A Gradient Harmonic Grammar Account of Lexically-Conditioned Phonetic Variation

Similar documents
SOUND STRUCTURE REPRESENTATION, REPAIR AND WELL-FORMEDNESS: GRAMMAR IN SPOKEN LANGUAGE PRODUCTION. Adam B. Buchwald

Precedence Constraints and Opacity

Parallel Evaluation in Stratal OT * Adam Baker University of Arizona

Canadian raising with language-specific weighted constraints Joe Pater, University of Massachusetts Amherst

Rachel E. Baker, Ann R. Bradlow. Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA

Phonological Processing for Urdu Text to Speech System

Pobrane z czasopisma New Horizons in English Studies Data: 18/11/ :52:20. New Horizons in English Studies 1/2016

Markedness and Complex Stops: Evidence from Simplification Processes 1. Nick Danis Rutgers University

Lexical phonology. Marc van Oostendorp. December 6, Until now, we have presented phonological theory as if it is a monolithic

An argument from speech pathology

Manner assimilation in Uyghur

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

Phonological encoding in speech production

Speech Recognition at ICSI: Broadcast News and beyond

The Perception of Nasalized Vowels in American English: An Investigation of On-line Use of Vowel Nasalization in Lexical Access

Underlying Representations

have to be modeled) or isolated words. Output of the system is a grapheme-tophoneme conversion system which takes as its input the spelling of words,

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

**Note: this is slightly different from the original (mainly in format). I would be happy to send you a hard copy.**

Acoustic correlates of stress and their use in diagnosing syllable fusion in Tongan. James White & Marc Garellek UCLA

Dyslexia/dyslexic, 3, 9, 24, 97, 187, 189, 206, 217, , , 367, , , 397,

LING 329 : MORPHOLOGY

Consonant-Vowel Unity in Element Theory*

The phonological grammar is probabilistic: New evidence pitting abstract representation against analogy

On the nature of voicing assimilation(s)

Som and Optimality Theory

Lecture 1: Machine Learning Basics

The Odd-Parity Parsing Problem 1 Brett Hyde Washington University May 2008

The analysis starts with the phonetic vowel and consonant charts based on the dataset:

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

SARDNET: A Self-Organizing Feature Map for Sequences

Universal contrastive analysis as a learning principle in CAPT

DEVELOPMENT OF LINGUAL MOTOR CONTROL IN CHILDREN AND ADOLESCENTS

The Journey to Vowelerria VOWEL ERRORS: THE LOST WORLD OF SPEECH INTERVENTION. Preparation: Education. Preparation: Education. Preparation: Education

5. Margi (Chadic, Nigeria): H, L, R (Williams 1973, Hoffmann 1963)

Infants learn phonotactic regularities from brief auditory experience

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Voiced-voiceless distinction in alaryngeal speech - acoustic and articula

Rhythm-typology revisited.

1. REFLEXES: Ask questions about coughing, swallowing, of water as fast as possible (note! Not suitable for all

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Language Acquisition by Identical vs. Fraternal SLI Twins * Karin Stromswold & Jay I. Rifkin

SEGMENTAL FEATURES IN SPONTANEOUS AND READ-ALOUD FINNISH

Word Stress and Intonation: Introduction

Program in Linguistics. Academic Year Assessment Report

Evolution of Symbolisation in Chimpanzees and Neural Nets

The presence of interpretable but ungrammatical sentences corresponds to mismatches between interpretive and productive parsing.

A Cross-language Corpus for Studying the Phonetics and Phonology of Prominence

I propose an analysis of thorny patterns of reduplication in the unrelated languages Saisiyat

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Sound symbolism in deictic words

2,1 .,,, , %, ,,,,,,. . %., Butterworth,)?.(1989; Levelt, 1989; Levelt et al., 1991; Levelt, Roelofs & Meyer, 1999

Ph.D. Linguistics, University of Arizona. Dissertation: Confluence in phonology: evidence from Micronesian reduplication Director: Diana Archangeli

A Neural Network GUI Tested on Text-To-Phoneme Mapping

Linguistics 220 Phonology: distributions and the concept of the phoneme. John Alderete, Simon Fraser University

On the Formation of Phoneme Categories in DNN Acoustic Models

An Evaluation of the Interactive-Activation Model Using Masked Partial-Word Priming. Jason R. Perry. University of Western Ontario. Stephen J.

To appear in The TESOL encyclopedia of ELT (Wiley-Blackwell) 1 RECASTING. Kazuya Saito. Birkbeck, University of London

Approaches to control phenomena handout Obligatory control and morphological case: Icelandic and Basque

Partial Class Behavior and Nasal Place Assimilation*

Correspondence between the DRDP (2015) and the California Preschool Learning Foundations. Foundations (PLF) in Language and Literacy

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

Demonstration of problems of lexical stress on the pronunciation Turkish English teachers and teacher trainees by computer

Radical CV Phonology: the locational gesture *

Phonological Encoding in Sentence Production

A Bayesian Model of Stress Assignment in Reading

Perceived speech rate: the effects of. articulation rate and speaking style in spontaneous speech. Jacques Koreman. Saarland University

CSC200: Lecture 4. Allan Borodin

UCLA Issues in Applied Linguistics

phone hidden time phone

Journal of Phonetics

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. VCV-sequencies in a preliminary text-to-speech system for female speech

Python Machine Learning

Florida Reading Endorsement Alignment Matrix Competency 1

Listener-oriented phonology

An Interactive Intelligent Language Tutor Over The Internet

English Language and Applied Linguistics. Module Descriptions 2017/18

First Grade Curriculum Highlights: In alignment with the Common Core Standards

Speech Recognition using Acoustic Landmarks and Binary Phonetic Feature Classifiers

Module 12. Machine Learning. Version 2 CSE IIT, Kharagpur

1 st Quarter (September, October, November) August/September Strand Topic Standard Notes Reading for Literature

5/26/12. Adult L3 learners who are re- learning their L1: heritage speakers A growing trend in American colleges

To appear in the Proceedings of the 35th Meetings of the Chicago Linguistics Society. Post-vocalic spirantization: Typology and phonetic motivations

Processing Lexically Embedded Spoken Words

Cambridgeshire Community Services NHS Trust: delivering excellence in children and young people s health services

Consonants: articulation and transcription

Mimetic gemination in Japanese: A challenge for Evolutionary Phonology*

UDL AND LANGUAGE ARTS LESSON OVERVIEW

Linguistics. Undergraduate. Departmental Honors. Graduate. Faculty. Linguistics 1

SOFTWARE EVALUATION TOOL

Frequency and pragmatically unmarked word order *

THE INFLUENCE OF TASK DEMANDS ON FAMILIARITY EFFECTS IN VISUAL WORD RECOGNITION: A COHORT MODEL PERSPECTIVE DISSERTATION

AGENDA LEARNING THEORIES LEARNING THEORIES. Advanced Learning Theories 2/22/2016

AN INTRODUCTION (2 ND ED.) (LONDON, BLOOMSBURY ACADEMIC PP. VI, 282)

Word Segmentation of Off-line Handwritten Documents

South Carolina English Language Arts

Lecture 2: Quantifiers and Approximation

Revisiting the role of prosody in early language acquisition. Megha Sundara UCLA Phonetics Lab

The influence of orthographic transparency on word recognition. by dyslexic and normal readers

Problems of the Arabic OCR: New Attitudes

Transcription:

A Gradient Harmonic Grammar Account of Lexically-Conditioned Phonetic Variation Matt Goldrick Department of Linguistics Northwestern University

Lexically-Conditioned Grammatical Patterns Categorical: Japanese phonotactics (Ito & Mester, 1999) Nasal-voiceless obstruent clusters (*mp) banned only in Yamato stratum. Variable: Finnish (Anttila, 2002) Variability: vowel mutation (a->o) vs. deletion. Selection of mutation vs. deletion vs. variation is morpheme-specific. Defining feature: Non-gradient Input: Conditioned by lexical categories. Output: Alternations among categorically distinct variants.

Lexically-Conditioned Phonetic Variation Lexical frequency/predictability: High frequency/more predictable forms are reduced relative to low frequency/less predictable forms. Shorter durations; more centralized vowels; elision/lenition (and->n; of->uh). (see Aylett & Turk, 2006, for a recent review) Neighborhood density: Words phonologically similar to many lexical items are hyperarticulated relative to words similar to few lexical items. Vowels in high vs. low density words are less centralized in F1/F2 space (Munson, in press; Munson & Solomon, 2004; Wright, 2004) Similar effects for consonants.

Lexically-Conditioned Phonetic Variation Voiceless stops in words that have a minimal pair neighbor (e.g., cod-god) show enhanced VOTs relative to stops in phonetically matched words that do not (e.g., cop-*gop). (Baese & Goldrick, 2007) 1.2 VOT Ratio (Minimal Pair/No Minimal Pair) 1.15 1.1 1.05 1 0.95 0.9 * * * p t k Initial Consonant

Lexical and Phonetic Gradience Current grammatical accounts of lexical effects on sound patterns capture categorical phenomena. Understanding lexically-conditioned phonetic variation requires incorporating gradience. Conditioning factors (frequency, density) are gradient. Phonetic effects (vowel space dispersion; VOT expansion) are gradient.

Gradient Harmonic Grammar Account of Lexically-Conditioned Phonetic Variation Gradience of phonetics. Output candidates allow for gradient activation. (following Flemming, 2001; Gafos, 2002, and other work in Articulatory Phonology) Gradience of conditioning lexical factors. Variation in activation of representations in the lexicon. Focus: Neighborhood density. Lexically-conditioned phonetic variation Constraint interaction in Gradient Harmonic Grammar is sensitive to gradient variation in activation of both input and output representations. (see Gafos & Benus, 2006, for an alternative approach within a dynamical systems framework)

Gradient Lexical Factor: Neighborhood Feedback activates lexical representation of neighbor. Non-target phonological representations become active. Contrasting, highly similar segments compete for same position in string. (Meyer & Gordon, 1985; Yaniv et al., 1990) k cod g a god d To resolve competition, target segment must grow in activation so as to inhibit competitor. Enhanced activation: Stronger input to Harmonic Grammar Word without minimal pair neighbor: Weaker competition, so no need to enhance activation of input.

Harmonic Grammars Connectionist precursor to Optimality Theory (Legendre et al. 1990; Smolensky & Legendre, 2006; see also Pater et al., 2007) Output of grammar is candidate with highest harmony. Harmony = sum of weighted constraint violations. /g/ *[+voi]: 2 ID(voi): 1 HARMONY --> [k] 1 1 [g] 2 2

Graphical Depiction of Harmony Differences --> /g/ *[+voi]: 2 ID(voi): 1 HARMONY --> [k] 1 1 [g] 2 2

Gradient Harmonic Grammars Representations: Gradient patterns of activation in range {0,1} over sets of symbolic representational units. Default activation levels: 0.1 / 0.9. [+voi]: 0.9 Neutral articulation level for voiced [+voi]: 0.99 Hyperarticulated voiced As in connectionist networks (Smolensky, 2006) Harmony reflects gradient activation levels. *[+voi]: Weight: 1 Output: [+voi]:0.9 Harmony 1 * 0.90 0.90 Output: [+voi]:0.99 Harmony 1 * 0.99 0.99 Critical factor for lexically conditioned phonetic variation: Gradience in input influences calculation of Harmony.

Calculating Harmony: Faithfulness Constraint: ExpressVoice: Weight = 4.1 Penalizes outputs that do not maximally express input. Following example above (cop/cod) assume voiceless input. Harmony contribution: Weight * Constraint strength (1 activation of [ voi]) * Prefers hyperarticulated to normal articulation level Activation of /k/ Preference is stronger if input is more highly activated (e.g., cod vs. cop)

Calculating Harmony: Markedness Constraint: MinimizeEffort: Weight = 1 Penalizes outputs with high levels of activation. Harmony contribution: Weight * Constraint strength (activation of [ voi]) * Prefers normal articulation level to hyperarticulated 0.9 General bias value for markedness constraints (maintain symmetry with Faithfulness constraints)

Calculating Harmony: Unit Harmony Assumption: Neutral activation level is 0.9 / 0.1 NOT 1.0 / 0.0 To enforce the neutral activation level: Unit harmony term Penalizes outputs with extreme values. Penalizes both 1.0 and 0.0 See Appendix D for details.

Weakly Activated Input->Normal Articulation --> Although Faithfulness prefers hyperarticulated candidate, the combined influence of Markedness and Unit Harmony prevent it from being the most harmonic form. (see Appendix B for Tableaux)

Strongly Activated Input->Hyperarticulation --> Because harmony is weighted by activation of input, increasing the input s strength increases the impact of Faithfulness. Hyperarticulated form is now more harmonic.

Lexically Conditioned Phonetic Variation Because Harmony is sensitive to gradient activation levels, changes to the activation of input representations alters the harmony of output forms. Neighbors increase activation of phonological representations in the lexicon. Harmony contribution of Faithfulness constraints increases. Can cause a hyperarticulated output representation to be more harmonic than one with normal articulation levels.

Implications for Theories of Phonetic Variation Above: Neighborhood density induces hyperarticulation Expanded vowel space, enhanced VOTs. Neighborhood density also enhances coarticulation E.g., greater nasalization on /ae/ in ban Similar results for V-V coarticulation (Scarborough, 2003, 2004) Not clearly predicted by simple hyperarticulation theory In anticipatory nasalization, why are features of consonant hyperarticulated, rather than hyperarticulating properties of the target vowel /ae/? In Gradient Harmonic Grammar, this can be understood as a consequence of constraint ranking.

Case Study: Anticipatory Nasalization Trigger of nasalization: Consonant features Phonetic realization of consonant reflects underlying nasality. i.e., ExpressNasal > MinimizeEffort Following above, causes hyperarticulation for words in dense neighborhoods.

Effect on Nasalization Target: Vowel Features Harmony contribution of faithfulness constraint (ExpressOral) increases due to enhanced activation of vowel input (see above). Harmony contribution of markedness constraint: *V[oral]N: Weight * [1 activation of nasal feature on vowel] * Activation of nasal feature on consonant Harmony contribution of markedness also increases due to hyperarticulation of nasal features on consonant. Both are strengthened; which dominates? Because coarticulation is present: *V[oral]N > ExpressOral Same proportional strengthening of Markedness and Faithfulness will provide a greater numerical benefit to Markedness.

Weakly Activated Input->Normal Coarticulation --> Although Markedness prefers hyper coarticulation candidate, the combined influence of Faithfulness and Unit Harmony prevent it from being the most harmonic form. (see Appendix C for Tableaux)

Strongly Activated Input->Hyper Coarticulation --> Increase in input increases impact of faithfulness, but also causes hyperarticulation of nasal providing an even bigger numerical benefit to markedness.

Gradient Harmonic Grammar Incorporating gradience into grammar Gradience in representations in both input (modulation by lexical factors) and output (phonetic variation). Gradience at both levels influences constraint interaction. Provides an account of Lexically-Conditioned Phonetic Variation Influence of lexical factors on expression of underlying features Influence of lexical factors on coarticulation Gradient representations may provide a means to capture opaque process interactions (see Appendix A for discussion).

Thanks Sound Lab and Phonatics Discussion Group at Northwestern NIH DC00797 for support Talk slides, papers, posters http://ling.northwestern.edu/~goldrick

Appendix A: Implications for Theories of Opacity Variation in activation as a model of phonological opacity. Counterbleeding Since harmony is determined by multiplying activations, partially activated structures can influence harmony. Prediction: Phonetics should reflect partially activated structures. See Benus & Gafos (2007): Transparent vowels in Hungarian Counterfeeding A weakly activated structure may have less of an influence on harmony than a strongly activated structure. Prediction: Phonetics should reflect weakening. See Gouskova & Hall (in press): Epenthetic vowels in Lebanese Arabic.

Appendix B: Hyperarticulation Tableaux Tableau B1. Input without minimal pair neighbor (e.g., cop) Weight 4.1 1 Input with no minimal pair /k/:0.9 Express Voice Minimize Effort Unit Harmony Total Harmony Non-voiceless output 3.321 0.090 1.104 4.515 [ voice]:0.1 --> 0.369 0.810 1.104 2.283 Voiceless output [ voice]:0.9 Hyperarticulated voiceless output 0.037 0.891 1.373 2.301 [ voice]:0.99

Appendix B: Hyperarticulation Tableaux Tableau B2. Input with minimal pair neighbor (e.g., cod) Weight 4.1 1 Input with minimal pair /k/:0.99 Express Voice Minimize Effort Unit Harmony Total Harmony Non-voiceless output 3.653 0.090 1.373 5.116 [ voice]:0.1 Voiceless output 0.405 0.810 1.373 2.588 [ voice]:0.9 --> Hyperarticulated voiceless output 0.040 0.891 1.642 2.573 [ voice]:0.99

Appendix C: Hyper Coarticulation Tableaux Tableau C1. Input in sparse neighborhood (e.g., strand) Weight 2.7 1 Input in sparse neighborhood /aen/:0.9 *V[oral]N Express Oral Unit Harmony Total Harmony No coarticulation 2.187 0.090 1.104 3.381 [+nas]:0.1 --> 0.608 0.675 0.867 2.150 Normal coarticulation [+nas]:0.75 Hyper coarticulation 0.243 0.810 1.104 2.157 [ voice]:0.9 Note: Candidates show [nas] of vowel. For all candidates, consonant [nas] = 0.9

Appendix C: Hyper Coarticulation Tableaux Tableau C2. Input in dense neighborhood (e.g., band) Weight 2.7 1 Input in dense neighborhood /aen/:0.99 *V[oral]N Express Oral Unit Harmony Total Harmony No coarticulation 2.406 0.099 1.642 4.147 [+nas]:0.1 Normal coarticulation 0.668 0.742 1.405 2.815 [+nas]:0.75 --> 0.267 0.891 1.642 2.800 Hyper coarticulation [ voice]:0.9 Note: Candidates show [nas] of vowel. For all candidates, consonant [nas] = 0.99

Appendix D: Unit Harmony Function From Movellan & McClelland (1993) For unit i with activation a i = [a i ln(a i )+(1 a i )ln(1 a i ) ln(0.5)] Total unit harmony is sum over all units Including bias unit for markedness constraints (e.g., 0.9 term in MinimizeEffort constraint)

References Anttila, A. (2002) Morphologically conditioned phonological alternations. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 20, 1-42 Aylett, M., & Turk, A. (2006). Language redundancy predicts syllabic duration and the spectral characteristics of vocalic syllable nuclei. Journal the Acoustical Society of America, 119, 3048-3058. Baese, M., & Goldrick, M. (2007). Mechanisms of interaction in speech production. Ms., Department of Linguistics, Northwestern University. Benus, S., & Gafos, A. (2007). Articulatory characteristics of Hungarian transparent vowels. Journal of Phonetics, 35, 271-300. Flemming, E. (2001). Scalar and categorical phenomena in a unified model of phonetics and phonology. Phonology, 18, 7-44. Gafos, A. I. (2002). A grammar of gestural coordination. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 20, 269-337. Gafos, A. I., & Benus, S. (2006). Dynamics of phonological cognition. Cognitive Science, 30, 1-39. Gouskova, M., & Hall, N. (in press). Acoustics of unstressable vowels in Lebanese Arabic. In S. Parker (Ed.), Phonological argumentation: Essays on evidence and motivation. London: Equinox Ito, J., & Mester A. (1999). The structure of the phonological lexicon. In N. Tsujimura (Ed.) The Handbook of Japanese Linguistics, Blackwell Publishers, Malden, MA, and Oxford, U.K. pp. 62-100. Legendre, G., Miyata Y. & Smolensky, P. (1990). Harmonic Grammar A formal multi-level connectionist theory of linguistic well-formedness: Theoretical foundations. In Proceedings of the Twelfth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, pp. 388 395. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

References Meyer, D. E., & Gordon, P. C. (1985). Speech production: Motor programming of phonetic features. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 3-26. Movellan, J. R., & McClelland, J. L. (1993). Learning continuous probability distributions with symmetric diffusion networks. Cognitive Science, 17, 463-496. Munson, B. (in press). Lexical access, lexical representation, and vowel articulation. In J. S. Cole & J. I. Hualde (Eds.), Change in phonology: Papers in Laboratory Phonology 9. New York: Mouton de Gruyter. Munson, B., & Solomon, P. N. (2004). The effect of phonological density on vowel articulation. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 47, 1048-1058. Pater, J., Bhatt, R., & Potts, C. (2007). Linguistic optimization. Ms., Linguistics Department, University of Massachusetts, Amherst. ROA-924. Scarborough, R. A. (2003). Lexical confusability and degree of coarticulation. Proceedings of the 29th annual meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society. Scarborough, R. A. (2004). Coarticulation and the structure of the lexicon. Doctoral dissertation. UCLA, Los Angeles. Smolensky, P. (2006). Optimization in neural networks: Harmony maximization. In P. Smolensky & G. Legendre (2006) Vol. 1, pp. 345 392. Smolensky, P., & Legendre, G. (2006). The harmonic mind: From neural computation to optimality-theoretic grammar (vol. 1: Cognitive architecture; vol. 2: Linguistic and philosophical implications). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Wright, R. A. (2004). Factors of lexical competition in vowel articulation. In J. J. Local, R. Ogden & R. Temple (Eds.), Phonetic interpretation: Papers in laboratory phonology 6, pp. 26-50. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Yaniv, I., Meyer, D., E., Gordon, P. C., Huff, C. A., & Sevald, C. A. (1990). Vowel similarity, connectionist models, and syllable structure in motor programming of speech. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 1-26.