3a. ATOM. CAS LX 500 Topics: Language acquisition. Spring 2010, January 26

Similar documents
CHILDREN S POSSESSIVE STRUCTURES: A CASE STUDY 1. Andrew Radford and Joseph Galasso, University of Essex

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Constraining X-Bar: Theta Theory

Case government vs Case agreement: modelling Modern Greek case attraction phenomena in LFG

CAS LX 522 Syntax I. Long-distance wh-movement. Long distance wh-movement. Islands. Islands. Locality. NP Sea. NP Sea

Approaches to control phenomena handout Obligatory control and morphological case: Icelandic and Basque

Theoretical Syntax Winter Answers to practice problems

2014 Colleen Elizabeth Fitzgerald

Multiple case assignment and the English pseudo-passive *

Phenomena of gender attraction in Polish *

A Computational Evaluation of Case-Assignment Algorithms

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

Part I. Figuring out how English works

Underlying and Surface Grammatical Relations in Greek consider

Minding the Absent: Arguments for the Full Competence Hypothesis 1. Abstract

a) analyse sentences, so you know what s going on and how to use that information to help you find the answer.

Construction Grammar. University of Jena.

Inleiding Taalkunde. Docent: Paola Monachesi. Blok 4, 2001/ Syntax 2. 2 Phrases and constituent structure 2. 3 A minigrammar of Italian 3

1/20 idea. We ll spend an extra hour on 1/21. based on assigned readings. so you ll be ready to discuss them in class

Introduction to HPSG. Introduction. Historical Overview. The HPSG architecture. Signature. Linguistic Objects. Descriptions.

The Acquisition of Person and Number Morphology Within the Verbal Domain in Early Greek

Agree or Move? On Partial Control Anna Snarska, Adam Mickiewicz University

Developing a TT-MCTAG for German with an RCG-based Parser

Developing Grammar in Context

Som and Optimality Theory

An Introduction to the Minimalist Program

Grammars & Parsing, Part 1:

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

Proof Theory for Syntacticians

Informatics 2A: Language Complexity and the. Inf2A: Chomsky Hierarchy

Language contact in East Nusantara

Cross-linguistic aspects in child L2 acquisition

The presence of interpretable but ungrammatical sentences corresponds to mismatches between interpretive and productive parsing.

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

The Structure of Relative Clauses in Maay Maay By Elly Zimmer

Citation for published version (APA): Veenstra, M. J. A. (1998). Formalizing the minimalist program Groningen: s.n.

In Udmurt (Uralic, Russia) possessors bear genitive case except in accusative DPs where they receive ablative case.

Writing the Personal Statement

Control and Boundedness

Using a Native Language Reference Grammar as a Language Learning Tool

Classification. Universals

More Morphology. Problem Set #1 is up: it s due next Thursday (1/19) fieldwork component: Figure out how negation is expressed in your language.

Chapter 4: Valence & Agreement CSLI Publications

Universität Duisburg-Essen

Basic Syntax. Doug Arnold We review some basic grammatical ideas and terminology, and look at some common constructions in English.

L1 and L2 acquisition. Holger Diessel

Argument structure and theta roles

Frequency and pragmatically unmarked word order *

Words come in categories

Derivational and Inflectional Morphemes in Pak-Pak Language

Korean ECM Constructions and Cyclic Linearization

Age Effects on Syntactic Control in. Second Language Learning

Today we examine the distribution of infinitival clauses, which can be

Derivational: Inflectional: In a fit of rage the soldiers attacked them both that week, but lost the fight.

A non-finite period in early Cypriot Greek?

Writing a composition

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 154 ( 2014 )

Parsing of part-of-speech tagged Assamese Texts

On the Notion Determiner

AN ANALYSIS OF GRAMMTICAL ERRORS MADE BY THE SECOND YEAR STUDENTS OF SMAN 5 PADANG IN WRITING PAST EXPERIENCES

LING 329 : MORPHOLOGY

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

Grammar Lesson Plan: Yes/No Questions with No Overt Auxiliary Verbs

Indeterminacy by Underspecification Mary Dalrymple (Oxford), Tracy Holloway King (PARC) and Louisa Sadler (Essex) (9) was: ( case) = nom ( case) = acc

DOWNSTEP IN SUPYIRE* Robert Carlson Societe Internationale de Linguistique, Mali

Beyond constructions:

Using computational modeling in language acquisition research

Getting Started with Deliberate Practice

INTRODUCTION TO MORPHOLOGY Mark C. Baker and Jonathan David Bobaljik. Rutgers and McGill. Draft 6 INFLECTION

L1/L2 Spanish grammars and the pragmatic deficit hypothesis

Participate in expanded conversations and respond appropriately to a variety of conversational prompts

CS 598 Natural Language Processing

FOREWORD.. 5 THE PROPER RUSSIAN PRONUNCIATION. 8. УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) 4 80.

Course Syllabus Advanced-Intermediate Grammar ESOL 0352

Notes on The Sciences of the Artificial Adapted from a shorter document written for course (Deciding What to Design) 1

5 Star Writing Persuasive Essay

Chapter 3: Semi-lexical categories. nor truly functional. As Corver and van Riemsdijk rightly point out, There is more

Language Acquisition by Identical vs. Fraternal SLI Twins * Karin Stromswold & Jay I. Rifkin

Hindi-Urdu Phrase Structure Annotation

Specifying a shallow grammatical for parsing purposes

Modeling full form lexica for Arabic

BANGLA TO ENGLISH TEXT CONVERSION USING OPENNLP TOOLS

ELD CELDT 5 EDGE Level C Curriculum Guide LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT VOCABULARY COMMON WRITING PROJECT. ToolKit

THE FU CTIO OF ACCUSATIVE CASE I MO GOLIA *

5/26/12. Adult L3 learners who are re- learning their L1: heritage speakers A growing trend in American colleges

The Strong Minimalist Thesis and Bounded Optimality

Parallel Evaluation in Stratal OT * Adam Baker University of Arizona

CX 101/201/301 Latin Language and Literature 2015/16

P-4: Differentiate your plans to fit your students

What the National Curriculum requires in reading at Y5 and Y6

CPS122 Lecture: Identifying Responsibilities; CRC Cards. 1. To show how to use CRC cards to identify objects and find responsibilities

5/29/2017. Doran, M.K. (Monifa) RADBOUD UNIVERSITEIT NIJMEGEN

LNGT0101 Introduction to Linguistics

Programma di Inglese

Object clitics, definite articles and genitive possessive clitics in Greek specific language impairment (SLI): deficits and explanations.

(3) Vocabulary insertion targets subtrees (4) The Superset Principle A vocabulary item A associated with the feature set F can replace a subtree X

ROSETTA STONE PRODUCT OVERVIEW

Compositional Semantics

CPS122 Lecture: Identifying Responsibilities; CRC Cards. 1. To show how to use CRC cards to identify objects and find responsibilities

UKLO Round Advanced solutions and marking schemes. 6 The long and short of English verbs [15 marks]

Transcription:

Spring 2010, January 26

English case errors Child errors with subject case Syntactic assumptions about case marking Child subject case errors Default case Among the ways in which kids talk funny are in their use of subject pronouns in English, to wit: (1) a. Him fall down. (Nina 2;3.14, file 17) b. Her have a big mouth. (Nina 2;2.6, file 13) What does this mean with respect to what the children know about syntax? Do they simply not know the right forms for pronouns? Also, this seems to be something that happens primarily in English why not in other languages?

Case forms in English Child errors with subject case Syntactic assumptions about case marking Child subject case errors Default case To begin to address this question, let s take a look at what exactly these errors are. In English, pronouns take on different forms depending on whether they are the subject (he, she, I), a possessor (his, her, my), or something else, like an object (him, her, me). The name for the different forms is case subject case ( nominative, like I), object case ( accusative, like me), possessor case ( genitive, like my).

Case forms more generally Child errors with subject case Syntactic assumptions about case marking Child subject case errors Default case In English, the only place we see this difference between subject and object case is with pronouns other nouns don t sound any different as subjects or objects. But lots of languages do differentiate their nouns in terms of what their syntactic role is. German, Japanese, Korean, Russian,..., it s very common to do this. A common assumption in theoretical syntax is that it s just kind of an accident that English only displays different case forms in its pronouns that is, English is like all the other languages in having different forms for subjects and objects, the only difference is that in English they all sound the same (except for the pronouns). So the subject case form of me is I, and the subject case form of Pat is Pat, while the object case forms are me and Pat.

Case and the syntax Child errors with subject case Syntactic assumptions about case marking Child subject case errors Default case The fact that subject case goes with subjects led to the idea that it is I that is responsible the subject is in the specifier of IP, and the thing in the specifier of IP gets assigned subject case by I. Meanwhile, objects get assigned object case by V. In the more elaborated syntax of IP we talked about last time (where IP was split into TP and AgrP), the subject case function is generally attributed to AgrP, which is what we will assume here.

Non-nominative subjects Child errors with subject case Syntactic assumptions about case marking Child subject case errors Default case Children acquiring English seem to somewhat frequently put subject pronouns in the wrong case. (2) Him fall down. (Nina 2;3.14, file 17) (3) Her have a big mouth. (Nina 2;2.6, file 13) This happens at the same time that children are also using root infinitives, in the ages from 2 to 3. Schütze & Wexler (1996) explored this connection and propose that in fact these phenomena arise from a common source, explaining why they happen together.

Finiteness versus case Child errors with subject case Syntactic assumptions about case marking Child subject case errors Default case They start by recalling an observation made by Loeb & Leonard (1991) that seems to indicate that children don t make these case errors when the verb form is finite. It s only with the root infinitives that case errors arise (and even then, only part of the time). Loeb & Leonard (1991), 7 normally developing children: subject Finite Nonfinite he/she 436 75 him/her 4 28 % non-nom 0.9% 27%

Additional observations Child errors with subject case Syntactic assumptions about case marking Child subject case errors Default case Schütze & Wexler (1996) follow this up by studying the transcripts of three further children (Nina, Peter, and Sarah, from the CHILDES database), and find basically the same pattern, but with some interesting additional observations. There are just about no errors with pronouns apart from subjects. All of the objects children use come out in the correct object case, and the same (just about) with the possessors. The children know the case forms. Almost all of the data support the generalization that when the verb is finite, the subject is nominative. Of the exceptions, most of the non-nominative subjects they found with a finite verb occurred with a past tense verb. (Weird, no?)

Nina, Peter, Sarah Subject case Child errors with subject case Syntactic assumptions about case marking Child subject case errors Default case Nina (1sg) subject Finite Nonfinite he/she 40 45 him/her 2 13 % non-nom 5% 22% Nina (3sg) subject Finite Nonfinite he/she 255 139 him/her 14 120 % non-nom 5% 46% Peter (1sg) subject Finite Nonfinite I 243 29 me/my 3 8 % non-nom 1.2% 22% Sarah (3sgf) subject Finite Nonfinite she 21 24 her 3 14 % non-nom 13% 37%

Child errors with subject case Syntactic assumptions about case marking Child subject case errors Default case Object case pronouns in subject position The vast majority of the errors are object case pronouns in subject position. Why might that be? Schütze & Wexler observe that in sentence fragments, and other places where a full sentence structure isn t expected, adults will use object case. Object case seems to be kind of a default. (4) Me/*I, I like beans. (5) Who did it? Me/*I. (6) Me/*I too.

Default case outside of English Child errors with subject case Syntactic assumptions about case marking Child subject case errors Default case In most other languages, subject case (nominative) seems to be the default. English is kind of weird in this respect. For example, German: (7) Der, den habe ich gesehen. he, him have I seen. He, him I saw. This might explain why subject case errors are so apparent in English but not in other languages. If children are using the default case in subject position, it will be an obvious error in English. In German, the default case is the same as it would have been anyway.

A simple account that doesn t work Separating Agr and T Morphological explanation of infinitive forms of predictions A kind of obvious sort of account might be to suppose that I is responsible for both subject case and for finiteness, and so if I is missing (or in some other way broken ), then we would expect default case in the subject and a root infinitive. The child data doesn t really bear that out, however, although it seems to be kind of in the right direction. For one thing, some of the root infinitives occur with nominative subjects. The simple account predicts they should all have accusative subjects. Second, there are a few accusative subjects with past tense verbs, but those are predicted to have been nominative.

Splitting IP into Agr and T Separating Agr and T Morphological explanation of infinitive forms of predictions Part of Schütze & Wexler s solution to this is to use the Split-IP hypothesis discussed at the end last time: There isn t just I, rather there is an AgrP and a TP. Their basic idea is that for children, either Agr or T could be missing, but it s only when Agr is missing that the subject receives default (object) case. When Agr is there, the subject gets subject case. However, when either Agr or T is missing (with one exception), the verb form is nonfinite. This explains why there are so many nominative subjects with root infinitives: those are cases where T was missing but Agr was there. The other cases (non-nominative subjects) are cases when Agr was missing but T was there.

Separating Agr and T Morphological explanation of infinitive forms of predictions Tense and agreement morphology in English The other part of Schütze & Wexler s story is that the tense and agreement morphology on a verb form is determined by the following rules in English: (8) a. [tns=pres, agr=3sg] s b. [tns=past] ed c. [] /0 That is: if the sentence contains both present (T) and 3rd singular (Agr) features, the verb will end in s (modulo irregulars). Otherwise, if it contains past (T) features, the verb will end in ed (cf. I danced, he danced, you danced). Otherwise, the verb is just a bare form.

Predictions for children Separating Agr and T Morphological explanation of infinitive forms of predictions If the rules in (8) are correct, known by both adults and children, then if T or Agr is missing from the structure, then 3sg present agreement should not surface, since both are required to condition the suffix s. Notice, though, that if Agr is missing (which should yield a non-nominative subject), the verb can still surface in a past tense form, since the conditions for ed do not require Agr. This explains those exceptions where past tense forms appeared with non-nominative subjects those were cases where Agr was missing, but the intended tense was past.

Evaluation Subject case Separating Agr and T Morphological explanation of infinitive forms of predictions thus makes the following predictions, which line up almost perfectly with the observations. T and Agr both included: nominative subject, finite verb. T missing, Agr included: nominative subject, nonfinite verb. T included and past, Agr missing: accusative subject, past tense verb. T included and not past, Agr missing: accusative subject, nonfinite verb. T and Agr both missing: nonfinite verb genitive subject? No configuration: accusative subject, 3sg present verb (him cries).

Why T and Agr might be missing Making lemonade Doing the best you can Explaining the cross-linguistic generalizations (ATOM) works pretty well in English, although it has reduced one question to another: Why are Agr and T sometimes omitted? Related to this is the question of why some languages (the null subject languages) do not seem to exhibit the root infinitive phenomenon, which is supposed to arise from the omission of Agr or T. We ll look at one particularly influential proposal about this, Wexler s (1998) Unique checking constraint.

Why T and Agr might be missing Making lemonade Doing the best you can The syntactic requirements on T and Agr The story begins at the intersection of the VP-internal subject hypothesis and the Split-IP hypothesis we discussed last time. In particular, the assumption that the subject moves first into the specifier of TP and then to the specifier of AgrP. C CP AgrP DP Agr She T+Agr TP will <she> T <T> VP <she> V V take DP notes

Motivating movement Why T and Agr might be missing Making lemonade Doing the best you can The trend in syntactic theory in the past 15 years has been to derive the properties of syntax from a lazy algorithm, a machine that basically does as little work as it can get away with doing. The requirement that the subject has to move first to the specifier of TP and then to the specifier of AgrP is encoded in the theory by saying that T and Agr each have a need for the subject. Since T needs the subject, the subject moves there the movement wouldn t happen if T didn t need it. Since Agr also needs the subject, the subject then moves there. The process of satisfying the need T has is referred to as checking the idea being something like checking an item off of the derivational to-do list.

Two things the subject has to do Why T and Agr might be missing Making lemonade Doing the best you can In an English sentence, both T and Agr need the subject (a DP). So there are two things that the subject must do. First, it must move to the specifier of TP, satisfying the need of T (checking the D-feature of T). Then, it must move to the specifier of AgrP (checking the D-feature of Agr). Wexler s proposal is that what s special about children in the root infinitive stage is that they are subject to the Unique Checking Constraint. Unique checking constraint A subject (DP) can only check one D-feature.

The best of bad options Why T and Agr might be missing Making lemonade Doing the best you can Under Wexler s proposal, the child in the root infinitive stage is basically stuck choosing from among bad options. The assumption is that there is something in the adult grammar that says you need to have T, and something that says you need to have Agr. Tense constraint A main clause must include T. Agr constraint A main clause must include Agr. But it is simply impossible to satisfy all three constraints (the Tense constraint, the Agr constraint, and the Unique checking constraint) at once. You can satisfy any two of them, but not all three.

Any choice is fine Subject case Why T and Agr might be missing Making lemonade Doing the best you can Given the constraints the child is under, one constraint must be violated, and the one which is violated is presumed to be chosen more or less arbitrarily. Satisfy Tense constraint and UCC, violate Agr constraint: AgrP is omitted. Satisfy Agr constraint and UCC, violate Tense constraint: TP is omitted. Satisfy Tense and Agr constraints, violate UCC: Adult form.

Minimize violations Why T and Agr might be missing Making lemonade Doing the best you can Wexler also proposes (also kind of in keeping with the lazy idea about syntax) that syntax will not violate more constraints than it has to. A child subject to the UCC acquiring English must violate at least one of these constraints (Tense constraint, Agr constraint, UCC), but it will never violate two of them, since it is possible to get away with violating just one. Minimize Violations Given two representations, choose the one that violates as few grammatical constraints as possible. If two representations violate the same number of constraints, then either one may be chosen.

Why T and Agr might be missing Making lemonade Doing the best you can The connection with null subject languages Wexler also proposes that this allows for an explanation of why Spanish, Italian, and Catalan do not seem to have a root infinitive stage. The idea, essentially, is that what differentiates null subject languages from non-null subject languages is Agr does not have a D-feature to check (Agr doesn t need the subject to move to its specifier). If Agr doesn t need D, then the UCC creates no conflict: The Agr constraint and Tense constraint can both be satisfied, at the same time as the UCC (the subject can check the D-feature of T and that takes care of everything). Minimize Violations then ensures that neither Agr nor T will ever be omitted.

Root infinitives in wh-questions Wh-questions Subject case errors Beyond root infinitives The ATOM/UCC proposal does not seem to make any prediction about the availability of root infinitives in wh-questions. Last time we saw that (a) root infinitives are rare or nonexistent in French and Dutch, and (b) the truncation model has a ready explanation for this: a root infinitive necessarily lacks CP, so of course there would be no wh-questions. Under the ATOM/UCC model, there is no clear reason why lacking either T or Agr would result in the inability to form a wh-question. On the other hand, English does seem to have wh-questions with root infinitives, unlike Dutch and French. (9) Where train go? (Adam 2;4)

Subject case results Wh-questions Subject case errors Beyond root infinitives On the flip side, the ATOM/UCC model seems to do a better job explaining the subject case results than the truncation model. Under the truncation model, we should only see (a) both AgrP and TP, or (b) TP without AgrP. Never AgrP without TP. So, this leaves the nominative subjects with root infinitives unaccounted for. (And even if the proposed order of AgrP and TP were reversed, this would then predict never TP without AgrP, so the past tense exceptions are left unexplained).

The UCC in other domains Wh-questions Subject case errors Beyond root infinitives Although we won t talk about it yet, we will come back later in the semester to some other predictions that the UCC makes as well, which seem at least plausibly borne out. These have to do with the production of object clitics in languages with object agreement on the verb (like French), and maybe also certain facts about objects and negation in Korean.

Two accounts of the optional infinitive stage ATOM/UCC: Agr is tied to subject case, verbal morphology depends on features of both Agr and T, and the UCC can cause Agr or T to be omitted. Null subject languages have an Agr with no needs, so the UCC can always be satisfied. Truncation: Children can (relatively arbitrarily) stop building their trees short of CP, with the consequences that go with that. Null subject languages must always have T. There are pros, there are cons. At the moment, maybe the UCC is winning, but we still have the question raised above about wh-questions.