Epenthetic Processes

Similar documents
The analysis starts with the phonetic vowel and consonant charts based on the dataset:

Pobrane z czasopisma New Horizons in English Studies Data: 18/11/ :52:20. New Horizons in English Studies 1/2016

Consonants: articulation and transcription

Phonetics. The Sound of Language

Word Stress and Intonation: Introduction

Phonological Processing for Urdu Text to Speech System

Consonant-Vowel Unity in Element Theory*

The Perception of Nasalized Vowels in American English: An Investigation of On-line Use of Vowel Nasalization in Lexical Access

SOUND STRUCTURE REPRESENTATION, REPAIR AND WELL-FORMEDNESS: GRAMMAR IN SPOKEN LANGUAGE PRODUCTION. Adam B. Buchwald

Parallel Evaluation in Stratal OT * Adam Baker University of Arizona

Manner assimilation in Uyghur

Speech Recognition using Acoustic Landmarks and Binary Phonetic Feature Classifiers

Books Effective Literacy Y5-8 Learning Through Talk Y4-8 Switch onto Spelling Spelling Under Scrutiny

Florida Reading Endorsement Alignment Matrix Competency 1

Phonology Revisited: Sor3ng Out the PH Factors in Reading and Spelling Development. Indiana, November, 2015

1. REFLEXES: Ask questions about coughing, swallowing, of water as fast as possible (note! Not suitable for all

Acoustic correlates of stress and their use in diagnosing syllable fusion in Tongan. James White & Marc Garellek UCLA

Prevalence of Oral Reading Problems in Thai Students with Cleft Palate, Grades 3-5

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

To appear in the Proceedings of the 35th Meetings of the Chicago Linguistics Society. Post-vocalic spirantization: Typology and phonetic motivations

Universal contrastive analysis as a learning principle in CAPT

Language Acquisition by Identical vs. Fraternal SLI Twins * Karin Stromswold & Jay I. Rifkin

Lexical phonology. Marc van Oostendorp. December 6, Until now, we have presented phonological theory as if it is a monolithic

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

ADDIS ABABA UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF GRADUATE STUDIES MODELING IMPROVED AMHARIC SYLLBIFICATION ALGORITHM

Radical CV Phonology: the locational gesture *

An argument from speech pathology

ELA/ELD Standards Correlation Matrix for ELD Materials Grade 1 Reading

1 st Quarter (September, October, November) August/September Strand Topic Standard Notes Reading for Literature

**Note: this is slightly different from the original (mainly in format). I would be happy to send you a hard copy.**

Linguistics 220 Phonology: distributions and the concept of the phoneme. John Alderete, Simon Fraser University

Contrasting English Phonology and Nigerian English Phonology

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Voiced-voiceless distinction in alaryngeal speech - acoustic and articula

Journal of Phonetics

source or where they are needed to distinguish two forms of a language. 4. Geographical Location. I have attempted to provide a geographical

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Lang Speech. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 January 1.

Similarity Avoidance in the Proto-Indo-European Root

SEGMENTAL FEATURES IN SPONTANEOUS AND READ-ALOUD FINNISH

(3) Vocabulary insertion targets subtrees (4) The Superset Principle A vocabulary item A associated with the feature set F can replace a subtree X

Underlying Representations

First Grade Curriculum Highlights: In alignment with the Common Core Standards

Christine Mooshammer, IPDS Kiel, Philip Hoole, IPSK München, Anja Geumann, Dublin

The Journey to Vowelerria VOWEL ERRORS: THE LOST WORLD OF SPEECH INTERVENTION. Preparation: Education. Preparation: Education. Preparation: Education

LING 329 : MORPHOLOGY

Precedence Constraints and Opacity

Unvoiced Landmark Detection for Segment-based Mandarin Continuous Speech Recognition

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Sound symbolism in deictic words

Contrastiveness and diachronic variation in Chinese nasal codas. Tsz-Him Tsui The Ohio State University

Taught Throughout the Year Foundational Skills Reading Writing Language RF.1.2 Demonstrate understanding of spoken words,

Get Your Hands On These Multisensory Reading Strategies

Markedness and Complex Stops: Evidence from Simplification Processes 1. Nick Danis Rutgers University

I propose an analysis of thorny patterns of reduplication in the unrelated languages Saisiyat

A Fact in Historical Phonology from the Viewpoint of Generative Phonology: The Underlying Schwa in Old English

DOWNSTEP IN SUPYIRE* Robert Carlson Societe Internationale de Linguistique, Mali

THE PHONOLOGICAL WORD IN STANDARD MALA Y

Houghton Mifflin Reading Correlation to the Common Core Standards for English Language Arts (Grade1)

5. Margi (Chadic, Nigeria): H, L, R (Williams 1973, Hoffmann 1963)

RP ENGLISH AND CASTILIAN SPANISH DIPHTHONGS REVISITED FROM THE BEATS-AND-BINDING PERSPECTIVE

Perceived speech rate: the effects of. articulation rate and speaking style in spontaneous speech. Jacques Koreman. Saarland University

Linguistics. Undergraduate. Departmental Honors. Graduate. Faculty. Linguistics 1

Learning Methods in Multilingual Speech Recognition

Phonological encoding in speech production

On the Formation of Phoneme Categories in DNN Acoustic Models

On the nature of voicing assimilation(s)

CLASSIFICATION OF PROGRAM Critical Elements Analysis 1. High Priority Items Phonemic Awareness Instruction

Listener-oriented phonology

Partial Class Behavior and Nasal Place Assimilation*

Automatic English-Chinese name transliteration for development of multilingual resources

Effect of Word Complexity on L2 Vocabulary Learning

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

English for Life. B e g i n n e r. Lessons 1 4 Checklist Getting Started. Student s Book 3 Date. Workbook. MultiROM. Test 1 4

The Indian English of Tibeto-Burman language speakers*

Stages of Literacy Ros Lugg

READ 180 Next Generation Software Manual

Sounds of Infant-Directed Vocabulary: Learned from Infants Speech or Part of Linguistic Knowledge?

ABSTRACT. Some children with speech sound disorders (SSD) have difficulty with literacyrelated

Richardson, J., The Next Step in Guided Writing, Ohio Literacy Conference, 2010

Quantitative Reasoning in Linguistics

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

Clinical Application of the Mean Babbling Level and Syllable Structure Level

Revisiting the role of prosody in early language acquisition. Megha Sundara UCLA Phonetics Lab

MANAGERIAL LEADERSHIP

The influence of orthographic transparency on word recognition. by dyslexic and normal readers

Liaison acquisition, word segmentation and construction in French: A usage based account

More Morphology. Problem Set #1 is up: it s due next Thursday (1/19) fieldwork component: Figure out how negation is expressed in your language.

Constraining X-Bar: Theta Theory

Weave the Critical Literacy Strands and Build Student Confidence to Read! Part 2

INSTANT VOCABULARY 6-10

Senior Stenographer / Senior Typist Series (including equivalent Secretary titles)

NCU IISR English-Korean and English-Chinese Named Entity Transliteration Using Different Grapheme Segmentation Approaches

Multilingual Speech Data Collection for the Assessment of Pronunciation and Prosody in a Language Learning System

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

Frequency and pragmatically unmarked word order *

Portuguese Vowel Harmony: A Comparative Analysis and the Superiority of Autosegmental Representations

The ABCs of O-G. Materials Catalog. Skills Workbook. Lesson Plans for Teaching The Orton-Gillingham Approach in Reading and Spelling

Jacqueline C. Kowtko, Patti J. Price Speech Research Program, SRI International, Menlo Park, CA 94025

Program in Linguistics. Academic Year Assessment Report

Eli Yamamoto, Satoshi Nakamura, Kiyohiro Shikano. Graduate School of Information Science, Nara Institute of Science & Technology

Transcription:

Papers in Linguistics 3:4 pp. 606 610 (1972) c 1972 D M Daly Epenthetic Processes D.M. Daly (Univ. of Texas) and L.W. Martin (Univ. of Iowa) Rules of epenthesis are used to introduce consonants, semi-vowels, and vowels either as phonological markers of syntactic information or as purely phonological phenomena. In this note we will examine several properties of purely phonological epenthesis. 1 Consonantal Epenthesis Current theories permit a wide variety of consonantal epentheses in terms of both the kinds of segments which might be inserted, and the contexts in which insertion might occur. In languages, however, these segments and contexts are highly restricted; there are two types. 1.1 A consonant is inserted between two consonants. Examples of this type include: 1

tem lo m > templum eks em lo m > exemplum anr os > andros (Latin temple ) (Latin example ) (Greek man ) In these cases, the epenthetic consonant is homorganic with the preceding nasal, and occurs at a syllable boundary. There are no examples of such hypothetical forms as: omlo > omklo omlo > okmlo This type of epenthetic consonant is an artifact of co-articulatory readjustments [1]. It should be regarded as a phonetic result of resegmentation. 1.2 A consonant is inserted between a boundary and a vowel. There are several examples of this type. In Zuñi, a glottal stop appears before a morpheme initial vowel if that morpheme is word initial or follows a morpheme ending in a vowel [2], so that: iy anna > Piyanna we anna > wepanna to come (he) will to become sick (he) will but: t iy anna > tiyanna they to come will In these cases, the epenthetic consonant is a glottal stop [3] and always appears at a syllable boundary. 2

In Maru, the stops [t] and [k] are inserted following words ending in a high front vowel or high back vowel respectively[4]. The only final stops are [p t k P]. In these cases the epenthetic consonants are limited to stops produced with the tongue (i.e., the bilabial and glottal stops are excluded); the stops are homorganic with the preceding vowel. There are no instances in either case of such hypothetical forms as: ae > k w ae ae > aek w ae > ak w e The epenthetic consonant from cases of this type should be regarded as a phonetic result of syllable preservation or retention of syllable shape. In both types of consonantal epenthesis, a phonetically predictable segment is inserted. Although in theory many phonetically unpredictable segments are also possible, none of these occurs in natural languages. These facts indicate that rules of epenthesis do not introduce new consonantal segments directly. Further, they suggest that there are no purely phonological rules of consonant epenthesis. One prediction which follows from this hypothesis is that metathetic processes never involve an intermediate step, such as: art kos > arktkos > arktos (Greek bear ) 3

In this derivation, the first step involves epenthesis of a consonant. If metathesis is a phonological process, this derivation is excluded in principle. If metathesis were a phonetic process, this derivation would violate the condition on homorganicity. Thus, such derivations cannot be an account of a metathetic process. In conjunction with our general hypothesis, it is interesting to note that: In general, it is easier to reconstruct a written passage that has consonants but no vowels, than it is to reconstruct a passage that has vowels but no consonants. Languages may have writing systems which preserve only consonants (i.e., the reader supplies the proper vowels), but no language has a writing system which preserves only vowels (i.e., the reader would supply the proper consonants). 2 Glide (Semi-vowel) Epenthesis Languages may allow epenthesis (more probably, formation) of glides. In such languages, the glide is generally inserted between two vowels. For example, in Cashinahua [5] a glide is inserted between two unlike vowels so that if the first vowel is: /i/, the glide is [y]. /u/, the glide is [w]. /2/, the glide is [G]. The vowels of Cashinahua are /i u 2 a/; /2/ is normally pronounced as [i ]. 4

The epenthetic glide always occurs at a syllable boundary, and is homorganic with the preceding vowel.[6] It should be regarded as a phonetic result of syllable retention. The restrictions on epenthetic glides are analogous to the restrictions on epenthetic consonants. In both glide and consonant epenthesis, the segment inserted is phonetically predictable; moreover, the epenthetic segment preserves the existing syllable structure. These facts suggest that there is no purely phonological epenthesis of either glides or consonants. 3 Vowel Epenthesis Languages allow epenthesis of segments which are not completely phonetically predictable. These segments are introduced for syllabification, and may undergo certain phonological rules; they are always vowels. The phonetic properties of these epenthetic vowels are at least partially determined by the base of articulation of the particular language. The base of articulation is the neutral position (for a speaker) of a language. It is determined by the positioning of the major articulators (i.e., the specific position of the velum, base of the tongue, jaw, and lips), and may vary for different languages. The neutral or lessmarked vowel(s) of a language reflect the base of articulation. Thus, the vowel will tend to be high if the base of the tongue is high, fronted if the base of the tongue is fronted, round if the lips are rounded, and so forth. 5

An epenthetic vowel is generally unmarked or less-marked. That is, no language has such segments as / 1/, / ā /, or a voiceless vowel as its only epenthetic segment; nor does any language have epenthetic diphthongs. There are two types of apparent exceptions to this claim; both result from locally marked contexts. 3.1 Languages with vowel harmony. Some languages have a series of vowels which is distinguished either by rounding or by both rounding and front-back. The characteristics of particular vowels are prescribed by rules of vowel harmony. In languages with vowel harmony, it is often the case that a normally unmarked vowel, if epenthetized, would produce a marked sequence of vowels. Under these conditions we would expect to find unusual epenthetic vowels, but only those vowels which are in accord with the specific vowel harmony rules. In Turkish [8], for example, foreign loan words with underlying initial consonant clusters often resyllabify, so that: priz > piriz tramvay > tiramvay klub > kulub > (or less commonly) kilub trafik > tirafik > (or less commonly) tirafik ( prize ) ( tramway ) ( club ) ( traffic ) In cases with an unusual epenthetic vowel, the segment results from the application of the vowel harmony rules. 6

3.2 Languages with marked series of consonants. Some languages have series of consonants which are marked in terms of either primary or secondary articulations. Russian, for example, has both a palatal and a palatalized series of consonants. Such series of consonants correlate with the base of articulation characteristic of the language, In contexts such as these, an otherwise unmarked vowel would be considered more highly marked than some otherwise non-neutral vowels. This suggests that: 1. A language may have an epenthetic / i / if and only if that language has a palatal or a palatalized series of consonants. (e.g., Turkish) 2. A language may have an epenthetic / å / if and only if that language has a pharyngeal or pharyngealized (perhaps a glottal or a glottalized) series of consonants. (e.g., Mohawk, Klamath) 3. A language may have an epenthetic / 1 / if and only if that language has a labio velar or (labio) velarized series of consonants. (e.g., Alaskan Eskimo [9]) Languages with unusual epenthetic vowels and marked series of consonants thus provide additional support for our account of vowel epenthesis. 7

4 Conclusions Languages have a number of processes available to satisfy language-specific constraints on syllable structure (or syllabification). These processes include vowel or consonant deletion, vowel or consonant gemination, and vowel epenthesis. Phonological rules of epenthesis reform consonant clusters by syllabification. The particular phonetic characteristics of epenthetic vowels reflect the languagespecific base of articulation, and may result from locally marked contexts. Acknowledgements A. Cearley provided the examples from Eskimo. David DeCamp and Charlotte Webb read and commented on earlier versions of this paper. Robert King generously offered examples, counter-examples, and criticism which helped clarify the issues. We are grateful to all of these people for their help. References [1] Harms, R. T. (1971). Some Non-rules of English. University of Texas manuscript. [2] Read, G. (personal communication). [3] Marking theory treats glottal stop as a highly marked segment. If / P/ is a segment, marking theory should be revised to accommodate these considerations. [4] Burling, R. (1966). The Addition of Final Stops in the History of Maru. Language, 42 (pp. 581 586). 8

[5] Kensinger, K. M. (1963). The Phonological Hierarchy of Cashinahua. In B. F. Nelson (ed.), Studies in Peruvian Indian Languages 1, (pp. 207 217). [6] In the spelling of Cashinahua, the sh is an English artifact, and the hu is a Spanish artifact. [7] Another possible vowel harmony type is based on a tense-lax distinction. There are several constraints on vowel-harmony types. R. T Harms (personal communication) has suggested A language may have a front-back vowel harmony if and only if that language also has a roundness harmony. A second constraint is that no language may have a vowel harmony that operates only over vowel height. That is, there can be no high:non-high, high:low, or non-high:low rules of vowel harmony. Glides introduced by glide formation are similarly constrained. No language has a height distinction in glides unless that language also has a front-back distinction for glides. [8] Granoff, I. (personal communication). [9] Our claim seems weakened by the restrictions on this third universal. Despite the fact that a majority of languages have velar consonant series, no language that we know has epenthetic /u G /. This observation may further clarify the restrictions on the base of articulation. A. Cearley (personal communication) noted that in Greenland Eskimo a word may have a voiceless epenthetic vowel, just in case all of the other vowels in the word are voiceless. This observation provides additional support for the notion of locally marked contexts. 9