Chapter 7: Summary of results - a blending account of the binyanim system.

Similar documents
Approaches to control phenomena handout Obligatory control and morphological case: Icelandic and Basque

Underlying and Surface Grammatical Relations in Greek consider

Constraining X-Bar: Theta Theory

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Construction Grammar. University of Jena.

Proof Theory for Syntacticians

LING 329 : MORPHOLOGY

Pseudo-Passives as Adjectival Passives

Derivational and Inflectional Morphemes in Pak-Pak Language

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 154 ( 2014 )

California Department of Education English Language Development Standards for Grade 8

Frequency and pragmatically unmarked word order *

Linguistic Variation across Sports Category of Press Reportage from British Newspapers: a Diachronic Multidimensional Analysis

Specification and Evaluation of Machine Translation Toy Systems - Criteria for laboratory assignments

AN INTRODUCTION (2 ND ED.) (LONDON, BLOOMSBURY ACADEMIC PP. VI, 282)

Inleiding Taalkunde. Docent: Paola Monachesi. Blok 4, 2001/ Syntax 2. 2 Phrases and constituent structure 2. 3 A minigrammar of Italian 3

Some Principles of Automated Natural Language Information Extraction

Chapter 3: Semi-lexical categories. nor truly functional. As Corver and van Riemsdijk rightly point out, There is more

Maximizing Learning Through Course Alignment and Experience with Different Types of Knowledge

Derivational: Inflectional: In a fit of rage the soldiers attacked them both that week, but lost the fight.

Author: Justyna Kowalczys Stowarzyszenie Angielski w Medycynie (PL) Feb 2015

Language Acquisition Fall 2010/Winter Lexical Categories. Afra Alishahi, Heiner Drenhaus

An Interactive Intelligent Language Tutor Over The Internet

Beyond constructions:

Compositional Semantics

The College Board Redesigned SAT Grade 12

Developing True/False Test Sheet Generating System with Diagnosing Basic Cognitive Ability

AQUA: An Ontology-Driven Question Answering System

Guidelines for Writing an Internship Report

LEXICAL COHESION ANALYSIS OF THE ARTICLE WHAT IS A GOOD RESEARCH PROJECT? BY BRIAN PALTRIDGE A JOURNAL ARTICLE

Argument structure and theta roles

Advanced Grammar in Use

DOI /cog Cognitive Linguistics 2013; 24(2):

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

Digital Fabrication and Aunt Sarah: Enabling Quadratic Explorations via Technology. Michael L. Connell University of Houston - Downtown

Introduction to HPSG. Introduction. Historical Overview. The HPSG architecture. Signature. Linguistic Objects. Descriptions.

CS 598 Natural Language Processing

Words come in categories

1/20 idea. We ll spend an extra hour on 1/21. based on assigned readings. so you ll be ready to discuss them in class

Type Theory and Universal Grammar

Probability and Statistics Curriculum Pacing Guide

Case government vs Case agreement: modelling Modern Greek case attraction phenomena in LFG

THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF HUMANITIES & SOCIAL STUDIES

The Strong Minimalist Thesis and Bounded Optimality

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

Welcome to the Purdue OWL. Where do I begin? General Strategies. Personalizing Proofreading

A construction analysis of [be done X] in Canadian English

GERM 3040 GERMAN GRAMMAR AND COMPOSITION SPRING 2017

Word Stress and Intonation: Introduction

Aspectual Classes of Verb Phrases

Update on Soar-based language processing

Today we examine the distribution of infinitival clauses, which can be

South Carolina English Language Arts

Lecture 2: Quantifiers and Approximation

Developing a TT-MCTAG for German with an RCG-based Parser

Linking Task: Identifying authors and book titles in verbose queries

Universal Grammar 2. Universal Grammar 1. Forms and functions 1. Universal Grammar 3. Conceptual and surface structure of complex clauses

ELD CELDT 5 EDGE Level C Curriculum Guide LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT VOCABULARY COMMON WRITING PROJECT. ToolKit

cambridge occasional papers in linguistics Volume 8, Article 3: 41 55, 2015 ISSN

Modeling full form lexica for Arabic

Which verb classes and why? Research questions: Semantic Basis Hypothesis (SBH) What verb classes? Why the truth of the SBH matters

Rubric for Scoring English 1 Unit 1, Rhetorical Analysis

On-Line Data Analytics

An Empirical and Computational Test of Linguistic Relativity

PROCESS USE CASES: USE CASES IDENTIFICATION

THE VERB ARGUMENT BROWSER

Parsing of part-of-speech tagged Assamese Texts

Dissertation Summaries. The Acquisition of Aspect and Motion Verbs in the Native Language (Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, 2014)

Target Language Preposition Selection an Experiment with Transformation-Based Learning and Aligned Bilingual Data

Basic Syntax. Doug Arnold We review some basic grammatical ideas and terminology, and look at some common constructions in English.

Ch VI- SENTENCE PATTERNS.

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

Variation of English passives used by Swedes

Reading Grammar Section and Lesson Writing Chapter and Lesson Identify a purpose for reading W1-LO; W2- LO; W3- LO; W4- LO; W5-

UC Berkeley Berkeley Undergraduate Journal of Classics

Opportunities for Writing Title Key Stage 1 Key Stage 2 Narrative

TRANSITIVITY IN THE LIGHT OF EVENT RELATED POTENTIALS

Parallel Evaluation in Stratal OT * Adam Baker University of Arizona

Objectives. Chapter 2: The Representation of Knowledge. Expert Systems: Principles and Programming, Fourth Edition

AGS THE GREAT REVIEW GAME FOR PRE-ALGEBRA (CD) CORRELATED TO CALIFORNIA CONTENT STANDARDS

Mathematics Scoring Guide for Sample Test 2005

Authors note Chapter One Why Simpler Syntax? 1.1. Different notions of simplicity

Discourse markers and grammaticalization

Copyright and moral rights for this thesis are retained by the author

PAGE(S) WHERE TAUGHT If sub mission ins not a book, cite appropriate location(s))

What the National Curriculum requires in reading at Y5 and Y6

Strategic discourse comprehension

THINKING TOOLS: Differentiating the Content. Nanci Cole, Michelle Wikle, and Sacha Bennett - TOSAs Sandi Ishii, Supervisor of Gifted Education

Linguistics. Undergraduate. Departmental Honors. Graduate. Faculty. Linguistics 1

A Grammar for Battle Management Language

Citation for published version (APA): Veenstra, M. J. A. (1998). Formalizing the minimalist program Groningen: s.n.

Conceptual and Procedural Knowledge of a Mathematics Problem: Their Measurement and Their Causal Interrelations

Chapter 1 The functional approach to language and the typological approach to grammar

On the Notion Determiner

OCR for Arabic using SIFT Descriptors With Online Failure Prediction

AGENDA LEARNING THEORIES LEARNING THEORIES. Advanced Learning Theories 2/22/2016

Prediction of Maximal Projection for Semantic Role Labeling

Transcription:

Chapter 7: Summary of results - a blending account of the binyanim system. In chapters 4-6 of the thesis, I developed a blending analysis of the binyanim system in Hebrew. The binyan, according to the analysis, is a grammatical marker for a conceptual and linguistic blending operation. In the blending operation, a complex event conceived in the world is blended with one of the languages' syntactic constructions. The blending operation involves the mapping of partial information from the conceived event onto the integrating syntactic construction. This operation results in a linguistic structure (the sentence communicated in the language) which represents the complex conceived event as an instance of a single tightly-integrated event structure. The role of the binyan, according to this analysis, is to mark the particular blending operation, and guide the hearer in the linguistic "de-integration" (or "un-packing") operation -- the reconstruction of a probable scenario which the speaker conceived and intended to communicate. The analysis suggests that each binyan is associated with a single blending (or mapping) schema (with the binyanim pa'al, nif'al, and hitpa'el each associated with two variations of a more generic schema). Irrespective of the particular syntactic construction being used (identifying the general event structure communicated in a sentence), the binyan marks the particular elements that are mapped from the conceived event onto the integrating syntactic construction (and are thus explicitly expressed in the sentence). Figure 7-1 summarizes the blending schema(s) associated with each binyan : 163

164 PA AL - CaCaC PI EL - CiCeC HIF IL - hiccic......... A 1... P... NIF AL - niccac PU AL - CuCaC HUF AL - huccac A 1 P A 2 HITPA EL - hitcacec A 1 P Figure 7-1: Summary - a blending analysis of the binyanim system.

165 The blending analysis of the binyanim system, as described in Figure 7-1, provides a unified and systematic account for the various grammatical functions of the system. Each binyan is associated with a single blending schema, and the different schemas complement each other. Each blending schema is defined as a function of two parameters: (1) the predicates which are mapped onto the main verbal slot of the integrating syntactic construction (and profiled by the verbal root); and (2) the participants which are mapped onto the first slot (the "subject" slot) of the integrating syntactic construction. With regard to the mapping of predicates, the analysis identifies all together three types of mapping schemas from the conceived event onto the verbal slot of the syntactic construction: (a) the mapping of a causing predicate within a causal event; (b) the mapping of an effected predicate within a causal event; and (c) the mapping of an autonomous predicate (i.e., a predicate which is not itself part of a larger event sequence). The autonomous predicate may be either a predicate in a conceived single-predicate event structure, or the conceptual and linguistic integration of a sequence of predicates - a causing and effected predicates from a causal sequence of events). The three mapping schemas of predicates are illustrated in Figure 7-2. With regard to the mapping of participants, the analysis identifies again all together three types of mapping schemas from the conceived event onto the first (subject) slot of the syntactic construction: (a) the mapping of a causal agent in a causal event; (b) the mapping of an affected entity in a causal event; and (c) the mapping of an autonomous entity (i.e., an entity which is not part of a larger multi-participant event). The autonomous entity may be either a participant in a conceived single-participant event structure, or the conceptual and linguistic integration of a causal agent and an affected entity in a causal event). The three mapping schemas of participants are illustrated in Figure 7-3.

166 "CAUSING PREDICATE" "EFFECTED PREDICATE" "AUTONOMUS PREDICATE" A X 1 B X 1 C (i) X 1 P 1 P 1 root root root X 2 X 2 X 2 P 2 C (ii) X 1 root P Figure 7-2: Summary - three mapping schemas of predicates in the binyanim system "CAUSAL AGENT" "AFFECTED ENTITY" "AUTONOMUS PARTICIPANT" A Subj X 1 B Subj X 1 C (i) Subj X 1 X 2 X 2 X 2 C (ii) Subj X 1 P Figure 7-3: Summary - three mapping schemas of participants in the binyanim system

167 Table 7-1 defines each binyan as a function of the two mapping parameters (the mapping of predicates and the mapping of participants from the conceived event onto the integrating syntactic contraction) identified in Figures 7-2 and 7-3. The horizontal axis in Table 7-1 represents the first parameter: what is mapped onto (and profiled by) the verbal root (Figure 7-2). The vertical axis represents the second parameter: what is mapped onto (and profiled by) the first nominal (subject) slot (Figure 7-3). Table 7-1: Summary - defining each binyan as a function of two parameters: a mapping schema of predicates (horizontal axis) and a mapping schema of participants (vertical axis). predicate mapping participant mapping causal agent Figure 7-3(A) affected entity Figure 7-3(B) autonomous Figure 7-3(C) autonomous predicate Figure 7-2(C) pa'al pi'el hif'il nif'al pu'al huf'al pa'al hitpa'el causing predicate Figure 7-2(A) hitpa'el effected predicate Figure 7-2(B) hif'il hitpa'el Note that according to Table 7-1, each binyan is associated indeed with a single blending schema (which is a combination of a mapping schema of predicates and a mapping schema of participants). When one binyan occurs in more than one cell in Table 7-1, the cells are in the same row or line: for example, hitpa'el occurs in three cells in Table 7-1, but all cells are in the same row. This means that hitpa'el is associated with one participant mapping schema (Figure 7-3/C), but is neutral with regard to (or generalizes over) the mapping of predicates. Each of the stems pa'al and hif'il occurs in two cells in Table 7-1, but the cells are in the same column. This means that pa'al and hif'il are each associated with one predicate mapping schema (Figures 7-2/C and 7-2/B respectively), but

168 generalize over the mapping of participants. Two binyanim that occur in one cell (e.g., pa'al and hitpa'el, or hif'il and hitpa'el) suggest that their semantics (blending function) overlaps in certain contexts (as discussed in section 6.4.2). The blending analysis of the Hebrew binyanim comes closest in its approach to the view suggested in Jenni's study of Biblical Hebrew (1968), summarized and reiterated in Siebesma (1991). Jenni assumes that the verbal binyanim system originally formed a closed system of grammatical categories. In the system each of the seven verbal stems had, morphologically and semantically, a clearly defined function distinct from that of others. Each verbal stem stood, in form and meaning, in distinctive opposition to each of the other verbal stems. The blending schemas identified for each binyan in this thesis also define a distinct function for each binyan, which is complementary to the functions of other binyanim. The blending schemas of all seven binyanim (Figure 7-1) form a unified grammatical system 1. The results of the blending analysis are also very similar to the ones reached by Waltke and O'Connor, 1990, for Biblical Hebrew (though the methodology and initial assumptions in the two accounts are very different). Compare Table 7-1, which summarizes the findings of the blending analysis of the binyanim system in this study, to Table 7-2 summarizing Waltke and O'Connor's results (as found in their book, 1990: 358). Waltke and O'Connor suggest that Hebrew binyanim are a function of the voice of a primary subject, and a secondary subject in a causative event (if one exists).the vertical axis in Table 7-2 represents the voice of the primary subject (PS), and the horizontal axis represents the voice of the secondary ("under-") subject (US). 1 It should be noted here again that the blending account proposed in this thesis for the binyanim system covers only the most frequent and prominent functions of each binyan. The large number of idiosyncrasies in the lexicon and minor grammatical functions associated with each binyan are not analyzed in this thesis.

169 Table 7-2. Waltke & O'Connor's (1990) analysis of the binyanim system oice of US Absence Passive Active oice of PS Active pa'al pi'el hif'il Passive/Middle nif'al pu'al huf'al Reflexive nif'al hitpa'el hif'il Note that both the analysis of binyanim proposed in this thesis, and the analysis by Waltke and O'Connor (1990), put causation (and the causative event structure) as the center pole around which the grammatical functions of the different binyanim are to be defined. And though the two analyses differ in their initial assumptions on what aspects of causation are grammatically marked by the binyanim system (Waltke and O'Connor focus on the marking of the voice of participants in the causal event, while my analysis focuses on the arrangement and interaction of predicates and participants in the causal sequence), the structural organization of the binyanim system as illustrated in Tables 7-1 and 7-2 is largely the same. The two tables differ in fact in the content of only one cell - the bottom left cell which is associated with the stem nif'al in Table 7-2 (Waltke & O'Connor's analysis), but with pa'al and hitpa'el in Table 7-1 (my analysis). Both analyses however associate this cell with a reflexive function (Waltke and O'Connor suggest that nif'al marks an "implicit reflexive notion... [where] the action affects the subject of the verb or her interests" (1990:356); In my analysis, the two binyanim -- pa'al and hitpa'el -- are associated with one-participant/one-predicate events with possible "distinguishability" of sub-components of the participant or predicate which provides the reflexive content (section 6.4.2). The gap between the two tables may thus be only due to the fact that Waltke and O'Connor analyze

170 Biblical Hebrew, in which time the nif'al played a larger reflexive role 2, while my analysis is of Modern Hebrew in which hitpa'el is the prominent reflexive binyan. The blending analysis of the binyanim system differs largely from traditional accounts of voice and causation. First, in contrast to accounts in which passive, middle, and causative constructions are derived from active clauses (see presentation in chapter 3), the blending analysis assumes that all binyanim (associated with active, passive, middle, reflexive, or causative functions) are generated from underlying conceptual structures, each binyan marking a special type of conceptual blending operation. Thus, no binyan is more "basic" than others. Consider for example, the traditional "active-passive" distinction. The three binyanim pa'al, pi'el and hif'il (the first row in Figure 7-1) are traditionally defined as "active". The three binyanim nif'al, pu'al and huf'al (the second row in Figure 7-1) are traditionally defined as "passive" (with nif'al also associated with "middle" and sometimes "reflexive" functions in Modern Hebrew grammar). In the blending analysis, the difference between the "active" and "passive" binyanim is defined in terms of their blending schemas: in all active binyanim, what is mapped onto the subject slot of the integrating syntactic construction is the causal agent; in the passive binyanim, what is mapped onto the subject slot is the affected entity in the conceived causal event (see Figure 7-1). The definition of the passive binyanim is therefore independent of the active binyanim, and is not a "derivation" from other basic forms. The link between a so called active binyan and its corresponding passive binyan (e.g., the link between hif'il and huf'al) is defined in the blending analysis in terms of similarity in predicate mapping schemas (e.g., in both hif'il and huf'al it is the effected predicate in the conceived causal sequence that is mapped onto 2 Research suggests that in pre-biblical time the nif'al was used primarily to denote the reflexive counterpart of pa'al verbs (Glinert, 1989; Siebesma, 1991). In biblical times, the nif'al took over the function of another binyan which was used to denote the passive counterpart of pa'al, and in Modern Hebrew, the nif'al functions primarily to denote passive and middle voice. Note that the development in the function of the binyan is motivated by the semantic overlap in the function of each of these "voice" phenomena (passive, middle, and reflexive) as suggested in this study as well as in Kemmer (1993).

171 the verbal slot of the integrating construction). By removing the requirement to link (or derive) passive, middle, or causative forms to more basic forms, the blending analysis proposed in this thesis has the advantage of easily accommodating instances of so-called "non-basic" verbs whose root does not occur in the standard Modern Hebrew lexicon in any other "underlying, basic" verbal form (from which the "complex" verb can be derived). Consider, for example, the many hitpa'el forms in standard and colloquial Hebrew (some of which are listed in Appendix C) which are based on adjectival or nominal roots (i.e., roots denoting stative predicates that occur with adjectival or nominal, but not other verbal, morphological patterns). In generative accounts which derive "complex" verbs from underlying "basic" verbal forms, such hitpa'el verbs require special account. For example, Junger (1987:88) who proposes to derive hitpa'el verbs by a detransitivization operation from the basic-transitive form of the root (typically pi'el), notes that reflexive verbs derived from nouns or adjectives "are not cases of detransitivization... since the root does not occur in any other verbal pattern", and hence require a different account. In the blending analysis, in contrast, since what is mapped onto the verbal slot of the integrating construction is a consonantal root (identifying an abstract semantic predicate) rather than any actual verbal form, the existence of other root-binyan combinations in the lexicon is irrelevant to the generation of a given "complex" form. As another example, consider the many nif'al verbs in the Modern Hebrew lexicon with no parallel pa'al form (or with a parallel pa'al form whose meaning is not the active variation of the nif'al). The roots of these nif'al verbs often co-occur with causative stems - hif'il, huf'al, pi'el or pu'al (discussed in section 6.2), as in examples 26-27 below (examples from Stern 1994:20; translations from Bolozky 1996): (26) a. nivhal (b.h.l-nif'al) - become frightened/startled b. hivhil (b.h.l--hif'il) - frighten/startle 3 (27) a. nexlac (x.l.c-nif'al) - be rescued, escape 4 3 The huf'al form of the root b.h.l (huvhal) is used as the passive form of a second homonymous sense of hivhil - 'to rush in', but not as the passive of the sense 'to frighten'.

172 b. xilec (x.l.c-pi'el) - rescue c. xulac (x.l.c-pu'al) - be rescued The nif'al forms in examples 26-27 (nivhal, nexlac) are defined in some studies as the "basic" forms from which the causative forms (hif'il) are derived, and in other studies (e.g., Stern, 1994:20) as additional "passive" derivations from the causative verb (the hif'il or pi'el form). These special analyses of particular nif'al verbs as derived from hif'il (rather than the "regular" pa'al) probably result from a requirement in most accounts of the binyanim system that the "complex" (passive, middle, or causative) verb be derived from a "basic" (active) verbal form. These proposals however seem somewhat ad-hoc, and present the nif'al stem as an irregular and ambiguous. In the blending account, examples such as 26-27 do not require special analysis. Since the semantic field is defined by the root, and not by any basic verbal form, there is no need to assign basic semantics to one of the realizations of the root over other realizations. The various root-stem combinations constitute different forms of linguistic blending and conceptual construal of the basic semantics denoted by the root 5. 4 The pa'al form of the root x.l.c (xalac) has the specific meaning of 'to remove', 'to take off (shoes)'. It is not the 'active' variation of the nif'al form semantics. 5 For example, starting with the hypothesis that the root semantics of x.l.c -`rescue' (in 27) prototypically involves causation and effect (i.e., some causal agent rescues another entity, thereby causing a change in the state of the entity - the 'affected' patient), the use of the pi'el stem to describe the agent's activity is motivated (i.e., it fits the general blending schema proposed for pi'el as marking causing predicates, i.e., predicates which involve some expected effect on a patient, in this case - the patient being `free' or `out')the nif'al form, as suggested in section 6.3, is used to denote an event where the causal force is unclear: it may be an external force which is not mentioned or is unknown (the passive construal), or the event may have "just happened" or caused by "internal" forces (the middle construal), i.e. the subject rescued herself, escaped. This characterization of the event contrast with the pu'al characterization which clearly suggests that another agent, external to the theme, was doing the rescue. Indeed, Stern (1994:20) notes a difference between the pu'al and nif'al forms in 27 that an agent phrase can be added to the pu'al form (denoting the causal agent in the rescuing event), but not to the nif'al form. This latter fact is in accord with the semantic characterization of the pu'al vs. the nif'al forms above. The important point to note is that the binyan assigned for describing each type of event (the pu'al type, or the nif'al type) in the blending analysis is independent from the existence of other forms in the standard lexicon: each binyan describes a particular type of blending schema (and thus a pragmatic construal) of an event, and the binyan whose schema best fits the characterization of the conceived event is the one to be used with the root (no derivation from other lexical forms is involved).

173 Waltke and O'Connor in their comprehensive work on Hebrew syntax (1990) express their dissatisfaction with previous accounts of the binyanim system: Three unfortunate problems have marred discussion of the stem system. First, scholars have tended to describe the system as based formally on the Qal [pa'al] stem. Second, they have described the stems notationally in an atomistic way, that is, assigning a meaning or set of meanings to each stem independently. Third, they have neglected the very systematic character of the system (p. 352). Waltke and O'Connor note in addition with regard to the first problem that while "it is correct to see a split between the Qal and the other stems,... there can be no doubt that all the stems are of the same order of phenomenon [italics added]" (p. 351, fn4). The blending analysis of Hebrew binyanim provides an account of the system which overcomes all three problems noted by Waltke and O'Connor. First, the analysis describes all binyanim as the same type of conceptual and linguistic phenomena. At the basis of this operation stand conceptual entities, each associated with a highly schematic lexical representation (i.e., representation which is not yet realized by an actual linguistic form). In Hebrew, these abstract representations equal the consonantal roots, which denote a "core" semantic field (or the semantic schema abstracted from all lexical items derived out of the root). Through a process of conceptual and linguistic blending, these lexical representations are blended with grammatical patterns (syntactic constructions and verbal stems) to yield the actual linguistic form (i.e., the verbal realization of the blend). The same type of operation is involved in generating basic pa'al (qal) verbs as well as causative pi'el and hif'il verbs and clauses. The difference lies in the particular mapping configuration associated with each stem, but not in the generic process. All the binyanim, hence, are of the "same order of phenomenon". Second, the blending analysis attempts to study the function of the Hebrew binyanim system as a whole. The analysis defines the different binyanim as marking different blending schemas: each binyan marks the mapping of different aspects of a conceived event into an integrating construction. Thus, the role of each binyan is defined in contrast to the

174 role of all other binyanim, and as complementing the function of others (Figure 7-1). Together, the seven binyanim form a unified formal system for marking a generic conceptual operation (blending). In addition to analyzing the connection between conceptual structure, meaning, and grammatical form in the binyanim system, the blending analysis provides some new insights into the system: the analysis proposes a clear distinction between the causative function of two different causative stems (pi'el and hif'il), and motivates some distributional facts about the system (which have not been explained so far) such as the limited proportion of three-place hif'il predicates (section 4.2.2). The analysis also accounts for the so-called "grammatically ambiguous" binyanim, such as nif'al and hitpa'el (chapter 6), by suggesting that a single mapping schema is associated with each binyan; and for the association of one grammatical function with several binyanim (e.g., the many passive binyanim) by pointing to differences in blending configurations associated with each passive binyan. Finally, the analysis suggests a novel way for analyzing the interaction between morphological binyanim and syntactic constructions and their interface with semantics: the analysis points to similarity in semantic structure of verbs in different binyanim but with the same argument structure (e.g., compare Figures 5-3-A and 5-3-B, in chapter 5), as well as of verbs in the same binyan but with different argument structures (e.g., compare Figures 4-3 and 4-4, in chapter 4).