitions, unintelligible words, interjections), and disruptions in fluency (e.g., pauses, restarts) were ignored by the examiner when classifying the SPT responses for syntactic structure, grammatical completeness, and semantic appropriateness.

Participant 1 Figure 1 Percentage of NS, SV, and SV+ constructions (top), grammatically complete sentences (middle), and semantically appropriate sentences (bottom) for the 20 action words corresponding to activities depicted in the trained photographs.

Participant 1 Figure 2 Percentage of NS, SV, and SV+ constructions (top), grammatically complete sentences (middle), and semantically appropriate sentences (bottom) for the 10 action words corresponding to activities depicted in the untrained photographs.

Participant 2 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 Before TX Treatment Grammatical Completeness 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 Before TX Treatment Semantic Appropriateness Figure 3 Percentage of NS, SV, and SV+ constructions (top), grammatically complete sentences (middle), and semantically appropriate sentences (bottom) for the 20 action words corresponding to activities depicted in the trained photographs.

Participant 2 100 80 Before TX Treatment 60 40 Semantic Appropriateness 20 0 Figure 4 Percentage of NS, SV, and SV+ constructions (top), grammatically complete sentences (middle), and semantically appropriate sentences (bottom) for the 10 action words corresponding to activities depicted in the untrained photographs.

Participant 3 Figure 5 Percentage of NS, SV, and SV+ constructions (top), grammatically complete sentences (middle), and semantically appropriate sentences (bottom) for the 20 action words corresponding to activities depicted in the trained photographs.

100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 Participant 3 Before TX Treatment F/U Before TX Treatment F/U Grammatical Completeness SV SV+ NS 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 Before TX Treatment F/U Semantic Appropriateness Figure 6 Percentage of NS, SV, and SV+ constructions (top), grammatically complete sentences (middle), and semantically appropriate sentences (bottom) for the 10 action words corresponding to activities depicted in the untrained photographs.

Table 4 Examples of responses to the SPT provided by participant 1 (top), participant 2 (middle), and participant 3 (bottom). Target: Jumping Jumping is nice Oh man, uh, jumping, uh, the like to jump Eighth SPT: Man is jumping the rope Target: Swinging Swinging um up there and exercise A swinging, uh, the girls swinging Eighth SPT: Uh, alright, the man is swinging the baby in the garage, uh, the, uh, garden Target: Hanging Hanging on the hook Um uh.hanging, the man is hanging Eighth SPT: Uh, alright, uh, the man is hanging around Target: Reading I like the reading, but I can t do it Reading is, uh uh, reading is understand the I like to read Eighth SPT: O.k., the, uh um alright, the man is reading the library book Target: Reading Seventh SPT: Target: Selling Seventh SPT: Target: Blowing Seventh SPT: Target: Mowing Seventh SPT: Target: Eating Eighth SPT: Target: Rinsing Eighth SPT: Target: Mowing Eighth SPT: Target: Raking Eighth SPT: Reading. I want to read I want to, let s see, I want to throw Reading, I was reading the book I um nope Selling uhm Selling, I was selling ba, ba, (unintelligible word, then laughter) Blowing blowing blowing I want a Blowing blow, blow, I hum, hum, hum, um I was blowing I was blowing Mowing, huh, I want. I want. I want. I want to mow Mowing bow, bowing, I want to mow Mowing, I was mowing I don t know Eat oh, uh I can t, here too good Uh man is I don t know, I, I can t Wash hair Uh uh hair blow, uh uh I don t know, I Uh uh, man is sss shave, no, shampoo I don t know Uh uh uh I don t, I can t Uh man is uh, I don t know, it s, uh, I don t know I don t know Rrr I don t know Uh, man is rake, raking, uh, leaves

Table 5 Results from Pre-study and Post-study Testing for Participant 1 and Participant 3 Measure Participant 1 Participant 3 Pre-study Post-study Pre-study Post-study Western Aphasia Battery-Revised Aphasia Quotient (AQ) 60.2 65.4 44.8 54.4 Subtests Informational content 8 8 8 8 Fluency 4 4 2 4 Auditory verbal comprehension 6.9 7.8 7 6.9 Repetition 5.8 6.1 1.4 2.4 Naming 5.4 6.8 4 5.9 Picture description Cookie Theft picture (Goodglass &Kaplan, 1983) Time 2:41 2:30 1:51 2:09 Number of CIUs 16 20 5 6 MLU 2.4 2.5 1.4 2.6 Picnic Scene picture (Kertesa, 2006) Time 4:31 4:19 3:54 3:59 Number of CIUs 19 24 10 24 MLU 2.04 3.0 1.5 3.2