Developmental Continuity in Infants Early Lexical Representations. Jie Ren and James L. Morgan Brown University

Similar documents
The Perception of Nasalized Vowels in American English: An Investigation of On-line Use of Vowel Nasalization in Lexical Access

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

Universal contrastive analysis as a learning principle in CAPT

Learners Use Word-Level Statistics in Phonetic Category Acquisition

raıs Factors affecting word learning in adults: A comparison of L2 versus L1 acquisition /r/ /aı/ /s/ /r/ /aı/ /s/ = individual sound

Linking object names and object categories: Words (but not tones) facilitate object categorization in 6- and 12-month-olds

Phonological encoding in speech production

Revisiting the role of prosody in early language acquisition. Megha Sundara UCLA Phonetics Lab

Florida Reading Endorsement Alignment Matrix Competency 1

Summary / Response. Karl Smith, Accelerations Educational Software. Page 1 of 8

Infants learn phonotactic regularities from brief auditory experience

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

Running head: DELAY AND PROSPECTIVE MEMORY 1

DIBELS Next BENCHMARK ASSESSMENTS

Lecture 2: Quantifiers and Approximation

Fribourg, Fribourg, Switzerland b LEAD CNRS UMR 5022, Université de Bourgogne, Dijon, France

An Evaluation of the Interactive-Activation Model Using Masked Partial-Word Priming. Jason R. Perry. University of Western Ontario. Stephen J.

Visual processing speed: effects of auditory input on

Running head: FAST MAPPING SKILLS IN THE DEVELOPING LEXICON. Fast Mapping Skills in the Developing Lexicon. Lisa Gershkoff-Stowe. Indiana University

Dyslexia/dyslexic, 3, 9, 24, 97, 187, 189, 206, 217, , , 367, , , 397,

Abstract Rule Learning for Visual Sequences in 8- and 11-Month-Olds

Effects of Vocabulary and Phonotactic Probability on 2-Year-Olds Nonword Repetition

Falling on Sensitive Ears

1. REFLEXES: Ask questions about coughing, swallowing, of water as fast as possible (note! Not suitable for all

The Oregon Literacy Framework of September 2009 as it Applies to grades K-3

Correspondence between the DRDP (2015) and the California Preschool Learning Foundations. Foundations (PLF) in Language and Literacy

**Note: this is slightly different from the original (mainly in format). I would be happy to send you a hard copy.**

9.85 Cognition in Infancy and Early Childhood. Lecture 7: Number

SOUND STRUCTURE REPRESENTATION, REPAIR AND WELL-FORMEDNESS: GRAMMAR IN SPOKEN LANGUAGE PRODUCTION. Adam B. Buchwald

To appear in The TESOL encyclopedia of ELT (Wiley-Blackwell) 1 RECASTING. Kazuya Saito. Birkbeck, University of London

NAME: East Carolina University PSYC Developmental Psychology Dr. Eppler & Dr. Ironsmith

Author: Justyna Kowalczys Stowarzyszenie Angielski w Medycynie (PL) Feb 2015

English Language and Applied Linguistics. Module Descriptions 2017/18

Perceptual processing of partially and fully assimilated words in French

Effects of Open-Set and Closed-Set Task Demands on Spoken Word Recognition

THE INFLUENCE OF TASK DEMANDS ON FAMILIARITY EFFECTS IN VISUAL WORD RECOGNITION: A COHORT MODEL PERSPECTIVE DISSERTATION

Common Core Exemplar for English Language Arts and Social Studies: GRADE 1

Good Enough Language Processing: A Satisficing Approach

A joint model of word segmentation and meaning acquisition through crosssituational

Stages of Literacy Ros Lugg

Linguistics 220 Phonology: distributions and the concept of the phoneme. John Alderete, Simon Fraser University

The New Theory of Disuse Predicts Retrieval Enhanced Suggestibility (RES)

Phonological Encoding in Sentence Production

Strategy Abandonment Effects in Cued Recall

learning collegiate assessment]

Different Task Type and the Perception of the English Interdental Fricatives

Downloaded on T18:40:04Z. Title. Using parent report to assess early lexical production in children exposed to more than one language

A Case-Based Approach To Imitation Learning in Robotic Agents

Is Event-Based Prospective Memory Resistant to Proactive Interference?

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

Lexical phonology. Marc van Oostendorp. December 6, Until now, we have presented phonological theory as if it is a monolithic

Content Language Objectives (CLOs) August 2012, H. Butts & G. De Anda

A Bootstrapping Model of Frequency and Context Effects in Word Learning

Eye Movements in Speech Technologies: an overview of current research

SLINGERLAND: A Multisensory Structured Language Instructional Approach

Language Acquisition Chart

Developing phonological awareness: Is there a bilingual advantage?

Clinical Application of the Mean Babbling Level and Syllable Structure Level

Automatization and orthographic development in second language visual word recognition

Communicative signals promote abstract rule learning by 7-month-old infants

JSLHR. Research Article. Lexical Characteristics of Expressive Vocabulary in Toddlers With Autism Spectrum Disorder

Cued Recall From Image and Sentence Memory: A Shift From Episodic to Identical Elements Representation

Considerations for Aligning Early Grades Curriculum with the Common Core

The Effect of Discourse Markers on the Speaking Production of EFL Students. Iman Moradimanesh

GOLD Objectives for Development & Learning: Birth Through Third Grade

The Effects of Super Speed 100 on Reading Fluency. Jennifer Thorne. University of New England

The Representation of Concrete and Abstract Concepts: Categorical vs. Associative Relationships. Jingyi Geng and Tatiana T. Schnur

Books Effective Literacy Y5-8 Learning Through Talk Y4-8 Switch onto Spelling Spelling Under Scrutiny

STUDIES WITH FABRICATED SWITCHBOARD DATA: EXPLORING SOURCES OF MODEL-DATA MISMATCH

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

Sensitivity to second language argument structure

Age-Related Differences in Communication and Audience Design

Learning and Teaching

Learning By Asking: How Children Ask Questions To Achieve Efficient Search

Arabic Orthography vs. Arabic OCR

Philosophy of Literacy Education. Becoming literate is a complex step by step process that begins at birth. The National

Effect of Word Complexity on L2 Vocabulary Learning

Comparison Between Three Memory Tests: Cued Recall, Priming and Saving Closed-Head Injured Patients and Controls

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

Atypical Prosodic Structure as an Indicator of Reading Level and Text Difficulty

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Perceived speech rate: the effects of. articulation rate and speaking style in spontaneous speech. Jacques Koreman. Saarland University

Evolution of Symbolisation in Chimpanzees and Neural Nets

Table of Contents. Introduction Choral Reading How to Use This Book...5. Cloze Activities Correlation to TESOL Standards...

NCEO Technical Report 27

Eyebrows in French talk-in-interaction

A Stochastic Model for the Vocabulary Explosion

Longitudinal family-risk studies of dyslexia: why. develop dyslexia and others don t.

Sex Differences in Self-Efficacy and Attributions: Influence of Performance Feedback

THE INFORMATION SYSTEMS ANALYST EXAM AS A PROGRAM ASSESSMENT TOOL: PRE-POST TESTS AND COMPARISON TO THE MAJOR FIELD TEST

2,1 .,,, , %, ,,,,,,. . %., Butterworth,)?.(1989; Levelt, 1989; Levelt et al., 1991; Levelt, Roelofs & Meyer, 1999

Manual Response Dynamics Reflect Rapid Integration of Intonational Information during Reference Resolution

Pobrane z czasopisma New Horizons in English Studies Data: 18/11/ :52:20. New Horizons in English Studies 1/2016

Probabilistic principles in unsupervised learning of visual structure: human data and a model

LEXICAL CATEGORY ACQUISITION VIA NONADJACENT DEPENDENCIES IN CONTEXT: EVIDENCE OF DEVELOPMENTAL CHANGE AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES.

Developing a concrete-pictorial-abstract model for negative number arithmetic

Perceptual foundations of bilingual acquisition in infancy

The phonological grammar is probabilistic: New evidence pitting abstract representation against analogy

On the Formation of Phoneme Categories in DNN Acoustic Models

Build on students informal understanding of sharing and proportionality to develop initial fraction concepts.

Transcription:

Developmental Continuity in Infants Early Lexical Representations Jie Ren and James L. Morgan Brown University 1 Introduction As adults, we are able to efficiently process the phonology of our native language, and are thus able to focus on the linguistic dimensions that are critical for distinguishing among potential word candidates in the lexicon (e.g. /t/ and /p/ in cat and cap ). Moreover, we can easily apply our phonological knowledge to identify new lexical entries when tokens differ sufficiently along phonological dimensions and fail to be attested as existing words in the lexicon. To infant learners, phonological knowledge is of equal importance. To build a lexicon of the language they are learning, infants need to create stable word representations in memory, learn the mappings between words and their referents, and differentiate words from one another. Previous research has sometimes suggested that early lexical representations are much less detailed than mature representations (e.g. Stager & Werker, 1997). However, recent findings indicate that infants are not only sensitive to one-feature mispronunciations of familiar words (e.g. Swingley & Aslin, 2000) but also display graded sensitivity to varying degrees of onset mispronunciations of familiar words. White & Morgan (2008) found that as mispronunciations increasingly deviated by one (/gog/), two (/kog/), or three features (/sog/) from the correct forms (/dog/), infants proportional looking to a referent of the familiar target word (e.g. the dog) versus a referent with no known label decreased in a graded fashion. In this chapter, we present four studies showing from different perspectives that infants early lexical representations are specified to the same degree and in the same manner as those of adults. 2 Experiments 2.1 Experiment 1 This experiment used White & Morgan s procedure to test whether their findings applied to coda as well as onset mispronunciations. 2.1.1 Subjects Thirty-two 19-month-olds (mean age = 586.5 days) subjects were tested. 2.1.2 Stimuli The familiar stimuli comprised a set of words that are comprehended by at least 50% of infants by 14 months, according to the MacArthur CDI norms (Dale & Fenson, 1996). In each trial, infants saw two images, one depicting a referent of a familiar word, the other depicting a referent with no known label (to 19-month-

olds). An example stimulus pair is depicted in Figure 1. Figure 1: Sample visual stimulus pair. Of 18 total trials, five had the familiar object s label pronounced correctly, three had familiar object s label pronounced withh a one-feature change (place) in the coda consonant, threee had two-feature changes (place + voicing) in the coda consonant, three had three-feature changes (placee + voicing + manner) in the coda consonant, and four novel trials in which the unfamiliar object was named (e.g., /keg/). Novel trials were included to provide a baseline to measuree looking behavior in the context of a completely unfamiliar word. Each of the 18 trials involved a unique item. Degree of mispronunciation of items was counterbalanced across four groups of infants. Examples of mispronunciations are given in Table 1. Pronunciation Condition Correct 1 feature (Place) 2 feature (Place + Voicing) 3 feature (Place + Voicing + Manner) Example Target /d k/ /d t/ /d d/ /d z/ Table1: Sample audio conditions of Experiment 1. All mispronunciations resulted in non-words or in words judged unlikely to be familiar to toddlers at this age. 2.1.3 Procedure An intermodal preferential looking procedure (IPLP) was used, in which one object with a known label and a second object with no known label were displayed on two horizontally opposed screens (See Figure 1). Each trial began with a salience phase, during whichh two objects were displayed silently for 4 seconds. The infants attention was attracted too midline to avoid contingencies between side of fixation at the end of the salience period and at the beginning of the test period. After recentralization, the experimenter initiated the test phase. During the test phase, the audio stimulus (Where s the X?) was played, then immediately after the coda of the target word, the same two visual stimuli were presented simultaneously for 8 seconds. Infants looking was recordedd and then coded off-line frame by frame.

2.1.4 Results and discussion The dependent measure was the change in subjects looking proportions to the familiar object between the (silent) salience phase and the test phase. Comparison across test and salience phases allowed us to use each stimulus pair as its own control, thereby controlling for any inherent preference for a particular stimulus in each pairing. As expected, there was significant linear trend of decreased looking to familiar objects with increasing severity of coda mispronunciation: F s (1,124) =4.083, p<.05, F i (1, 44) =4.109, p< <.05, replicating White & Morgan (2008) s findings for onsets. Test-minus-salience difference scores are depicted in Figure 2. Figure 2: Mean Differences of Looking Proportions. Condition is represented on the x-axis. The y-axis represents the difference between proportion looking at the familiar object in the test phase after a label was heard and proportion looking at the familiar object in the silent salience phase. This pattern of results shows that 19-month-olds graded sensitivity to varying degrees of specify coda consonants in their lexical representations and have word coda mispronuncia ations. Since both 14-month-olds (Swingley and Aslin, 2002) and 22-month-olds (Bailey and Plunkett, 2002) are sensitive to one-feature mispronuncia ations of familiar words, together with these findings and those of White & Morgan (2008), we may infer that infants consistently have detailed lexical representation. 2.2 Experiments 2 & 3 As claimed by certain phonology theories and supported by some psycholinguistic research, adults lexical representations may not always be fully

specified. In particular, unmarked coronal consonants (/d/, /t/, /l/, /n/) may be underspecified whereas non-coronal consonants (/b/. /k/, /m/, /r/) are fully specified. For instance Lahiri & Reetz (2010) found less priming in adults from non-coronal coronal mispronunciation changes (/dag/ /dad/), than from coronal non-coronal changes (/kæt/ /kæk/). If infants representations are similarly underspecified, they should also show such asymmetrical patterns in mispronunciation tasks, depending on whether the mispronunciations are from underspecified to specified segments or from specified to underspecified segments. Experiments 2 and 3 address this question for consonantal onsets and codas, respectively. 2.2.1 Subjects Twenty-two 19-month-olds were tested in Experiment 2; twenty-six were tested in Experiment 3. 2.2.2 Stimuli As in Experiment 1, the visual stimuli in each trial comprised a familiar object and an unfamiliar object. Of the 18 trials, three were correct underspecified /d k/ or /kæt/), three were mispronounced underspecified (/g k/ or /kæk/), three were correct specified (/kæt/ or /d k/) three were mispronounced specified trials (/tæt/ or /d t/, and the remaining six were correct and novel fillers. Mispronunciations only involved changes in place of articulation. 2.2.3 Procedure Same as Experiment 1. 2.2.4 Results and discussion As in Experiment 1, the dependent measure was the change of looking proportion to familiar objects between salience and test phases. To explore the effects of specification and mispronunciation, ANOVAs were performed on the four conditions (correct-mispronounced*specified-underspecified) for each experiment. For both onsets and codas (see Figure 3), we found significant effects of pronunciation (correct-mispronounced): Fo s (1, 21) = 10.92, p <.005; Fo i (1, 10) = 16.02, p <.005. Fc s (1, 21) = 0.05, p = 0.82; Fc i (1, 10) = 0.32, p =.58. However, the pronunciation*specification interactions were not significant either for onsets Fo s (1, 21) = 0.003, p = 0.96; Fo i (1, 10) = 0.21, p =.65, or for codas Fc s (1, 25) = 0.04, p =.84; Fc i (1, 10) = 0.84, p =.85. Contrary to underspecification accounts for both infants (Dijkstra & Fikkert, 2010) and adults (Lahiri & Reetz, 2010), our findings indicated that 19-montholds symmetrically specify both coronal and noncoronal consonants in their lexical representation. Possibly infants at this age have more detailed information than needed in their lexical representation, and they need more experience to learn which segments can be safely underspecified. On the other hand, most previous studies on underspecification in adults used priming tasks to examine effects of

Figure 3: Mean Differences of Looking Proportions for onsets and codas. Pronunciation is represented on the x-axis. The y-axis represents the difference between proportion looking at the familiar object during the test phase after a label was heard vs. during the silent salience phase. Blue lines represent words with specified consonants and red lines represent words with underspecified consonants.

mispronuncia ations. Priming studies necessarily introduce lags between primes and targets, and it is thus unclear whether results such as those reported by Lahiri and Reetz are perceptual or post-perceptu ual. To disentangle this,, in Experiment 4, we ested adults on their immediate responses to mispronunciations using an eyerepresent tracking procedure comparable to our testing of infants. 2.3 Experiment 4 The purpose of this study was to test whetherr adults asymmetrically coronal and non-coronal word onsets in an on-line word recognition task. 2.3.1 Subjects Twenty-four mono-lingual English speaking 37 were tested. adults with the age range of 19-2.3.2 Stimuli Familiar stimuli comprised a set of highly frequent objects with highly familiar names; the distractors comprised a set off unusual or nonce objects, whose names were unlikely to be known. An example stimulus pairr is depicted in Figure 4. Figure 4: Sample visual stimulus pair. There were 108 trials, in which 18 were correct underspecified trials (/d k/), 18 were mispronounced underspecified (/g k/), 18 were correct specified pronunciations (/bæθ/) 18 mispronounced specified trials (/dæθ/), and the remaining 36 were correct and novel fillers. As in Experiments 2 & 3, mispronuncia ations only involved changes in place of articulation. All mispronuncia ations resulted in non-words. 2.2.3 Procedures Participants looked at a screen in which one object with a known label and a second object with unknown label were horizontally displayed (See Figure 4). As in Experiment 1, each trial began with a 4-second silentt display of the two objects. A center fixation display then attracted participants attention to midline. After 1.5 seconds, the audio stimuluss for the target word wass played; immediately after the coda of the target word, the same two visual stimulii were presented for 8 seconds. Participants looking was recorded and coded online by the eye-tracker.

2.2.4 Results and discussion The dependent measure was subjects looking proportions to target objects. As in Experiments 2 & 3, a 2*2 ANOVA showed a significant effect of mispronunciation: F s (1,23)=31.14, p<.005, F i (1,140)=24.41, p<.001. However, no significant pronunciation by specification interaction was found F s (1, 23) = 1.52, p = 0.23; F i (1, 140) = 0.17, p = 0.68 (See Figure 5). Adults showed symmetrical sensitivity to both directions of mispronunciation changes during on-line word recognition. This is contrary to the predictions of underspecification accounts and conflict with previous findings on underspecification, which may have tapped different levels of representation than did our tasks. Underspec Spec Proportional Looking to Target 0.45 0.43 0.41 0.39 0.37 0.35 0.33 0.31 0.29 0.27 0.25 Correct Pronunciation Mispro Figure 5: Adults Mean Proportional Looking to the Target. Pronunciation is represented on the x-axis. The y-axis represents the proportional looking to the target object. The blue line represents words with underspecified consonants and the red line represents words with specified consonants. 3 General Discussion Like adults, infants, at least by 19 months, have detailed phonological representations of both onset and coda consonants: in each case, increased severity of mispronunciations increasingly impairs recognition of familiar words. Meanwhile, like adults, infants symmetrically specify both coronal and

noncoronal phonemes in on-line word recognition. Therefore, the findings in our current study challenge holistic hypotheses of lexical development by showing that from different perspectives, infants lexical details are of adult-like sophistication. Rather than developmental discontinuity, there appears to be substantial developmental continuity in lexical representation. References Bailey, T. M., & Plunkett, K. 2002. Phonological specificity in early words. Cognitive Development, 17, 1265 1282. Dale, P. S., & Fenson, L. 1996. Lexical development norms for young children. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 28, 125 127. Dijkstra. N. & Fikkert. P. 2010. Universal constraints on the discrimination of Place of Articulation? Asymmetries in the discrimination of 'paan' and 'taan' by 6-month-old Dutch infants. BUCLD 35. November, 2010. Lahiri, A., & Reetz, H. 2010. Distinctive features: Phonological underspecification in representation and processing. Journal of Phonetics, 38(1), 44-59. Stager, C. L., & Werker, J. F. 1997. Infants listen for more phonetic detail in speech perception than in word learning tasks. Nature, 388, 381 382. Swingley, D. 2009. Onsets and codas in 1.5-year-olds word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language. 60, 252-269. Swingley, D. and Aslin, R.N. 2000. Spoken word recognition and lexical representation in very young children. Cognition, 76, 147-166. White, K. S., & Morgan, J. L. 2008. Subsegmental detail in infants early lexical representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 114-132.