The SAGE Encyclopedia of Lifespan Human Development Core Knowledge

Similar documents
9.85 Cognition in Infancy and Early Childhood. Lecture 7: Number

What is PDE? Research Report. Paul Nichols

Full text of O L O W Science As Inquiry conference. Science as Inquiry

Lecture 2: Quantifiers and Approximation

Backwards Numbers: A Study of Place Value. Catherine Perez

Focus of the Unit: Much of this unit focuses on extending previous skills of multiplication and division to multi-digit whole numbers.

No Parent Left Behind

Unraveling symbolic number processing and the implications for its association with mathematics. Delphine Sasanguie

Rendezvous with Comet Halley Next Generation of Science Standards

Extending Place Value with Whole Numbers to 1,000,000

Concept Acquisition Without Representation William Dylan Sabo

Linking object names and object categories: Words (but not tones) facilitate object categorization in 6- and 12-month-olds

AGS THE GREAT REVIEW GAME FOR PRE-ALGEBRA (CD) CORRELATED TO CALIFORNIA CONTENT STANDARDS

Introduction to Psychology

Exact Equality and Successor Function : Two Keys Concepts on the Path towards Understanding Exact Numbers

Learning Disability Functional Capacity Evaluation. Dear Doctor,

Evolution of Symbolisation in Chimpanzees and Neural Nets

Assessment and Evaluation

APA Basics. APA Formatting. Title Page. APA Sections. Title Page. Title Page

UNDERSTANDING DECISION-MAKING IN RUGBY By. Dave Hadfield Sport Psychologist & Coaching Consultant Wellington and Hurricanes Rugby.

Science Fair Project Handbook

Montana Content Standards for Mathematics Grade 3. Montana Content Standards for Mathematical Practices and Mathematics Content Adopted November 2011

Proposal of Pattern Recognition as a necessary and sufficient principle to Cognitive Science

KS1 Transport Objectives

Building A Baby. Paul R. Cohen, Tim Oates, Marc S. Atkin Department of Computer Science

ECON 365 fall papers GEOS 330Z fall papers HUMN 300Z fall papers PHIL 370 fall papers

Strategies for Solving Fraction Tasks and Their Link to Algebraic Thinking

Stages of Literacy Ros Lugg

A Bayesian Model of Imitation in Infants and Robots

A Study of the Effectiveness of Using PER-Based Reforms in a Summer Setting

Monitoring Metacognitive abilities in children: A comparison of children between the ages of 5 to 7 years and 8 to 11 years

Book Reviews. Michael K. Shaub, Editor

Physical Features of Humans

1 3-5 = Subtraction - a binary operation

Innovative Methods for Teaching Engineering Courses

CWIS 23,3. Nikolaos Avouris Human Computer Interaction Group, University of Patras, Patras, Greece

THE HEAD START CHILD OUTCOMES FRAMEWORK

Contents. Foreword... 5

Notes on The Sciences of the Artificial Adapted from a shorter document written for course (Deciding What to Design) 1

On Human Computer Interaction, HCI. Dr. Saif al Zahir Electrical and Computer Engineering Department UBC

Language Acquisition Fall 2010/Winter Lexical Categories. Afra Alishahi, Heiner Drenhaus

Radius STEM Readiness TM

Language Development: The Components of Language. How Children Develop. Chapter 6

Contact: For more information on Breakthrough visit or contact Carmel Crévola at Resources:

Genevieve L. Hartman, Ph.D.

Abstractions and the Brain

Save Children. Can Math Recovery. before They Fail?

All Systems Go! Using a Systems Approach in Elementary Science

Standard 1: Number and Computation

SOFTWARE EVALUATION TOOL

University of Groningen. Systemen, planning, netwerken Bosman, Aart

Introducing the New Iowa Assessments Mathematics Levels 12 14

Build on students informal understanding of sharing and proportionality to develop initial fraction concepts.

Piaget s Cognitive Development

ICTCM 28th International Conference on Technology in Collegiate Mathematics

THE ROLE OF TOOL AND TEACHER MEDIATIONS IN THE CONSTRUCTION OF MEANINGS FOR REFLECTION

Achievement Level Descriptors for American Literature and Composition

Grade 6: Correlated to AGS Basic Math Skills

Common Core State Standards for English Language Arts

AGENDA LEARNING THEORIES LEARNING THEORIES. Advanced Learning Theories 2/22/2016

Philosophy of Literacy Education. Becoming literate is a complex step by step process that begins at birth. The National

Classifying combinations: Do students distinguish between different types of combination problems?

PSYC 588A (3 credits): Special Topics in Social and Personality Development Primary Focus: The Development of Implicit Social Cognition

Visual CP Representation of Knowledge

Language acquisition: acquiring some aspects of syntax.

Simple Random Sample (SRS) & Voluntary Response Sample: Examples: A Voluntary Response Sample: Examples: Systematic Sample Best Used When

GDP Falls as MBA Rises?

EGRHS Course Fair. Science & Math AP & IB Courses

(Still) Unskilled and Unaware of It?

PART C: ENERGIZERS & TEAM-BUILDING ACTIVITIES TO SUPPORT YOUTH-ADULT PARTNERSHIPS

Room: Office Hours: T 9:00-12:00. Seminar: Comparative Qualitative and Mixed Methods

Ohio s Learning Standards-Clear Learning Targets

While you are waiting... socrative.com, room number SIMLANG2016

Digital Fabrication and Aunt Sarah: Enabling Quadratic Explorations via Technology. Michael L. Connell University of Houston - Downtown

What is Research? A Reconstruction from 15 Snapshots. Charlie Van Loan

Computerized Adaptive Psychological Testing A Personalisation Perspective

Oklahoma State University Policy and Procedures

Developed by Dr. Carl A. Ferreri & Additional Concepts by Dr. Charles Krebs. Expanded by

Essentials of Ability Testing. Joni Lakin Assistant Professor Educational Foundations, Leadership, and Technology

Foundations of Knowledge Representation in Cyc

Lesson M4. page 1 of 2

Curriculum Design Project with Virtual Manipulatives. Gwenanne Salkind. George Mason University EDCI 856. Dr. Patricia Moyer-Packenham

Prentice Hall Literature Common Core Edition Grade 10, 2012

Statewide Framework Document for:

MYCIN. The MYCIN Task

SAT MATH PREP:

IST 649: Human Interaction with Computers

Promoting Active Learning in University Classes

MERGA 20 - Aotearoa

Introductory thoughts on numeracy

Social Emotional Learning in High School: How Three Urban High Schools Engage, Educate, and Empower Youth

South Carolina College- and Career-Ready Standards for Mathematics. Standards Unpacking Documents Grade 5

Math 96: Intermediate Algebra in Context

Procedia Social and Behavioral Sciences 8 (2010)

Missouri Mathematics Grade-Level Expectations

STRATEGIC LEADERSHIP PROCESSES

Syllabus ENGR 190 Introductory Calculus (QR)

OCR for Arabic using SIFT Descriptors With Online Failure Prediction

Kaufman Assessment Battery For Children

Math-U-See Correlation with the Common Core State Standards for Mathematical Content for Third Grade

Transcription:

The SAGE Encyclopedia of Lifespan Human Development Core Knowledge Contributors: Susan J. Hespos Edited by: Marc H. Bornstein Book Title: Chapter Title: "Core Knowledge" Pub. Date: 2018 Access Date: March 30, 2018 Publishing Company: SAGE Publications, Inc. City: Thousand Oaks, Print ISBN: 9781506307657 Online ISBN: 9781506307633 DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.4135/9781506307633.n176 Print pages: 470-471 2018 SAGE Publications, Inc.. All Rights Reserved. This PDF has been generated from SAGE Knowledge. Please note that the pagination of the online version will vary from the pagination of the print book.

Core knowledge is a psychological theory that addresses age-old questions of what capacities are present from birth (therefore a product of evolution) and what capacities are acquired through experience. The central focus of this approach is whether uniquely human capacities are evident early in development or whether the differences between human abilities and those of other species emerge later in development. To help people understand their uniquely human capacities, one goal of this theory is to compare human cognitive capacities with those of other species to see where these paths diverge. The key tenet of this approach is that underneath all the things that vary across humans, there exists a set of perceptual and conceptual capacities common to everyone. These core knowledge systems are innate mechanisms that do specific tasks. Some examples of core knowledge domains across species are a songbird s ability to learn a song characteristic of its species, an ant s ability to navigate a terrain in search of food and then make a direct line back to the nest, and a human s ability to acquire language. To illustrate the ideas behind core knowledge theory, this entry describes two examples of core knowledge domains and the research that supports these ideas. It then discusses the larger question of how these findings lend new insight into what makes people so smart. The Domain of Objects Growing up, children never receive explicit instruction about how objects behave and interact, yet they draw universally similar expectations. For example, they all expect unsupported objects to fall down; they universally agree that hidden objects do not cease to exist when they are occluded from view. Such expectations appear to be universal across age groups as well as across individuals. Expectations about object knowledge are evident in other species as well. Rhesus macaques expect an object to stop when it comes in contact with a wall and not to pass through it. Humans and chickens have similar expectations about partially occluded objects. These abilities may have a long history across generations and species and are most likely the product of evolution. Experiments with infants show that from an early age infants parse the visual array into objects and events. As early as 2 months of age, infants expect objects to be permanent (e.g., they do not blink in and out of existence) and that two objects cannot occupy the same space at the same time (e.g., a ball cannot pass through a wall). Common to all humans, objects are perceived as bounded, solid, continuous entities. Yet object knowledge abilities are not fully mature at birth. Infants are active learners with powerful mechanisms of associative learning and memory abilities that become integrated with core knowledge systems. These initial core knowledge representations are primitive and leave many aspects of object knowledge undefined. With development, specifically through experience interacting with the environment, infants learn which variables predict the outcomes of events more precisely. For example, at 2 months of age infants already know that an object placed inside a container will move with the container when the container travels to a new location because objects do not pass through one another. However, infants have not identified detailed variables such as how the height of an object determines how much of it will be hidden inside a container. Experiments with 5-month-old infants show that infants are not surprised if a very tall object is completely hidden in a short Page 2 of 5

container. In contrast by 8 months of age, infants have gained more experience manipulating objects in their environment and now expect that a tall object would be only partially occluded if lowered into a short container. These findings provide evidence that the change in object knowledge is one of elaboration and refinement. Taken together, these examples reveal that knowledge about objects goes beyond the information available in the environment in ways that are universal. The evidence from young infants suggests that these inferences are made based on little or no experience. Furthermore, infants have expectations that objects are cohesive and continuous. These expectations emerge early in life and there is relatively little variation for these expectations across species. The Domain of Number Similar to knowledge about objects, individuals also have knowledge about number. They can tell that there is a difference between eight dots and 16 dots without counting. They can make this judgment quickly and accurately. Many other species are good at making these types of judgments, including nonhuman primates, rats, and pigeons. For example, monkeys have the ability to represent and compare the approximate numbers of sets. This discrimination ability appears to be based on analog magnitudes; the proportion rather than the absolute difference between two values determines their discriminability. For example, adults find it easier to discriminate 100 from 200 dots (1:2 ratio) than 200 to 300 dots (a 2:3 ratio) even though both comparisons differ by 100 dots. There are developmental changes in the precision of this ability. Experiments with infants show that from as early as 6 months of age, infants can detect a 1:2 ratio change in large numbers (e.g., 4 vs. 8, 8 vs. 16, 16 vs. 32), but not a 2:3 ratio change. By 9 months of age, infants can detect a 2:3 ratio change; the precision in detection increases to a 7:8 ratio in adults. These numerical discriminations predict performance across a variety of stimuli, including objects, tones, and actions (e.g., jumps of a puppet). These examples reveal the ability to abstract numerical information across a variety of perceptual inputs, and all share a common ratio-based signature. As with object knowledge, the developmental change in magnitude judgments appears to be one of elaboration and refinement. How Core Knowledge Systems Integrate With Other Cognitive Systems The core knowledge domains share three characteristics. First, core knowledge is task specific in that each system functions to solve a limited set of problems. For example, the number system can discriminate quantities based on their ratio, but this is not equivalent to the ability to count. The natural number system that supports counting is a complex construction that requires integration from the object and number systems as well as induction about the relation between number words and their referents. Second, core knowledge domains are encapsulated in that each system operates with a fair degree of independence from other cognitive systems. An advantage of encapsulation is that core abilities are universal and are effortlessly acquired with very little experience. A limitation of encapsulation is that these abilities lack flexibility and precision. Evidence supporting the independence of core systems comes from neurological research identifying specialized brain areas for processing this information as well as clinical studies showing that these domainspecific abilities can be lost, whereas other cognitive abilities remain intact. Page 3 of 5

Third, core knowledge is made up of a limited number of domain-specific systems that constitute evolutionarily important abilities. There is evidence supporting domain-specific systems of objects, number, agents and their actions, and geometry of the environment. Goals for the Future The core knowledge theory has been around for 30 years. In that time, how has it changed insights about what makes humans so smart? The field started with the premise that whatever it is that is special about humans should reveal itself in infants before the impact of social, cultural, and educational experiences. The notion was that people should see the essential differences between themselves and other species from the start. Over the past three decades, it has been found that this initial premise was wrong. On the one hand, infants have sophisticated knowledge that they build on over the course of development. At the same time, research has discovered that there are striking similarities between humans initial cognitive capacities and those of other animals. The expectations that chickens have about objects and the number skills of nonhuman primates show that human s core cognitive abilities share many of the same signatures as other species. The next step for the field moving forward is to use the comparisons between humans initial cognitive capacities and those of other animals to see where these paths diverge. The uniquely human ability may be grounded in using the core systems as a foundation for building new cognitive skills. Perhaps the reason humans seem so smart is that other species lack the ability to combine the input from various core knowledge systems. This ability creates entirely new representation systems such as tool use, symbolic arithmetic, and navigating by maps and landmarks. See alsocognition; Infancy; Knowledge; Language; Numeracy infants species number systems ratio knowledge theory of knowledge domain Susan J. Hespos http://dx.doi.org/10.4135/9781506307633.n176 10.4135/9781506307633.n176 Further Readings Carey, S. (2009). The origin of concepts. New York, NY: Oxford. Hespos, S. J., & vanmarle, K. (2012). Physics for infants: Characterizing the origins of knowledge about objects, substances, and number. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 3(1), 19 27. Spelke, E. S. (2000). Core knowledge. American Psychologist, 55(11), 1230 1243. Spelke, E. S. (2007). Core knowledge. Developmental Science, 10(1), 89 96. Spelke, E. S. (2010). Innateness, choice, and language. In J. Bricmont & J. Franck (Eds.), Chomsky notebook (pp. 203 210). New York, NY: Columbia University Press. Page 4 of 5

Page 5 of 5