RUNNING HEAD: TRAINING THE APPROXIMATE NUMBER SYSTEM. Title Children s expectations about training the approximate number system

Similar documents
Lecture 2: Quantifiers and Approximation

9.85 Cognition in Infancy and Early Childhood. Lecture 7: Number

+32 (0)

Evidence for distinct magnitude systems for symbolic and non-symbolic number

Unraveling symbolic number processing and the implications for its association with mathematics. Delphine Sasanguie

How to Judge the Quality of an Objective Classroom Test

FROM QUASI-VARIABLE THINKING TO ALGEBRAIC THINKING: A STUDY WITH GRADE 4 STUDENTS 1

A Study of Metacognitive Awareness of Non-English Majors in L2 Listening

Early Warning System Implementation Guide

Maximizing Learning Through Course Alignment and Experience with Different Types of Knowledge

Evidence for Reliability, Validity and Learning Effectiveness

Reflective Teaching KATE WRIGHT ASSOCIATE PROFESSOR, SCHOOL OF LIFE SCIENCES, COLLEGE OF SCIENCE

School Competition and Efficiency with Publicly Funded Catholic Schools David Card, Martin D. Dooley, and A. Abigail Payne

AGS THE GREAT REVIEW GAME FOR PRE-ALGEBRA (CD) CORRELATED TO CALIFORNIA CONTENT STANDARDS

Strategies for Solving Fraction Tasks and Their Link to Algebraic Thinking

Backwards Numbers: A Study of Place Value. Catherine Perez

Summary / Response. Karl Smith, Accelerations Educational Software. Page 1 of 8

Evaluation of Teach For America:

STA 225: Introductory Statistics (CT)

THE USE OF TINTED LENSES AND COLORED OVERLAYS FOR THE TREATMENT OF DYSLEXIA AND OTHER RELATED READING AND LEARNING DISORDERS

Capturing and Organizing Prior Student Learning with the OCW Backpack

school students to improve communication skills

MERGA 20 - Aotearoa

COMPUTER-ASSISTED INDEPENDENT STUDY IN MULTIVARIATE CALCULUS

Developing a concrete-pictorial-abstract model for negative number arithmetic

Grade 6: Correlated to AGS Basic Math Skills

SCHEMA ACTIVATION IN MEMORY FOR PROSE 1. Michael A. R. Townsend State University of New York at Albany

Learning By Asking: How Children Ask Questions To Achieve Efficient Search

Grade 2: Using a Number Line to Order and Compare Numbers Place Value Horizontal Content Strand

Essentials of Ability Testing. Joni Lakin Assistant Professor Educational Foundations, Leadership, and Technology

Extending Place Value with Whole Numbers to 1,000,000

OCR for Arabic using SIFT Descriptors With Online Failure Prediction

Monitoring Metacognitive abilities in children: A comparison of children between the ages of 5 to 7 years and 8 to 11 years

How Does Physical Space Influence the Novices' and Experts' Algebraic Reasoning?

12- A whirlwind tour of statistics

Peer Influence on Academic Achievement: Mean, Variance, and Network Effects under School Choice

Dyslexia and Dyscalculia Screeners Digital. Guidance and Information for Teachers

King-Devick Reading Acceleration Program

American Journal of Business Education October 2009 Volume 2, Number 7

Curriculum Design Project with Virtual Manipulatives. Gwenanne Salkind. George Mason University EDCI 856. Dr. Patricia Moyer-Packenham

Exact Equality and Successor Function : Two Keys Concepts on the Path towards Understanding Exact Numbers

OVERVIEW OF CURRICULUM-BASED MEASUREMENT AS A GENERAL OUTCOME MEASURE

PROGRESS MONITORING FOR STUDENTS WITH DISABILITIES Participant Materials

Guru: A Computer Tutor that Models Expert Human Tutors

Accuracy and Speed Feedback: Global and Local Effects on Strategy Use

Greek Teachers Attitudes toward the Inclusion of Students with Special Educational Needs

MULTIDISCIPLINARY TEAM COMMUNICATION THROUGH VISUAL REPRESENTATIONS

The New Theory of Disuse Predicts Retrieval Enhanced Suggestibility (RES)

Limitations to Teaching Children = 4: Typical Arithmetic Problems Can Hinder Learning of Mathematical Equivalence. Nicole M.

Conceptual and Procedural Knowledge of a Mathematics Problem: Their Measurement and Their Causal Interrelations

The Effect of Discourse Markers on the Speaking Production of EFL Students. Iman Moradimanesh

Running head: DELAY AND PROSPECTIVE MEMORY 1

Learning Mathematics with Technology: The Influence of Virtual Manipulatives on Different Achievement Groups

NANCY L. STOKEY. Visiting Professor of Economics, Department of Economics, University of Chicago,

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

Sheila M. Smith is Assistant Professor, Department of Business Information Technology, College of Business, Ball State University, Muncie, Indiana.

How People Learn Physics

What is related to student retention in STEM for STEM majors? Abstract:

Algebra 2- Semester 2 Review

Linking the Common European Framework of Reference and the Michigan English Language Assessment Battery Technical Report

Testing protects against proactive interference in face name learning

Mathematics Education

2013 TRIAL URBAN DISTRICT ASSESSMENT (TUDA) RESULTS

BENCHMARK TREND COMPARISON REPORT:

Learning From the Past with Experiment Databases

Mathematics subject curriculum

Introduction to Psychology

The ADDIE Model. Michael Molenda Indiana University DRAFT

Probability and Statistics Curriculum Pacing Guide

Paper presented at the ERA-AARE Joint Conference, Singapore, November, 1996.

Mathematics Program Assessment Plan

The present study investigated whether subjects were sensitive to negative

USING CONCEPT MAPPING TO FACILITATE METACOGNITIVE CONTROL IN PRESCHOOL CHILDREN

OPTIMIZATINON OF TRAINING SETS FOR HEBBIAN-LEARNING- BASED CLASSIFIERS

Build on students informal understanding of sharing and proportionality to develop initial fraction concepts.

Morphosyntactic and Referential Cues to the Identification of Generic Statements

Research Design & Analysis Made Easy! Brainstorming Worksheet

Kristin Moser. Sherry Woosley, Ph.D. University of Northern Iowa EBI

WHY SOLVE PROBLEMS? INTERVIEWING COLLEGE FACULTY ABOUT THE LEARNING AND TEACHING OF PROBLEM SOLVING

Statewide Framework Document for:

Preliminary Report Initiative for Investigation of Race Matters and Underrepresented Minority Faculty at MIT Revised Version Submitted July 12, 2007

Cooper Upper Elementary School

The Talent Development High School Model Context, Components, and Initial Impacts on Ninth-Grade Students Engagement and Performance

Psychometric Research Brief Office of Shared Accountability

Requirements for the Degree: Bachelor of Science in Education in Early Childhood Special Education (P-5)

Software Maintenance

Update on the Next Accreditation System Drs. Culley, Ling, and Wood. Anesthesiology April 30, 2014

University-Based Induction in Low-Performing Schools: Outcomes for North Carolina New Teacher Support Program Participants in

Ohio s Learning Standards-Clear Learning Targets

MULTIPLE CHOICE. Choose the one alternative that best completes the statement or answers the question.

Race, Class, and the Selective College Experience

Radius STEM Readiness TM

DIDACTIC MODEL BRIDGING A CONCEPT WITH PHENOMENA

PH.D. IN COMPUTER SCIENCE PROGRAM (POST M.S.)

The Good Judgment Project: A large scale test of different methods of combining expert predictions

VII Medici Summer School, May 31 st - June 5 th, 2015

Merry-Go-Round. Science and Technology Grade 4: Understanding Structures and Mechanisms Pulleys and Gears. Language Grades 4-5: Oral Communication

Bayley scales of Infant and Toddler Development Third edition

Quantifying Student Progress through Bloom s Taxonomy Cognitive Categories in Computer Programming Courses

Transcription:

Title Children s expectations about training the approximate number system Authors Moira R. Dillon 1*, Ana C. Pires 2 3, Daniel C. Hyde 4, Elizabeth S. Spelke 1 Affiliations 1 Harvard University 2 Center for Basic Research on Psychology, Universidad de la República, Uruguay 3 Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain 4 University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign * To whom correspondence should be addressed: Moira R. Dillon Psychology Department, Harvard University 33 Kirkland St., Cambridge, MA 02138 USA E-mail: mdillon@fas.harvard.edu Acknowledgements We thank the Institute for Quantitative Social Sciences at Harvard University for their assistance with statistical analyses. This work was supported by grants to E. Spelke from the National Science Foundation (DRL-1348140), to M. Dillon from the National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship (DGE-1144152), and to A. Pires from the Fundação para a Ciencia e a Tecnologia (FCT, Portugal; SFRH/BD/77098/2011).! Word Count: 1,900 2!

Abstract Humans possess a developmentally precocious and evolutionarily ancient Approximate Number System (ANS) whose sensitivity correlates with uniquely human symbolic arithmetic skills. Recent studies suggest that ANS training improves symbolic arithmetic, but such studies may engender performance expectations in their participants that in turn produce the improvement. Here we assess 6- to 8-year-old children s expectations about the effects of numerical and nonnumerical magnitude training, as well as states of satiety and restfulness, in the context of a study linking children s ANS practice to their improved symbolic arithmetic. We found that children did not expect gains in symbolic arithmetic after exercising the ANS, though they did expect gains in ANS acuity after training on any magnitude task. Moreover, children expected gains in symbolic arithmetic after a good night s sleep and their favorite breakfast. Thus, children s improved symbolic arithmetic after ANS training cannot be explained by their expectations about that training. Keywords: approximate number system; symbolic mathematics; cognitive training; expectations 3!

1. Introduction Investigators from diverse areas in the cognitive sciences have proposed causal links between the processes that engage approximate numerical representations and those that underlie performance on tests of symbolic mathematics (e.g., Dehaene, 2011). The evidence supporting such claims begins with the widespread findings that the acuity with which an individual can employ their approximate number system (ANS) to distinguish between arrays of objects based on their number correlates with performance on tests of symbolic mathematics (e.g., Chen & Li, 2014; Halberda, Mazzocco & Feigenson, 2008; Hornung, Schiltz, Brunner & Martin, 2014; Starr, Libertus & Brannon, 2013). Recent cognitive training studies have suggested a causal link between ANS practice and improved performance on tests of symbolic mathematics: Both adults and children perform exact, symbolic arithmetic faster and more accurately after training on approximate, non-symbolic, representations of number, relative to training on other magnitude tasks (Hyde, Khanum & Spelke, 2014; Kuhn & Holling, 2014; Park & Brannon, 2013). The causal conclusions of such training studies appeal to a broad audience, including psychologists who are interested in their basic scientific claims and educators who may apply their methods to teaching mathematics in school. Boot, Simons, Stothart & Stutts (2013) argue, however, that cognitive training studies with active control conditions, presenting tasks with the same structure but with different content, are susceptible to placebo effects: Participants may expect training to yield benefits on assessments with similar content, and these expectations, rather than any gains in competence, may account for performance differences between the experimental and active control groups. ANS training experiments could well be vulnerable to such placebo effects: Participants may expect benefits on assessments with numerical content after training on an approximate 4!

numerical task, relative to training on tasks with no numerical content. Previous ANS training experiments thus risk yielding misleading findings about the causal link between the ANS and symbolic arithmetic. Here we ask whether children s expectations could drive the effects of ANS training on symbolic arithmetic. More specifically, does training approximate, non-symbolic numerical skills affect elementary school children s performance on exact, symbolic mathematics simply because those who received this training approach a symbolic mathematics assessment with different expectations or greater confidence compared to those who received the control training? This question is important both for developing accurate theories of numerical development and for teaching mathematics in school. If such expectation effects can be ruled out, then existing ANS training studies hold promise for revealing the causal links between intuitive and earlyemerging representations of number and uniquely human formal systems supporting symbolic mathematics. In contrast, if children s expectations account for the effects of ANS training observed in past experiments, it will be critical to craft new cognitive training experiments that control for such effects. Training experiments with children may carry heightened risks of expectation effects because children s motivations and expectations play a central role in their school achievement (Dweck, 1999). In addition, elementary school children have metacognitive awareness of their numerical discrimination judgments, and this awareness predicts their performance on the mathematics learned in school (Vo, Li, Kornell, Pouget & Cantlon, 2014). In the present study, we thus examine 6- to 8-year-old children s performance expectations in the context of a recent short-term training experiment linking children s non-symbolic numerical practice to their performance on symbolic mathematics (Hyde, et al., 2014, Experiment 1). 5!

In the experiment by Hyde et al. (2014), separate groups of children were given about 15 minutes of training on one of four approximate, non-symbolic magnitude tasks. One training task focused on adding arrays of dots; another task focused on comparing arrays of dots; a third task focused on adding line lengths; and a fourth task focused on comparing objects on the basis of surface brightness (Figure 1). Then children completed tests of written symbolic addition in Arabic notation and of non-symbolic numerical acuity (comparing pairs of dot arrays based on number, after Halberda et al., 2008). On the symbolic addition test, children who were trained on one of the two ANS training tasks (adding or comparing dot arrays) performed faster (with no loss in accuracy) than those who were trained on line length addition or surface brightness comparison. In addition, those trained on numerical comparison of dot arrays performed more accurately than those trained on line length addition. In contrast, ANS-trained children showed no gains in non-symbolic numerical acuity compared to those trained on non-numerical content. These results provide evidence for enhanced exact symbolic arithmetic after approximate, nonsymbolic number system training. Because this study employed active control groups trained on non-numerical content, however, we investigate whether children s expectations about each type of training could account for the obtained training outcomes. 2. Methods Twenty-four 6- to 8-year-old children (as in Hyde et al., 2014; mean = 7y 5m, range 6y 10m 7y 10m) participated in this study. Three more children were excluded because of symbolic addition scores more than four standard deviations below the mean obtained by Hyde et al. (2014). Children first completed portions of the outcome assessments used by Hyde et al. (2014) in a fixed order: They solved 20 1- and 2-digit symbolic addition problems (half of the 6!

original assessment in Hyde et al., 2014) and completed 60 items from a non-symbolic numerical comparison test (the complete assessment from Hyde et al., 2014), probing their sensitivity to numerical differences between pairs of dot arrays at ratios of 2:1, 3:2, 4:3, and 6:5 (see Hyde et al., 2014 for further details). In counterbalanced order, children were then introduced to each of the four training tasks in Hyde et al. (2014). To give them a good idea of each training task, they were presented with eight problems for each task from the original study. The first four were from the practice problems and the second four were from the training itself (Hyde et al., 2014). After each training task was introduced, children viewed a figure depicting that training task and a figure depicting the symbolic addition test. They were asked whether, after playing that training game for a while, they thought that their performance on the addition test would change, relative to their initial performance. Specifically, children were asked if they thought they would get more, fewer, or the same number of questions right and whether they would answer more quickly, more slowly, or in the same amount of time. Then children were shown a figure depicting the non-symbolic numerical acuity test and were asked the same questions about their expected accuracy and speed after performing the training task. Finally, children were asked in counterbalanced order to consider these same questions after a great night s sleep and their favorite breakfast, or after little sleep and no breakfast. Children s responses were recorded on a three-leveled ordinal scale. 3. Results Mirroring the analyses of other investigations probing placebo effects in cognitive training studies (e.g., Boot et al., 2013), we first compared children s expectations about the 7!

effects of training approximate numerical addition and approximate numerical comparison (both ANS training tasks), as well as line length addition and brightness comparison (both non-ans training tasks) to the specific outcomes obtained in Hyde et al. s (2014) experiment. None of children s expectations aligned with the previously observed ANS training effects: In particular, children did not expect that their speed and accuracy on Hyde et al. s (2014) test of symbolic addition would improve more after training on ANS tasks than after training non-numerical magnitude tasks (Table 1). Nevertheless, children judged that they would perform both tests more accurately and faster after a good night s sleep and their favorite breakfast than after little sleep and no breakfast (Table 2). Such a result reveals that, although children s expectations about the effects of ANS training on symbolic arithmetic did not align with Hyde et al. s (2014) training outcomes, they appeared to understand the questions, were motivated to answer them, and formed clear expectations about the effects of satiety and restfulness on their test performance. We further analyzed children s expectations for each training task, test administered, and outcome measure (accuracy or speed) from Hyde et al. (2014) using an ordinal logistic regression mixed model. (These data met the specifications of having proportional odds at each level of the ordinal scale: Likelihood Ratio Test: p =.112). Children showed no differential expectations about the effects of the four training tasks. However, across training tasks, they predicted greater gains in non-symbolic numerical acuity than in symbolic addition, and in accuracy than in speed (Figure 2; Table 3). In other words, children expected improvement on a measure of ANS acuity after training on any approximate magnitude task, whether or not that task specifically targeted the numerical representations that underlie the ANS. In addition, children did not expect that magnitude training would transfer to tests of symbolic arithmetic. 8!

These expectations contrast further with the obtained training effects of Hyde et al. (2014): Only children trained on ANS tasks (i.e., dot addition or comparison) showed training gains relative to those trained on non-ans tasks (i.e., line length addition or brightness comparison); these gains were shown on the test of symbolic addition but not numerical acuity; and they were manifest by gains in speed but not accuracy (except in one instance; Table 1). 4. Discussion and Conclusions The present study revealed that children s expectations concerning the effects of ANS training tasks relative to non-numerical training tasks are unlikely to account for the findings of Hyde et al. (2014): Six- to 8-year-old children did not expect that ANS training would improve their subsequent performance on Hyde et al. s (2014) test of symbolic arithmetic more than nonnumerical training. Nevertheless, children expected differential gains in symbolic arithmetic given different states of satiety and restfulness, and in general, children expected greater gains in ANS acuity than in symbolic arithmetic after training on any of Hyde et al. s (2014) magnitude tasks. By showing that children s expectations about ANS training do not align with training outcomes, we thus address an alternative account based on expectations that would challenge a causal link between the ANS and symbolic mathematics. Our findings also underscore the importance of testing more broadly for placebo effects in cognitive training studies with children: The children in our sample displayed a positive pattern of expectations about how states of satiety and restfulness might affect their performance on Hyde et al. s (2014) assessments and how magnitude training in general might affect their performance on an outcome measure with a similar structure. These findings, in turn, accord with a wealth of evidence that children of this age gain increasing awareness both of thinking in 9!

general (e.g., Flavell, Green, & Flavell, 1995) and of their sensitivity to number in particular (Vo et al., 2014). The growing body of work relying on cognitive training studies with children will benefit from further elucidating the expectations and motivations that particular training methods engender (Barner et al., in review; Cheng & Mix, 2014; Goldin et al., 2014; Kuhn & Holling, 2014), and the present study offers one method for doing so. The present findings are consistent with the claim that our developmentally precocious and evolutionally ancient numerical sensitivity is causally linked to the uniquely human and culturally constructed formal system of mathematics that supports human knowledge at its highest reaches. Nevertheless, the robustness and generality of ANS training effects merit further study with adults and children of different ages through training experiments that assess and, if necessary, control for effects of participants expectations on their cognitive performance. 10

References Barner, D., Alvarez, G., Sullivan, J., Brooks, N., Srinivasan, M., & Frank, M. (in review). Learning mathematics in a visuo-spatial format: A randomized controlled trial of mental abacus instruction. Boot, W. R., Simons, D. J., Stothart, C., & Stutts, C. (2013). The pervasive problem with placebos in psychology. Why active control groups are not sufficient to rule out placebo effects. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(4), 445-454. doi: 10.1177/1745691613491271 Chen, Q., & Li, J. (2014). Association between individual differences in non-symbolic number acuity and math performance: A meta-analysis. Acta Psychologica, 148, 163-172. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.01.016 Cheng, Y. L., & Mix, K. S. (2014). Spatial training improves children s mathematics ability. Journal of Cognition and Development, 15(1), 2-11. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2012.725186 Dehaene, S. (2011). The Number Sense: How The Mind Creates Mathematics, Revised And Updated Edition. New York: Oxford University Press. Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. Philadelphia: Psychology Press. Flavell, J. H., Green, F. L., Flavell, E. R., Harris, P. L., & Astington, J. W. (1995). Young children s knowledge about thinking. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, i-113. Goldin, A. P., Hermida, M. J., Shalom, D. E., Costa, M. E., Lopez-Rosenfeld, M., Segretin, M. S.,... & Sigman, M. (2014). Far transfer to language and math of a short software-based 11

gaming intervention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(17), 6443-6448. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1320217111 Halberda, J., Mazzocco, M., & Feigenson, L. (2008). Individual differences in nonverbal number acuity predicts maths achievement. Nature, 455, 665 669. doi:10.1038/nature07246 Hornung, C., Schiltz, C., Brunner, M., & Martin, R. (2014). Predicting first-grade mathematics achievement: the contributions of domain-general cognitive abilities, nonverbal number sense, and early number competence. Frontiers in Psychology, 5 (272). doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00272 Hyde, D. C., Khanum, S., & Spelke, E. S. (2014). Brief non-symbolic, approximate number practice enhances subsequent exact symbolic arithmetic in children. Cognition, 131(1), 92-107. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2013.12.007 Kuhn, J. T., & Holling, H. (2014). Number sense or working memory? The effect of two computer-based trainings on mathematical skills in elementary school. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 10(2), 59. doi: 10.5709/acp-0157-2 Park, J., & Brannon, E. M. (2013). Training the approximate number system improves math proficiency. Psychological Science, 24(10), 2013-2019. doi: 10.1177/0956797613482944 Starr, A., Libertus, M. E., & Brannon, E. M. (2013). Number sense in infancy predicts mathematical abilities in childhood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(45), 18116-18120. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1302751110 Vo, V. A., Li, R., Kornell, N., Pouget, A., & Cantlon, J. F. (2014). Young Children Bet on Their Numerical Skills Metacognition in the Numerical Domain. Psychological Science, 25(9), 1712-1721. doi: 10.1177/0956797614538458 12

Figure 1. Reprinted from Hyde et al. (2014), in which children were presented with one of four non-symbolic training tasks. Two of these tasks presented numerical content (a. Numerical Addition and c. Numerical Comparison) and two of these tasks presented non-numerical content (b. Line Length Addition and d. Brightness Comparison). The sequences of events in a sample training trial from each task are organized vertically from start (top) to finish (bottom) in the figure. The horizontal arrows indicate the movement of an item in the visual display. 13

RUNNING HEAD: TRAINING THE APPROXIMATE NUMBER SYSTEM Figure 2. The proportion of children s responses to whether they would perform better, the same, or worse on Hyde et al. s (2014) symbolic addition assessment and assessment of non-symbolic numerical acuity after training on the four non-symbolic magnitude tasks and given different states of satiety and restfulness.!

Table 1. Performance on the outcome measures that yielded differences in Hyde et al. s (2014) Experiment 1, compared to children s expectations about their efficacy in the present study. Training Task Test Measure Hyde et al. (2014) Expectations Numerical Comparison Symbolic Addition Speed Brightness Comparison Symbolic Addition Speed Numerical Comparison Symbolic Addition Speed Line Length Addition Symbolic Addition Speed Numerical Addition Symbolic Addition Speed Brightness Comparison Symbolic Addition Speed Numerical Addition Symbolic Addition Speed Line Length Addition Symbolic Addition Speed t(46) = 2.527, p <.05 McNemar = 2.87, p =.413 t(46) = 2.327, p <.05 McNemar = 2.14, p =.543 t(46) = 2.176, p <.05 McNemar = 2.14, p =.543 t(46) = 2.030, p <.05 McNemar = 0.53, p =.912 Numerical Comparison Symbolic Addition Accuracy t(46) = 2.576, p <.05 McNemar = 1.00, p =.801 Line Length Addition Symbolic Addition Accuracy!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!! 15

Table 2. Children's expectations about the effects of different! states of satiety and restfulness on! Hyde et al.'s (2014)! tests! of symbolic addition! and non-symbolic numerical acuity.!!!!!!!!!! State of Satiety & Restfulness Good Night's Sleep, Favorite Breakfast! Symbolic Addition! Accuracy Test Measure Poor Night's Sleep, No Breakfast Symbolic Addition Accuracy! Expectations McNemar = 16.33, p =.001 Good Night's Sleep, Favorite Breakfast Symbolic Addition Speed Poor Night's Sleep, No Breakfast Symbolic Addition Speed McNemar = 9.50, p =.023 Good Night's Sleep, Favorite Breakfast Non-Symbolic Numerical Acuity Accuracy Poor Night's Sleep, No Breakfast Non-Symbolic Numerical Acuity Accuracy McNemar = 14.00, p =.003 Good Night's Sleep, Favorite Breakfast Non-Symbolic Numerical Acuity Speed McNemar = 13.80, p =.003 Poor Night's Sleep, No Breakfast Non-Symbolic Numerical Acuity Speed!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!! 16

Table 3. The ordinal logistic regression mixed model evaluating children s expectations across pairs of: training conditions; tests; and measures.!! Reference Comparison p value* % Change in Odds Ratio 95% CI for % Change Training Numerical Comparison Numerical Addition.720 55.81 a 10.86 b - 172.33 a Numerical Comparison Brightness Comparison 1.000 14.06 a 34.60 b - 98.90 a Numerical Comparison Line Length Addition 1.000 7.33 a 38.55 b - 87.49 a Numerical Addition Brightness Comparison 1.000 26.80 b 58.21 b - 28.24 a Numerical Addition Line Length Addition.970 31.11 b 60.74 b - 20.88 a Brightness Comparison Line Length Addition 1.000 5.89 b 46.28 b - 64.85 a Test Symbolic Addition Non-Symbolic Numerical Acuity.031 35.54 a 4.04 a - 56.70 a!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!! Measure Speed Accuracy.008 41.66 a 13.09 a - 60.85 a!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!! * Holm corrected for multiple comparisons. Percentage changes in the proportional odds ratios produced by the model are included to quantify the differences in which children expected that the odds for improving would be a greater for the comparison group than the reference group or the b reverse. For example, children expected that after training, the odds of improving on the non-symbolic numerical acuity test were 35.54% more likely than the odds of improving on the symbolic addition test. p values and 95% Confidence Intervals are provided for interpreting these differences. 17