Preschoolers Perspective Taking in Word Learning Do They Blindly Follow Eye Gaze?

Similar documents
Cognition 112 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Cognition. journal homepage:

Visual processing speed: effects of auditory input on

9.85 Cognition in Infancy and Early Childhood. Lecture 7: Number

Linking object names and object categories: Words (but not tones) facilitate object categorization in 6- and 12-month-olds

Language-Specific Patterns in Danish and Zapotec Children s Comprehension of Spatial Grams

Abstractions and the Brain

Statistical Analysis of Climate Change, Renewable Energies, and Sustainability An Independent Investigation for Introduction to Statistics

Shared Book Reading between Mother and Infant Facilitates The Frequency of Joint Attention

NAME: East Carolina University PSYC Developmental Psychology Dr. Eppler & Dr. Ironsmith

An Empirical and Computational Test of Linguistic Relativity

A Bootstrapping Model of Frequency and Context Effects in Word Learning

Language Acquisition Fall 2010/Winter Lexical Categories. Afra Alishahi, Heiner Drenhaus

The Evaluation of Students Perceptions of Distance Education

Word learning as Bayesian inference

Revisiting the role of prosody in early language acquisition. Megha Sundara UCLA Phonetics Lab

Good Enough Language Processing: A Satisficing Approach

Lecture 2: Quantifiers and Approximation

POLA: a student modeling framework for Probabilistic On-Line Assessment of problem solving performance

Eye Movements in Speech Technologies: an overview of current research

Cooperation and human cognition: the Vygotskian intelligence hypothesis

Critical Thinking in Everyday Life: 9 Strategies

Partner-Specific Adaptation in Dialog

Extending Place Value with Whole Numbers to 1,000,000

Arkansas Tech University Secondary Education Exit Portfolio

WE GAVE A LAWYER BASIC MATH SKILLS, AND YOU WON T BELIEVE WHAT HAPPENED NEXT

Rubric for Scoring English 1 Unit 1, Rhetorical Analysis

Monitoring Metacognitive abilities in children: A comparison of children between the ages of 5 to 7 years and 8 to 11 years

teacher, peer, or school) on each page, and a package of stickers on which

Learning By Asking: How Children Ask Questions To Achieve Efficient Search

A Critique of Running Records

A cautionary note is research still caught up in an implementer approach to the teacher?

Evidence for Reliability, Validity and Learning Effectiveness

Taylor & Francis, Ltd. is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Cognition and Instruction.

SOFTWARE EVALUATION TOOL

Testing protects against proactive interference in face name learning

Classifying combinations: Do students distinguish between different types of combination problems?

The Good Judgment Project: A large scale test of different methods of combining expert predictions

The Effect of Discourse Markers on the Speaking Production of EFL Students. Iman Moradimanesh

The Perception of Nasalized Vowels in American English: An Investigation of On-line Use of Vowel Nasalization in Lexical Access

The effects of imagery-based occupation on duration of prone extension in four-and five-year old children

Full text of O L O W Science As Inquiry conference. Science as Inquiry

Measurement. When Smaller Is Better. Activity:

Create Quiz Questions

Effects of speaker gaze on spoken language comprehension: Task matters

Rule-based Expert Systems

Effective practices of peer mentors in an undergraduate writing intensive course

The Impact of the Multi-sensory Program Alfabeto on the Development of Literacy Skills of Third Stage Pre-school Children

Probability estimates in a scenario tree

Scoring Guide for Candidates For retake candidates who began the Certification process in and earlier.

Concept Acquisition Without Representation William Dylan Sabo

Facing our Fears: Reading and Writing about Characters in Literary Text

Systematic reviews in theory and practice for library and information studies

Language Development: The Components of Language. How Children Develop. Chapter 6

Jacqueline C. Kowtko, Patti J. Price Speech Research Program, SRI International, Menlo Park, CA 94025

Feature-oriented vs. Needs-oriented Product Access for Non-Expert Online Shoppers

Student-led IEPs 1. Student-led IEPs. Student-led IEPs. Greg Schaitel. Instructor Troy Ellis. April 16, 2009

Age Effects on Syntactic Control in. Second Language Learning

Stopping rules for sequential trials in high-dimensional data

Revision activity booklet for Paper 1. Topic 1 Studying society

CHAPTER 5: COMPARABILITY OF WRITTEN QUESTIONNAIRE DATA AND INTERVIEW DATA

SETTING STANDARDS FOR CRITERION- REFERENCED MEASUREMENT

Mathematics process categories

Module 12. Machine Learning. Version 2 CSE IIT, Kharagpur

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

Results In. Planning Questions. Tony Frontier Five Levers to Improve Learning 1

Running head: FAST MAPPING SKILLS IN THE DEVELOPING LEXICON. Fast Mapping Skills in the Developing Lexicon. Lisa Gershkoff-Stowe. Indiana University

Meaning and Motor Action

Visual CP Representation of Knowledge

Strategy Abandonment Effects in Cued Recall

By Merrill Harmin, Ph.D.

Learning Structural Correspondences Across Different Linguistic Domains with Synchronous Neural Language Models

Preparing for the School Census Autumn 2017 Return preparation guide. English Primary, Nursery and Special Phase Schools Applicable to 7.

Increasing Student Engagement

1 3-5 = Subtraction - a binary operation

Unit 13 Assessment in Language Teaching. Welcome

A Case Study: News Classification Based on Term Frequency

with The Grouchy Ladybug

SAT MATH PREP:

Active Ingredients of Instructional Coaching Results from a qualitative strand embedded in a randomized control trial

TK20 FOR STUDENT TEACHERS CONTENTS

Paraprofessional Evaluation: School Year:

MGT/MGP/MGB 261: Investment Analysis

Build on students informal understanding of sharing and proportionality to develop initial fraction concepts.

A Stochastic Model for the Vocabulary Explosion

PHILOSOPHY & CULTURE Syllabus

TEXT FAMILIARITY, READING TASKS, AND ESP TEST PERFORMANCE: A STUDY ON IRANIAN LEP AND NON-LEP UNIVERSITY STUDENTS

Genevieve L. Hartman, Ph.D.

What s in View for Toddlers? Using a Head Camera to Study Visual Experience

A Case-Based Approach To Imitation Learning in Robotic Agents

The Development of Altruistic Behavior: Helping in Children and Chimpanzees

Strategic Practice: Career Practitioner Case Study

Degeneracy results in canalisation of language structure: A computational model of word learning

I N T E R P R E T H O G A N D E V E L O P HOGAN BUSINESS REASONING INVENTORY. Report for: Martina Mustermann ID: HC Date: May 02, 2017

GROUP COMPOSITION IN THE NAVIGATION SIMULATOR A PILOT STUDY Magnus Boström (Kalmar Maritime Academy, Sweden)

Guidelines for the Master s Thesis Project in Biomedicine BIMM60 (30 hp): planning, writing and presentation.

Assessing speaking skills:. a workshop for teacher development. Ben Knight

The role of word-word co-occurrence in word learning

Initial English Language Training for Controllers and Pilots. Mr. John Kennedy École Nationale de L Aviation Civile (ENAC) Toulouse, France.

Millersville University Degree Works Training User Guide

Transcription:

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report Preschoolers Perspective Taking in Word Learning Do They Blindly Follow Eye Gaze? Erika Nurmsoo and Paul Bloom Yale University ABSTRACT When learning new words, do children use a speaker s eye gaze because it reveals referential intent? We conducted two experiments that addressed this question. In Experiment 1, the experimenter left while two novel objects were placed where the child could see both, but the experimenter would be able to see only one. The experimenter returned, looked directly at the mutually visible object, and said either, There s the [novel word]! or Where s the [novel word]? Two- through 4-year-olds selected the target of the speaker s gaze more often on there trials than on where trials, although only the older children identified the referent correctly at above-chance levels on trials of both types. In Experiment 2, the experimenter placed a novel object where only the child could see it and left while the second object was similarly hidden. When she returned and asked, Where s the [novel word]? 2- through 4-year-olds chose the second object at abovechance levels. Preschoolers do not blindly follow gaze, but consider the linguistic and pragmatic context when learning a new word. When learning an object name, young children often assume that the name refers to the object the speaker is looking at, even if the children themselves are looking at a different object when the word is used (e.g., Baldwin, 1991). Eye gaze cannot be a necessary cue for word learning; blind children can learn object names, after all, and children can infer that a word refers to an object even if the speaker is not looking at it (e.g., through mutual exclusivity Markman & Wachtel, 1988; when learning names for absent referents Akhtar & Tomasello, 1996). Even so, eye gaze does seem to be sufficient for word learning. Address correspondence to Erika Nurmsoo, University of Bristol, Department of Experimental Psychology, 12a Priory Rd., Bristol, BS8 1TU, Great Britain, e-mail: nurmsoo@aya.yale.edu. What does this indicate about how children learn the meanings of words? Such results are often taken as demonstrating the importance of social cognition in word learning, under the assumption that children use a speaker s eye gaze as a cue to referential intent (e.g., Bloom, 2000). According to another account, however, children attend to a speaker s line of regard because of simpler, possibly unlearned, orienting responses (e.g., Moore & Corkum, 1994; but see Woodward, 2003); once they have done so, they come to associate the object they are attending to and the word they are hearing (Smith, Jones, & Landau, 1996; Plunkett, 1997). The precise details of such claims differ, but they share the view that, regardless of what role social cognition plays in other aspects of word learning, this primary and early-emerging sensitivity to eye gaze is unmediated by any inferences about the speaker s referential intent. The two accounts can be distinguished by what they predict about how mandatory this sensitivity to eye gaze is. If children use gaze cues out of an understanding that eye gaze reflects the speaker s meaning, gaze information should be exploited when it reveals the speaker s intent, but ignored when it is irrelevant or uninformative. We tested this hypothesis in two studies. EXPERIMENT 1: VISUAL PERSPECTIVE In Experiment 1, the child and the speaker explored two novel unnamed objects together. Then, in the speaker s absence, the objects were placed such that the speaker would be able to see only one of them, although the child could see both. The speaker returned, looked at the mutually visible object, and said, There s the [novel word]! or Where s the [novel word]? If children mandatorily follow eye gaze to a target object when learning a new word, they would be expected to map the word to the mutually visible object in both conditions. Alternatively, if children are sensitive to the speaker s visual perspective and use gaze information in word learning only when it is informative, they would be expected to disregard eye-gaze cues on where Volume 19 Number 3 Copyright r 2008 Association for Psychological Science 211

Perspective and Word Learning trials and instead map the word to the object hidden from the speaker. Method Participants Thirty-two older children (M 5 49 months, range 5 46 54 months; 16 boys and 16 girls) and 32 younger children (M 5 31 months, range 5 25 36 months; 13 boys and 19 girls) were tested in a university lab or at their day-care center. Materials We used an opaque cardboard screen (32 cm 22 cm) containing two compartments, one with and the other without a window. This screen was placed between the child and the speaker so that the child could see into both compartments, but the speaker could see only through the window into one compartment. We used four pairs of novel objects, and the novel labels spoodle, nurmy, flurg, and gorp. The target object, its location relative to the window, and the left/right position of the window in the screen were all counterbalanced. Procedure In the familiarization phase, each participant was shown what the screen looked like from both sides. Two familiar toys were placed on the child s side of the screen, one in each compartment. The child was asked to identify which toy the speaker could see, and which she could not see. The screen was then rotated so that the child had the speaker s perspective and could see only the object in the window. In the experimental phase, there were two where trials and two there trials, identical except for the test question. On each trial, the child explored a pair of novel objects with two adults (the speaker and the assistant). The speaker left the room or turned her back while the assistant placed each object in its compartment. From the child s perspective, both objects were visible, one in the compartment with the window and the other in the compartment without the window. When the speaker returned (or turned around), only the object in the window was visible from her perspective (see Fig. 1). She fixed her gaze on the object in the window and asked the test question: On there trials, the speaker said, Oh! There s the [novel label]! There it is! On where trials, she said, Oh! Where s the [novel label]? Where is it? In both conditions, the speaker then looked up at the child, held out her hand, and asked, Can I have the [novel label]? Results and Discussion Older children correctly selected the mutually visible object on an average of 1.53 of the 2 there trials, whereas younger children averaged 1.38 correct there trials. Both levels of performance Fig. 1. The event shown during Experiment 1, with sample novel objects. On all trials, the experimenter fixed her gaze on the object in the window. On there trials, the experimenter said, There s the [spoodle/ nurmy/flurg/gorp]! There it is! On where trials, she asked, Where s the [spoodle/nurmy/flurg/gorp]? Where is it? were above chance, ts(31) 5 4.48 and 3.00, p rep s 5.996 and.97, respectively. On there trials, the speaker s eye gaze was directed at the correct target, and both older and younger children correctly used this cue to identify the referent. Older children correctly selected the hidden object on an average of 1.41 of the 2 where trials, whereas younger children averaged 1.00 correct where trials. Only older children s performance was consistently above chance level, t(31) 5 3.23, p rep 5.97. On where trials, the speaker s gaze information was uninformative, as it was directed at an incorrect object. Older children were able to disregard these gaze cues and identify the correct referent, although the younger children were not. To determine whether children in each age group selected the visible object more often on there trials (when it was correct) than on where trials (when it was incorrect), we gave each participant a score of 1 if he or she selected the visible object more often on there trials and a score of 1 if he or she selected the visible object more often on where trials. Sign tests revealed that participants in both age groups showed the correct pattern of responses; both the older and the younger children selected the visible object more often on there trials than on where trials, ps <.05. Children at both ages appear to have used the adult s eye-gaze cues flexibly, relying on gaze direction to map the novel word only when gaze direction was informative. The younger children, although uncertain of the correct answer in the where condition, nevertheless did not err by selecting the target of the speaker s eye gaze when it was uninformative. The older children appear to have determined the speaker s referential intent in the where condition. 212 Volume 19 Number 3

Erika Nurmsoo and Paul Bloom An alternative analysis, however, is that the older children employed the strategy of selecting any object that was hidden from the speaker when asked a where question. That is, rather than making an inference about referential intent, they might have simply responded to this sort of question by looking for a hidden object. We explored this alternative in Experiment 2. Method EXPERIMENT 2: KNOWLEDGE PERSPECTIVE Participants Eighteen older children (M 5 46 months, range 5 41 56 months; 11 boys and 7 girls) and 18 younger children (M 5 31 months, range 5 29 33 months; 9 boys and 9 girls) were tested in a university lab or their day-care center. No child participated in both experiments. Materials An opaque cardboard screen (33 cm 25 cm) was used. Objects placed behind the screen were visible to the child, but not to the speaker seated on the opposite side (see Fig. 2). We used three pairs of novel objects, and the novel labels fendle, nurmy, and toma. Each object served as target and as distractor an equal number of times. Procedure As in Experiment 1, in the familiarization phase of Experiment 2, participants were introduced to the screen from the speaker s and the child s perspective. There were two knowledge trials and one control trial, identical except for the timing of the placement of the objects. On all trials, the child first explored a pair of novel objects with the speaker and the assistant. On knowledge trials, the speaker then placed one of the objects on the child s side of the screen, where the child could see it but the speaker could not. The other object remained in plain view as the speaker left or turned her back, at which point the assistant placed it behind the screen. Both objects were therefore hidden to the speaker, but she knew the location of one of them because she had placed it behind the screen herself. The speaker returned (or turned around), feigned surprise, and asked, Where is the [novel label]? Where is it? She held out her hand and asked, Can I have the [novel label]? The control trial was identical, except that both objects were placed behind the screen in the speaker s absence, and she could be seen as ignorant about the location of both. The control trial appeared equally often as the first, last, or middle trial. Only one control trial was used because pilot testing revealed that children found the trial frustrating, as there was no clear correct answer. Results and Discussion On knowledge trials, the speaker was ignorant of the location of the object that was hidden second, in her absence. Children received a point each time they correctly selected this object, for a possible maximum score of 2. Children occasionally selected both objects (2 older children on a total of three trials, 1 younger child on one trial). This response was coded as incorrect, as the object hidden second was not chosen as the sole referent of the target word. The average scores were 1.72 for older children and 1.61 for younger children. Children in both age groups answered correctly significantly more often than predicted by chance, ts(17) 5 5.33 and 5.17, respectively, p rep s 5.996. On the control trial, the speaker was ignorant of the location of both objects. On this trial, many children responded by selecting both objects (8 older and 5 younger children). Of the children who selected only one object, 4 of 10 older children and 8 of 13 younger children chose the object hidden second, a level of performance no different from chance, w 2 (1, N 5 14) 5 0.4, p >.53, and w 2 (1, N 5 13) 5 0.7, p >.40, respectively. GENERAL DISCUSSION Fig. 2. The event shown during Experiment 2, with sample novel objects. On all trials, the experimenter asked, Where s the [fendle/nurmy/ toma]? Where is it? In our experiments, children ages 2 to 4 were able to take the perspective of a speaker (Experiments 1 and 2), and 3- to 4-yearolds were able to override eye-gaze cues when they were not relevant (Experiment 1). These findings might seem surprising, given that older children and adults have difficulty with similar tasks. For example, Epley, Morewedge, and Keysar (2004) explored perspective taking by having a speaker ask participants to move objects from one location to another in an upright array of boxes. They found that 6-year-olds and even adults often failed to take into account the speaker s visual perspective, responding as if the speaker could see an object that was visible Volume 19 Number 3 213

Perspective and Word Learning only to the participant (but see Nadig & Sedivy, 2002). This task differs from our own in that it involves a much more complex array of objects and requires participants to determine the referents of existing words, not to make sense of new ones. Our study shows that 4-year-olds, and to some extent 2-year-olds, are able to consider the speaker s perspective in a referential task when given a simpler context. Our findings are consistent with those of Moll and Tomasello (2006), who found that when an adult asked for help searching for an object, 2-year-olds did not offer the object in plain view, but instead fetched the object visible to the child but hidden from the adult. In this task, however, the adult s eye gaze alternated between the visible object and the location of the hidden object, whereas in the parallel condition in our Experiment 1, the adult s eye gaze was directed at the mutually visible nontarget object, making the mapping problem more difficult. Although 2-year-olds were not able to correctly select the hidden object on these trials, as a group they did not mistakenly map the novel word to the target of the adult s gaze. These findings provide support for a pragmatic approach to word learning, in which children do not simply follow surface cues to the speaker s referential intent, but instead seek to understand the situation as a whole. Studies consistent with this perspective have shown that somewhat older children do not learn a new label despite clear referential cues toward a novel object when the reliability of the speaker is called into question. For example, Koenig and Harris (2005) showed that 4-year-olds, and in some cases 3-year-olds, preferred to learn a novel label from a previously reliable speaker rather than from one who claimed ignorance of the names of familiar objects or who labeled them inaccurately (see also Birch, Vauthier, & Bloom, in press; Jaswal & Neely, 2006; Nurmsoo & Robinson, in press). And Sabbagh and Baldwin (2001) found that when a speaker explicitly claimed ignorance about the correct referent of a novel word (e.g., I don t know what a blicket is. Maybe it s this one. ), 3- and 4-year-olds did not learn the word. The present set of studies adds to this body of research, by showing that even when faced with a speaker who shows every sign of competent naming, and no uncertainty about the meaning of a novel label, children do not rely solely on eye-gaze cues to determine the referent of the new word, but use additional pragmatic or linguistic information to infer the correct target. Experiment 2 demonstrated that when two objects were hidden from the speaker, who could be seen as ignorant about the location of only one of them, 2- to 4-year-olds correctly identified the speaker s intended referent when she asked, Where is the [novel word]? This finding converges with research suggesting that children use other cues to a speaker s referential intent when eye-gaze information is not sufficient. For example, when preschoolers are told, Let s find the toma, they will map toma to the object that satisfies the searcher even when multiple novel objects are examined (and rejected; Tomasello & Barton, 1994) or when the unseen novel object is hidden in a locked toy barn (Akhtar & Tomasello, 1996). Somewhat older children are sensitive to a speaker s false beliefs when interpreting the meaning of a novel word used to refer to the contents of a container, mapping the word to the object that the speaker falsely believes is hidden within (Carpenter, Call, & Tomasello, 2002; Happe & Loth, 2002). There is considerable evidence, then, that children will track other cues to a speaker s referential intent when there are no eyegaze cues that can be used to identify the correct target. We have shown here that preschoolers are sensitive to a speaker s referential intent even when eye-gaze cues are present and directed at an incorrect target. Preschoolers do not blindly follow the direction of gaze. REFERENCES Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M. (1996). Two-year-olds learn words for absent objects and actions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 14, 79 93. Baldwin, D.A. (1991). Infants contribution to the achievement of joint reference. Child Development, 62, 875 890. Birch, S.A.J., Vauthier, S., & Bloom, P. (in press). Three- and four-yearolds spontaneously use others past performance to guide their learning. Cognition. Bloom, P. (2000). How children learn the meanings of words. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2002). A new false belief test for 36-month-olds. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20, 393 420. Epley, N., Morewedge, C.K., & Keysar, B. (2004). Perspective taking in children and adults: Equivalent egocentrism but differential correction. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 760 768. Happe, F., & Loth, E. (2002). Theory of mind and tracking speakers intentions. Mind & Language, 17, 24 36. Jaswal, V.K., & Neely, L.A. (2006). Adults don t always know best: Preschoolers use past reliability over age when learning new words. Psychological Science, 17, 757 758. Koenig, M.A., & Harris, P.L. (2005). Preschoolers mistrust ignorant and inaccurate speakers. Child Development, 76, 1261 1277. Markman, E.M., & Wachtel, G.F. (1988). Children s use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 121 157. Moll, H., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Level I perspective-taking at 24 months of age. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24, 603 613. Moore, C., & Corkum, V. (1994). Social understanding at the end of the first year of life. Developmental Review, 14, 349 372. Nadig, A.S., & Sedivy, J.C. (2002). Evidence of perspective-taking constraints in children s on-line reference resolution. Psychological Science, 13, 329 336. Nurmsoo, E., & Robinson, E.J. (in press). Identifying unreliable informants: Do children excuse past inaccuracy? Developmental Science. Plunkett, K. (1997). Theories of early language acquisition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1, 146 153. Sabbagh, M.A., & Baldwin, D.A. (2001). Learning words from knowledgeable versus ignorant speakers: Links between preschoolers 214 Volume 19 Number 3

Erika Nurmsoo and Paul Bloom theory of mind and semantic development. Child Development, 72, 1054 1070. Smith, L.B., Jones, S.S., & Landau, B. (1996). Naming in young children: A dumb attentional mechanism? Cognition, 60, 143 171. Tomasello, M., & Barton, M. (1994). Learning words in non-ostensive contexts. Developmental Psychology, 30, 639 650. Woodward, A.L. (2003). Infants developing understanding of the link between looker and object. Developmental Science, 6, 297 311. (RECEIVED 5/30/07; REVISION ACCEPTED 8/27/07) Volume 19 Number 3 215