Phonological Theories

Similar documents
Parallel Evaluation in Stratal OT * Adam Baker University of Arizona

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Lexical phonology. Marc van Oostendorp. December 6, Until now, we have presented phonological theory as if it is a monolithic

Precedence Constraints and Opacity

The presence of interpretable but ungrammatical sentences corresponds to mismatches between interpretive and productive parsing.

An argument from speech pathology

The analysis starts with the phonetic vowel and consonant charts based on the dataset:

Pobrane z czasopisma New Horizons in English Studies Data: 18/11/ :52:20. New Horizons in English Studies 1/2016

Listener-oriented phonology

Rhythm-typology revisited.

Markedness and Complex Stops: Evidence from Simplification Processes 1. Nick Danis Rutgers University

**Note: this is slightly different from the original (mainly in format). I would be happy to send you a hard copy.**

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Linguistics 220 Phonology: distributions and the concept of the phoneme. John Alderete, Simon Fraser University

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

A Cross-language Corpus for Studying the Phonetics and Phonology of Prominence

SOUND STRUCTURE REPRESENTATION, REPAIR AND WELL-FORMEDNESS: GRAMMAR IN SPOKEN LANGUAGE PRODUCTION. Adam B. Buchwald

Derivations (MP) and Evaluations (OT) *

The Odd-Parity Parsing Problem 1 Brett Hyde Washington University May 2008

Word Stress and Intonation: Introduction

LING 329 : MORPHOLOGY

I propose an analysis of thorny patterns of reduplication in the unrelated languages Saisiyat

Som and Optimality Theory

Phonological Processing for Urdu Text to Speech System

Universal contrastive analysis as a learning principle in CAPT

Partial Class Behavior and Nasal Place Assimilation*

The optimal placement of up and ab A comparison 1

Acoustic correlates of stress and their use in diagnosing syllable fusion in Tongan. James White & Marc Garellek UCLA

Introduction to HPSG. Introduction. Historical Overview. The HPSG architecture. Signature. Linguistic Objects. Descriptions.

Constraining X-Bar: Theta Theory

5 Minimalism and Optimality Theory

Speech Recognition at ICSI: Broadcast News and beyond

Underlying Representations

A Fact in Historical Phonology from the Viewpoint of Generative Phonology: The Underlying Schwa in Old English

On the nature of voicing assimilation(s)

Handout #8. Neutralization

Manner assimilation in Uyghur

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

Natural Language Processing. George Konidaris

Syntax Parsing 1. Grammars and parsing 2. Top-down and bottom-up parsing 3. Chart parsers 4. Bottom-up chart parsing 5. The Earley Algorithm

Ternary rhythm in alignment theory René Kager Utrecht University

Consonant-Vowel Unity in Element Theory*

Norwegian stress and quantity: The implications of loanwords

Consonants: articulation and transcription

Towards a Robuster Interpretive Parsing

Undergraduate Program Guide. Bachelor of Science. Computer Science DEPARTMENT OF COMPUTER SCIENCE and ENGINEERING

Florida Reading Endorsement Alignment Matrix Competency 1

The Ohio State University. Colleges of the Arts and Sciences. Bachelor of Science Degree Requirements. The Aim of the Arts and Sciences

Language Acquisition by Identical vs. Fraternal SLI Twins * Karin Stromswold & Jay I. Rifkin

An Introduction to the Minimalist Program

Idaho Public Schools

Acquiring Competence from Performance Data

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

Lexical specification of tone in North Germanic

THE PHONOLOGICAL WORD IN STANDARD MALA Y

ADDIS ABABA UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF GRADUATE STUDIES MODELING IMPROVED AMHARIC SYLLBIFICATION ALGORITHM

Portuguese Vowel Harmony: A Comparative Analysis and the Superiority of Autosegmental Representations

Vowel Reduction in Russian: A Unified Account of Standard, Dialectal, and Dissimilative Patterns *

Quantitative Reasoning in Linguistics

Firms and Markets Saturdays Summer I 2014

1. Introduction. 2. The OMBI database editor

Context-Sensitive Bidirectional OT: a New Approach to Russian Aspect

Program in Linguistics. Academic Year Assessment Report

Multiple case assignment and the English pseudo-passive *

Tutorial on Paradigms

Is French Optimal?* 1 Introduction. 2 Two output-driven processes

WORK OF LEADERS GROUP REPORT

On Human Computer Interaction, HCI. Dr. Saif al Zahir Electrical and Computer Engineering Department UBC

Learning Methods for Fuzzy Systems

Hueber Worterbuch Learner's Dictionary: Deutsch Als Fremdsprache / German-English / English-German Deutsch- Englisch / Englisch-Deutsch By Olaf

The Strong Minimalist Thesis and Bounded Optimality

Frequency and pragmatically unmarked word order *

Books Effective Literacy Y5-8 Learning Through Talk Y4-8 Switch onto Spelling Spelling Under Scrutiny

Canadian raising with language-specific weighted constraints Joe Pater, University of Massachusetts Amherst

Joan Bybee, Phonology and Language Use. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2001,

Spanish progressive aspect in stochastic OT

Generating Test Cases From Use Cases

Reduce the Failure Rate of the Screwing Process with Six Sigma Approach

A simpler view of Danish stød

Speech Recognition using Acoustic Landmarks and Binary Phonetic Feature Classifiers

On the Rhythmic Vowel Deletion in Maga Rukai *

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

Lip reading: Japanese vowel recognition by tracking temporal changes of lip shape

Susanne J. Jekat

Demonstration of problems of lexical stress on the pronunciation Turkish English teachers and teacher trainees by computer

AGS THE GREAT REVIEW GAME FOR PRE-ALGEBRA (CD) CORRELATED TO CALIFORNIA CONTENT STANDARDS

5. Margi (Chadic, Nigeria): H, L, R (Williams 1973, Hoffmann 1963)

Radical CV Phonology: the locational gesture *

ONLINE COURSES. Flexibility to Meet Middle and High School Students at Their Point of Need

YOUR FUTURE IN IB. Why is the International Baccalaureate a great choice for you? Mrs. Debbie Woolard IB Director Marietta High School

Derivational and Inflectional Morphemes in Pak-Pak Language

Using SAM Central With iread

A Bayesian Model of Stress Assignment in Reading

COMPUTATIONAL COMPLEXITY OF LEFT-ASSOCIATIVE GRAMMAR

Specification and Evaluation of Machine Translation Toy Systems - Criteria for laboratory assignments

Linking Task: Identifying authors and book titles in verbose queries

REPRESENTATIONAL HANDLING OF POZNAŃ-CRACOW VOICING IN GOVERNMENT PHONOLOGY

(3) Vocabulary insertion targets subtrees (4) The Superset Principle A vocabulary item A associated with the feature set F can replace a subtree X

IDENTIFYING AND DESCRIBING HIGH QUALITY SECONDARY SCHOOL SPANISH INSTRUCTION. Greg Duncan, InterPrep Myriam Met, Consultant

Speech Segmentation Using Probabilistic Phonetic Feature Hierarchy and Support Vector Machines

Transcription:

Optimality Theory Session 7

Grammar as an Input-Output Mechanism A (partial) grammar in the generative tradition formally captures the observable output in its relation to (an assumed) input. The assumption is that not all observable forms (surface forms) are a direct reflection of the input (underlying forms) In German, all coda obstruents a voiceless even if the word is related to a morpheme that otherwise has a voiced obstruent: Input: (German /ba:d/) = /ba:t/ In English (or Russian, or Bulgarian) vowels change their quality (are reduced) if the syllable they bear is unstressed: Input: (English /p6litikæl/) = /p@"litik@l/ Input: (Belorussian /kola/) = /ka"la/

OT SPE The wish to capture the Input - Output relations is part of all generative grammars. The differences lie in the assumptions about how the Output is determined. In classical (linear) Generative Phonology the Output is derived from the Input by applying an ordered set of rules. Optimality theory expresses the Output as the result of an ordered set of constraints which only allow certain forms to exist on the surface. This way of looking at it stresses the parallels between the two approaches. Accounts of OT tend to stress the differences.

What s special about OT? The goal of Optimality Theory is to present Universal Constraints (i.e. they operate in all languages; there are no language-specific constraints). All constraints are essentially violable (= they can be ignored ) These should explain both language-specific observations and differences between (a) speaking styles, (b) dialects, (c) different languages. A different set of constraints may apply in different languages (but they are all selected from the same pool of universal constraints). The same constraints may apply in a different order (thus changing the Output that appears on the surface). The Input is (of course?) different from one language to another (because the underlying forms of the lexicon comprise the Input)

Components of an OT grammar (1) Input (Lexicon ): The lexicon contains the lexical representations (underlying forms) of the morphemes and supplies the Input for the Generator. (the phonological form of the morphemes is language-specific) Generator: The Generator produces a potentially infinite number of Output candidates: Gen(Input) {K 1, K 2, K 3,..., K n } and passes them to the Evaluator. Evaluator: The Evaluator consists of a set of ordered Constraints: {B 1 B 2... B n } and evaluates the Output candidates with regard to their harmony values (the degree to which they comply with the constraints). It selects the optimal candidate. The selection ist unique:, there is one optimal candidate as Output: Eval({K 1, K 2, K 3,..., K n }) Output.

Components of an OT grammar (2) Output: If two candidates both comply with several constraints, there must be further (lower-order) constraints which differentiate between the two and select one candidate. If two candidates cannot be differentiated, they are identical.

The Architecture of Optimality Theory

OT-Representations (Tableaux) /Y/ A B C x * * y * z * Top left corner Underlying Representation; Candidates generated by Gen (x, y, z) one per line; Columns indicate the order of dominance (relative strength) of the constraints (A, B, C); Solid lines indicate a hierarchy; dashed lines idicate equal rank; Constraint satisfaction is signalled by an empty cell; Asterisk indicates constrain violation; Exclamation mark! (or *) signifies a fatal violation (non-optimality); Grey shading = irrelevant; Rshows the optimal candidate.

Basic OT concepts Constraints Conflict Dominance Optimality

Constraints A Constraint is a structural condition, which can either be satisfied by an Output-Form or it can be violated. There are three types of constraints: Faithfulness constraints Markedness constraints Alignment constraints

Markedness Constraints Markedness constraints require the Output Form to fulfill certain well-formedness criteria. These may be positively or negatively formulated, so we distinguish between: Negative constraints Vowels are not nasalized (*VNASAL) Syllables have no coda (NOCODA or *CODA) Coda obstruents are not voiced (*VOICECODA) Positive constraints Sonorant must be voiced (SONVOICE) Syllables must have an onset (ONSET) Syllables must have a peak (PEAK)

Faithfulness Constraints In contrast to Markedness-Constraints, which only refer to the Output Form, Faithfulness Constraints require the OutputForms to retain the properties of the Input (the underlying lexical form). In the ideal case, the Output is identical to the Input. In the Output all segments of the Input must be preserved (no elision) The Output must preserve the linear sequence of all Input segments (no metathesis) Output segments must have a corresondence in the Input (no epenthesis) Output segments und Input segments must have identical feature values (IDENT-IO feature or PRESERVE-IO feature ).

Alignment Constraints Alignment constraints create connections between different forms: Example: A-R stem-σ = All stems end at the right-hand edge of a syllable. Cf. Liaison in French (as a violation): on est au salon [ o.ne.to.sal o]

Optimality: Dominance and Conflict Optimality: An Output is optimal when it best fulfils the hierarchically ordered set of constraints, i.e. when it has the least serious violations. Conflict: Constraints compete with one another. In particular, there is a fundamental conflict between Markedness constraints and Faithfulness constraints. Dominance: The higher-ranking of two conflicting constraints dominates the lower-ranking one.

Constraint Interaction: an example from Belorussian Goal: No mid vowels in unstressed syllables! Markedness constraint: LIC-MID/STRESS: Mid vowels are only allowed when they are stressed. The following Faithfulness constraint conflicts with it: IDENT-IO[low] or PRESERVE[low]: The specification of the feature [low] for an Input segment must be preserved in the corresponding Output segment. IDENT-IO[high] or PRESERVE[high]: The specification of the feature [high] for an Input segment must be preserved in the corresponding Output segment. These two constraint (types) are in conflict with each other.

Constraint Interaction The underlying lexical form (Input) is /kola/ The Generator produces the candidates [kola], [kala], [kila], [kula], [kela]. Constraint ranking: In Belorussian the feature [low] replaces [mid], so as to avoid mid vowels. Therefore Belorussian will tolerate the violation of PRESERVE[low]. However, LIC-MID/STRESS and PRESERVE[high] will never be violated. Therefore the ranking is: LIC-MID/STRESS PRESERVE[high] PRESERVE[low]

Vowel Reduction - Belorussian The Constraint Tableau: /kola/ LIC MID/STRESSPRESERVE[high]PRESERVE[low] [ka"la] * [ku"la] *! [ki"la] *! [ko"la] *! [ke"la] *!

Factorial Typology: a non-linguistic Example Let us assume the following Universal Constraints: CAT = Keep the cat in. WINDOW = Keep the window open. DOOR = Keep the door open. Possible constraint ordering: Results of each order: 1. CAT WINDOW DOOR Cat inside; window and door closed. 2. CAT DOOR WINDOW Cat inside; window and door closed. 3. WINDOW CAT DOOR Cat outside; window open, door closed. 4. WINDOW DOOR CAT Cat outside; window and door open. 5. DOOR CAT WINDOW Cat outside; door open, window closed. 6. DOOR WINDOW CAT Cat outside; door and window open.

Factorial Typology: a commentary (factorial 3) 3! = 6 i.e., there are 6 possible Grammars. But Grammars 1 and 2 generate the same Output; Grammars 4 and 6 also. How many Output conditions are there? Is the ordering of DOOR and WINDOW important?

Factorial Typology: an exercise (1) Create a factorial typology. A 5-Vowel System with vowel reduction is assumed. 3 constraints: LIC-MID/STRESS PRESERVE[low] PRESERVE[high] = 6 possible orderings.

Factorial Typology: an exercise (2) LIC-MID/STRESS PRESERVE[Low] PRESERVE[High] LIC-MID/STRESS PRESERVE[High] PRESERVE[Low] = Belorussian PRESERVE[Low] PRESERVE[High] LIC-MID/STRESS PRESERVE[High] PRESERVE[Low] LIC-MID/STRESS PRESERVE[Low] LIC-MID/STRESS PRESERVE[High] PRESERVE[High] LIC-MID/STRESS PRESERVE[Low] = Belorussian The Constraint Tableau: /kola/ LIC MID/STRESSPRESERVE[high]PRESERVE[low] [ka"la] * [ku"la] *! [ki"la] *! [ko"la] *! [ke"la] *!

Factorial Typology: an exercise (3) /kola/ LIC MID/STRESSPRESERVE[low]PRESERVE[high]PRESERVE[rnd] [ka"la] *! * [ku"la] * [ki"la] * *! [ko"la] *! [ke"la] *! * This grammar provides for the raising of unstressed mid vowels. An unstressed /e/ is reduced to [i] ([kila]): PRESERVE[Front]. This reduction pattern occurs in Luiseño: /e/ > [i], /o/ > [u]. The same pattern can be arrived at with the hierarchy: PRESERVE[Low] LIC-MID/STRESS PRESERVE[High]

Factorial Typology: an exercise (4) /kola/ PRESERVE[low]PRESERVE[high]LIC MID/STRESS [ka"la] *! [ku"la] *! [ki"la] *! [ko"la] [ke"la] This grammar doesn t allow reduction of unstressed mid vowels This reduction pattern can be observed in many languages: e.g. Spanish, Polish The same pattern results from the hierarchy: PRESERVE[High] PRESERVE[Low] LIC-MID/STRESS * *

Constraint Interaction (1) E.g.: Final devoicing in German Obstruents in the coda are voiceless: /hant/ Hand vs. /hend@/ Hände The underlying lexical Form is /hand/ The following constraint is assumed: *VOICED-CODA = Obstruents in the coda cannot be voiced. The following Faithfulness constraint conflicts with it: IDENT-IO[vce] = The Specification of the feature [voice] in the Input segment must be retained in the corresponding Output segment

Constraint Interaction (2) The Generator generates the candidates [hand] and [hant] (as well as many others such as: [han], [hend], etc.) We restrict ourselves to the first two: [hand] conforms to IDENT-IO[vce], but violates *VOICED-CODA [hant] violates IDENT-IO[vce], but conforms to *VOICED-CODA We get the optimal form [hant] if we assume the following hierarchy of constraints: *VOICED-CODA IDENT-IO[vce] In English the hierarchy has to be reversed: IDENT-IO[vce] *VOICED-CODA

Auslautverhärtung German /hand/ *VOICED CODAIDENT IO[vce] [hand] *! [hant] *

Alternative: English /hand/ IDENT IO[vce]*VOICED CODA [hand] * [hant] *!

What about the following? German Direktor Doktor Reaktor Italian Direttore Dottore Reattore What constraints in what order can explain these two different Outputs in the two languages?

And another comparison? English /fi"l6s@f@/ /fil@"s6fik@l/ /"sk6l@/ /sk@"læstik/ /"mærin@/ /m@"ri:n@/ /"ærid/ /@"riditi/ /"si:kw@ns/ /si"kwens@l/ /"i:kw@l/ /I"kw6lIti/ /"f@ut@gra:f/ /f@"t6gr@f@/ German /filo"so:f/ /filoso"fi:/ /medi"tsi:n/ /meditsi"na:l/ /foto"gra:f/ /fotogra"fi:/ /"ma:gi@r/ /ma"gi:/ /"habitus/ /habi"ta:t/ /"lo:gis/ /lo"gismus/ What constraints in what order can explain these two different Outputs in the two languages? Think of the effect of stress on vowel quality in Belorussian.