Acquisition of Nasal Place Assimilation by 4.5-month-old Infants Peter W. Jusczyk, Paul Smolensky, Karen Arnold, and Elliott Moreton

Similar documents
Parallel Evaluation in Stratal OT * Adam Baker University of Arizona

Markedness and Complex Stops: Evidence from Simplification Processes 1. Nick Danis Rutgers University

**Note: this is slightly different from the original (mainly in format). I would be happy to send you a hard copy.**

Precedence Constraints and Opacity

An argument from speech pathology

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

Infants learn phonotactic regularities from brief auditory experience

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

Manner assimilation in Uyghur

The presence of interpretable but ungrammatical sentences corresponds to mismatches between interpretive and productive parsing.

LING 329 : MORPHOLOGY

SOUND STRUCTURE REPRESENTATION, REPAIR AND WELL-FORMEDNESS: GRAMMAR IN SPOKEN LANGUAGE PRODUCTION. Adam B. Buchwald

Pobrane z czasopisma New Horizons in English Studies Data: 18/11/ :52:20. New Horizons in English Studies 1/2016

Partial Class Behavior and Nasal Place Assimilation*

Som and Optimality Theory

Linguistics 220 Phonology: distributions and the concept of the phoneme. John Alderete, Simon Fraser University

The Perception of Nasalized Vowels in American English: An Investigation of On-line Use of Vowel Nasalization in Lexical Access

On the nature of voicing assimilation(s)

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Lexical phonology. Marc van Oostendorp. December 6, Until now, we have presented phonological theory as if it is a monolithic

To appear in The TESOL encyclopedia of ELT (Wiley-Blackwell) 1 RECASTING. Kazuya Saito. Birkbeck, University of London

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

AGENDA LEARNING THEORIES LEARNING THEORIES. Advanced Learning Theories 2/22/2016

SOFTWARE EVALUATION TOOL

Underlying Representations

Tutoring First-Year Writing Students at UNM

The Effect of Written Corrective Feedback on the Accuracy of English Article Usage in L2 Writing

The Odd-Parity Parsing Problem 1 Brett Hyde Washington University May 2008

Acquiring Competence from Performance Data

Speech Recognition at ICSI: Broadcast News and beyond

AMLA 600: Second Language and Immersion Methodologies Summer 2015 Concordia College/Concordia Language Villages Dr. Paul J. Hoff

Using computational modeling in language acquisition research

Revisiting the role of prosody in early language acquisition. Megha Sundara UCLA Phonetics Lab

I propose an analysis of thorny patterns of reduplication in the unrelated languages Saisiyat

Communicative signals promote abstract rule learning by 7-month-old infants

Linking object names and object categories: Words (but not tones) facilitate object categorization in 6- and 12-month-olds

raıs Factors affecting word learning in adults: A comparison of L2 versus L1 acquisition /r/ /aı/ /s/ /r/ /aı/ /s/ = individual sound

Language Acquisition by Identical vs. Fraternal SLI Twins * Karin Stromswold & Jay I. Rifkin

Lecture 2: Quantifiers and Approximation

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

The Acquisition of English Grammatical Morphemes: A Case of Iranian EFL Learners

NAME: East Carolina University PSYC Developmental Psychology Dr. Eppler & Dr. Ironsmith

LEXICAL CATEGORY ACQUISITION VIA NONADJACENT DEPENDENCIES IN CONTEXT: EVIDENCE OF DEVELOPMENTAL CHANGE AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES.

Monitoring Metacognitive abilities in children: A comparison of children between the ages of 5 to 7 years and 8 to 11 years

Learning and Teaching

Universal contrastive analysis as a learning principle in CAPT

A Level Playing-Field: Perceptibility and Inflection in English Compounds. Robert Kirchner and Elena Nicoladis (U. Alberta)

Listener-oriented phonology

Age Effects on Syntactic Control in. Second Language Learning

Learners Use Word-Level Statistics in Phonetic Category Acquisition

Constraining X-Bar: Theta Theory

Linguistics Program Outcomes Assessment 2012

Unraveling symbolic number processing and the implications for its association with mathematics. Delphine Sasanguie

Canadian raising with language-specific weighted constraints Joe Pater, University of Massachusetts Amherst

How Does Physical Space Influence the Novices' and Experts' Algebraic Reasoning?

L1 and L2 acquisition. Holger Diessel

The analysis starts with the phonetic vowel and consonant charts based on the dataset:

A GENERIC SPLIT PROCESS MODEL FOR ASSET MANAGEMENT DECISION-MAKING

The phonological grammar is probabilistic: New evidence pitting abstract representation against analogy

Different Task Type and the Perception of the English Interdental Fricatives

Understanding the Relationship between Comprehension and Production

Language Acquisition Fall 2010/Winter Lexical Categories. Afra Alishahi, Heiner Drenhaus

Part I. Figuring out how English works

Listening and Speaking Skills of English Language of Adolescents of Government and Private Schools

Why PPP won t (and shouldn t) go away

Perceived speech rate: the effects of. articulation rate and speaking style in spontaneous speech. Jacques Koreman. Saarland University

The Effect of Extensive Reading on Developing the Grammatical. Accuracy of the EFL Freshmen at Al Al-Bayt University

Ling/Span/Fren/Ger/Educ 466: SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION. Spring 2011 (Tuesdays 4-6:30; Psychology 251)

STAFF DEVELOPMENT in SPECIAL EDUCATION

Acoustic correlates of stress and their use in diagnosing syllable fusion in Tongan. James White & Marc Garellek UCLA

Developing a TT-MCTAG for German with an RCG-based Parser

Visual processing speed: effects of auditory input on

The optimal placement of up and ab A comparison 1

Notes on The Sciences of the Artificial Adapted from a shorter document written for course (Deciding What to Design) 1

Comparison Between Three Memory Tests: Cued Recall, Priming and Saving Closed-Head Injured Patients and Controls

Process-specific constraints in Optimality Theory

To appear in the Proceedings of the 35th Meetings of the Chicago Linguistics Society. Post-vocalic spirantization: Typology and phonetic motivations

Phenomena of gender attraction in Polish *

Towards a Robuster Interpretive Parsing

The Role of Test Expectancy in the Build-Up of Proactive Interference in Long-Term Memory

Optimality Theory and the Minimalist Program

Word learning as Bayesian inference

have to be modeled) or isolated words. Output of the system is a grapheme-tophoneme conversion system which takes as its input the spelling of words,

Writing an Effective Research Proposal

Dyslexia/dyslexic, 3, 9, 24, 97, 187, 189, 206, 217, , , 367, , , 397,

Phonological Encoding in Sentence Production

Presentation Format Effects in a Levels-of-Processing Task

What is PDE? Research Report. Paul Nichols

Morphosyntactic and Referential Cues to the Identification of Generic Statements

Evolution of Symbolisation in Chimpanzees and Neural Nets

Correspondence between the DRDP (2015) and the California Preschool Learning Foundations. Foundations (PLF) in Language and Literacy

Speech Recognition using Acoustic Landmarks and Binary Phonetic Feature Classifiers

ROSETTA STONE PRODUCT OVERVIEW

Rubric for Scoring English 1 Unit 1, Rhetorical Analysis

English Language and Applied Linguistics. Module Descriptions 2017/18

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 143 ( 2014 ) CY-ICER Teacher intervention in the process of L2 writing acquisition

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Sound symbolism in deictic words

Laporan Penelitian Unggulan Prodi

VIEW: An Assessment of Problem Solving Style

Books Effective Literacy Y5-8 Learning Through Talk Y4-8 Switch onto Spelling Spelling Under Scrutiny

Transcription:

Acquisition of Nasal Place Assimilation by 4.5-month-old Infants Peter W., Paul Smolensky, Karen Arnold, and Elliott Moreton Infants 4.5 months of age are hypothesized to be in the "initial state" for acquisition, which in Optimality Theory means their grammars rank all markedness constraints above any faithfulness constraints. We investigated their sensitivity to nasal place assimilation as a means of comparing two versions of OT: the standard version of Alan Prince and Paul Smolensky, and the "Targeted OT" of Colin Wilson. Nasal place assimilation is a cross-linguistically common process in which nasal stops take on the place of articulation of a following obstruent consonant: impossible, intangible, incongruous. Adult English speakers do not have nasal place assimilation in most morphological contexts, while speakers of many other languages do e.g., Japanese, Latin, Polish, Zoque, Luganda, Swahili. Since adult languages with nasal place assimilation have a proper subset of the consonant clusters found in adult languages without nasal place assimilation, the language learner must start with the hypothesis that the input language has nasal place assimilation (lacks different-place NC clusters), so that positive data (the existence of different-place NC clusters in the input language) can cause him or her to reject that hypothesis. Starting with the opposite assumption would make nasal place assimilation unlearnable, since no positive evidence would ever contradict the hypothesis that there is no nasal place assimilation a typical subset problem. The analysis of nasal place assimilation in standard phonological Optimality Theory (Prince & Smolensky, 993; McCarthy & Prince, 995) is that a markedness constraint against different-place NC clusters, AGREE (PLACE), dominates a faithfulness constraint against altering place of articulation, IDENT (PLACE). The initial assumption of nasal place assimilation is expressed as an initial state in which AGREE (PLACE) dominates IDENT (PLACE). A competing analysis is offered by the theory of targeted constraints (Wilson, 2000): Nasal Place Assimilation happens in order to satisfy a markedness constraint that only compares candidates that are sufficiently close with respect to a particular perceptual-similarity relation. The crucial difference between these two accounts, for our purposes, is this: In the initial state, with all markedness constraints ranked above all faithfulness constraints, Regular OT says that any change to the concatenation /am+di/ that gets rid of the different-place cluster is a good repair (improves harmony), while the targeted-constraint theory says that only Nasal Place Assimilation is. To test these two competing accounts, we made use of the Headturn Preference Procedure (Kemler Nelson et al., 995) under the auxiliary hypothesis that there would be a familiarity preference: When given a choice, infants prefer to listen to speech that is more like the language they are acquiring. We interpret this as meaning that they prefer speech that matches their internal model of that language, i.e, the grammar they have acquired so far. This assumption allows us to probe the state of a child s grammar by giving them a choice between two different kinds of stimuli, A (which has a particular property) and B (which lacks it). A preference for B can be interpreted as evidence that the property is ungrammatical for that child.

We used a paradigm developed by, Smolensky, and Alloco (2002). Stimuli simulated inter-word Nasal Place Assimilation: () Stimulus schema X... Y... X Y am... da... anda (for example) That is, the X and Y are to be interpreted as faithfully realized individual words, and the X Y as their concatenation; in this example, with nasal place assimilation. Stimuli like these could be presented in the same experimental session with stimuli like am... da... amda, with no nasal place assimilation, and infants listening preferences for the two kinds of stimulus could be compared. et al. (2002) found that infants whether in the inital state at 4.5 months, or after considerable learning at 0 months preferred the assimilated clusters over faithful realizations, at least when the only faithfulness violation is the change of place in the nasal. That is, (2) Findings of Juszyk et al. (2002) for 4.5 month-olds /X/ /Y/ [X Y ] et al. 200 + Doesn t need assimilation. Needs assimilation, but is not + Needs assimilation; nasal. am am 2. an an N N assimilated. assimilates to obstruent 4. an N andi N C Needs assimilation; obstruent assimilates to nasal. eriment. What if instead the violation were repaired by changing place the place of the obstruent? Standard OT predicts that this will improve harmony compared to the fully faithful candidate; the theory of targeted constraints predicts it will do the opposite. Using the same experimental paradigm and subject population as et al. (2002), we presented 4.5 month-olds with a choice between stimuli of the form an...an (no assimilation) and an...andi (assimilation of the obstruent to the nasal). 49 English-learning infants participated. 7 were excluded fussed out, 4 did not look to sides, was interrupted by noise from construction work, and heard French at home leaving N=32 valid listeners 8 female, 4 male, ranging in age from 22 days to 56 days, with a mean age of 39 days.. Mean listening time for the no-assimilation stimuli was 3.6s, while that for the obstruent-assimilated stimuli was 4.0s. The mean difference for each subject was 0.37s in favor of the unassimilated stimuli, which was not

significantly different from 0 by a 2-sided t test (s.d. = 4.26s, p > 0.40). Surprisingly, it appeared that the infants in this experiment did not distinguish between the unassimilated stimuli (normal in many languages, including English) and the obstruent-assimilated ones (which never occur in any language). This result did not support either the standard OT hypothesis or the targeted-constraint hypothesis. (3) Accumulated findings /X/ /Y/ [X Y ] et al. 200. am am + 2. an an = N N + 4. an andi = N N C These results could have come about through antagonism between AGREE (PLACE) and IDENT (PLACE). Since each of the stimulus types in this experiment (Types 2 and 4 in the tables above) violated one constraint or the other, it could be that listener preferences were affected equally by both. If so, this would then raise the question of why the listeners of et al. (200) preferred Type 3 stimuli (violating IDENT (PLACE) but satisfying AGREE (PLACE)) over Type 2 stimuli (which did the reverse). An obvious hypothesis was that they could hear the place difference in the obstruent syllable onsets of this experiment, but not in the nasal syllable codas of et al. (200). eriment 2 was conducted to test this. Listeners were presented with a choice between assimilated and unassimilated stimuli which both satisfied AGREE (PLACE): an...am, Type 3, versus am...am, Type. Stimuli were recorded by the same speaker as in eriment. 33 listeners participated. 9 were excluded: 5 fussed out, 2 would not look, was lost to experimenter error, and produced extreme outlying data. This left N=24 for analysis, 9 female, 5 male, ranging in age from 28 to 77 days, with a mean age of 42 days. Mean listening time for the Type 3 stimuli was 6.7s; that for the Type stimuli was 4.6s. The average of each subject s difference was 2.s in favor of the Type 3 stimuli, significantly different from 0 by a 2-sided t test (s.d. = 4.56s, p < 0.05). This confirmed that 4.5-month-olds are indeed able to detect a violation of IDENT (PLACE) caused by a change in coda nasal place, as hypothesized by et al. (2002). However, it raised the troubling question of why listeners should favor stimuli which violate a constraint over those which violate no constraint, when both are consistent with their grammar. (4) Accumulated findings

/X/ /Y/ [X Y ] et al. 200 2. am am + + 2. an an = N N + 4. an andi = N N C A possible explanation for the findings of eriments and 2 was that we had the wrong linking hypothesis: Rather than preferring stimuli consistent with their current grammar over stimuli inconsistent with it, infants prefer stimuli which are consistent with their current grammar over stimuli which are consistent with an alternative grammar. The lack of preference for Type 4 over Type 2 stimuli in eriment would then result from the impossilty of any grammar in which Type 4 outputs would be optimal (no adult language has that pattern). The preference for Type 3 over Type stimuli in eriment 2 would come about because the Type 3 stimuli are consistent with the grammar AGREE (PLACE)»IDENT (PLACE) and inconsistent with the opposite ranking, while the Type stimuli are consistent with both. eriment 3 was designed to falsify this. Listeners were presented with a choice between stimuli which undergo nasal place assimilation (Type 3, an...am) and stimuli which undergo obstruent place assimilation (Type 4, the impossible an...andi). If listeners did in fact prefer grammatical over ungrammatical stimuli, they should listen longer to the Type 3s. Materials were recorded by the same speaker as in eriments and 2, and the same procedure was followed as in those experiments. 33 infants participated. 7 fussed out, was dropped due to experimenter error, and was dropped due to parental interference, leaving N=24 valid participants, 8 female, 6 male, ranging in age from 7 days to 53 days, with a mean age of 39 days. Mean listening time for the Type 3 stimuli was 5.4s; that for the Type 4 stimuli was 4.9s. The mean of each subject s difference was 0.52s in favor of the Type 3 stimuli, not significantly different from 0 by a 2-sided t test (s.d. = 3.73s, p > 0.30). (5) Accumulated findings /X/ /Y/ [X Y ] et al. 2002 2 3. am N 2 2. an N am N 2 + + an N

+ = 4. an andi = = N N C The results, such as they are, are consistent with the hypothesis that this listener population only shows preference between stimuli that are consistent with the current grammar, and stimuli that are consistent with some alternative grammar. When offered a choice between stimuli which are inconsistent with any grammar (Type 4) and stimuli that are consistent with either the current grammar (Type 2) or an alternative grammar (Type 3), infants show no preference; when offered a choice between stimuli which are consistent with the current grammar, and those consistent with an alternative, they prefer the former. References, Peter W., Paul Smolensky, and Theresa Allocco (2002). How Englishlearning infants respond to markedness and faithfulness constraints. Language Acquisition 0():3 73. Kemler Nelson, D. G., P. W., D. R. Mandel, J. Myers, A. Turk, and L. Gerken (995). The headturn preference procedure for testing auditory perception. Infant Behavior and Development, 8, -6. McCarthy, John J., and Alan Prince (995). Faithfulness and reduplicative identity. In: Jill N. Beckman, Laura Walsh Dickey, and Suzanne Urbanczyk (eds.), Papers in Optimality Theory. Amherst, Mass.: Graduate Linguistic Students Association. Prince, Alan, and Paul Smolensky (993). Optimality Theory: constraint interaction in generative grammar. MS, Department of Linguistics, Rutgers University. Wilson, Colin (2000). Targeted constraints: an approach to contextual neutralization in Optimality Theory. Ph. D. dissertation, Department of Cognitive Science, Johns Hopkins University.