Morphological Cues in Children s Processing of Ambiguous Sentences: a Study of Subject / Object Ambiguities in Greek

Similar documents
Case government vs Case agreement: modelling Modern Greek case attraction phenomena in LFG

Syntactic Ambiguity Resolution in Sentence Processing: New Evidence from a Morphologically Rich Language

The Real-Time Status of Island Phenomena *

Copyright and moral rights for this thesis are retained by the author

Approaches to control phenomena handout Obligatory control and morphological case: Icelandic and Basque

Underlying and Surface Grammatical Relations in Greek consider

Good Enough Language Processing: A Satisficing Approach

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Good-Enough Representations in Language Comprehension

The Acquisition of Person and Number Morphology Within the Verbal Domain in Early Greek

The presence of interpretable but ungrammatical sentences corresponds to mismatches between interpretive and productive parsing.

CS 598 Natural Language Processing

Phenomena of gender attraction in Polish *

Argument structure and theta roles

Ambiguity in the Brain: What Brain Imaging Reveals About the Processing of Syntactically Ambiguous Sentences

Introduction to HPSG. Introduction. Historical Overview. The HPSG architecture. Signature. Linguistic Objects. Descriptions.

SCHEMA ACTIVATION IN MEMORY FOR PROSE 1. Michael A. R. Townsend State University of New York at Albany

Dissertation Summaries. The Acquisition of Aspect and Motion Verbs in the Native Language (Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, 2014)

cmp-lg/ Jul 1995

Korean ECM Constructions and Cyclic Linearization

SOFTWARE EVALUATION TOOL

Age Effects on Syntactic Control in. Second Language Learning

Concept Acquisition Without Representation William Dylan Sabo

The Structure of Multiple Complements to V

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

THE VERB ARGUMENT BROWSER

1/20 idea. We ll spend an extra hour on 1/21. based on assigned readings. so you ll be ready to discuss them in class

An Interactive Intelligent Language Tutor Over The Internet

University of Groningen. Verbs in spoken sentence processing de Goede, Dieuwke

The College Board Redesigned SAT Grade 12

Ch VI- SENTENCE PATTERNS.

Language Acquisition Chart

Some Principles of Automated Natural Language Information Extraction

PAGE(S) WHERE TAUGHT If sub mission ins not a book, cite appropriate location(s))

Aging and the Use of Context in Ambiguity Resolution: Complex Changes From Simple Slowing

Constraining X-Bar: Theta Theory

Running head: DELAY AND PROSPECTIVE MEMORY 1

Syntax Parsing 1. Grammars and parsing 2. Top-down and bottom-up parsing 3. Chart parsers 4. Bottom-up chart parsing 5. The Earley Algorithm

Words come in categories

Candidates must achieve a grade of at least C2 level in each examination in order to achieve the overall qualification at C2 Level.

COMPUTATIONAL COMPLEXITY OF LEFT-ASSOCIATIVE GRAMMAR

Derivational: Inflectional: In a fit of rage the soldiers attacked them both that week, but lost the fight.

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

Author: Justyna Kowalczys Stowarzyszenie Angielski w Medycynie (PL) Feb 2015

Multiple case assignment and the English pseudo-passive *

Inleiding Taalkunde. Docent: Paola Monachesi. Blok 4, 2001/ Syntax 2. 2 Phrases and constituent structure 2. 3 A minigrammar of Italian 3

An Evaluation of the Interactive-Activation Model Using Masked Partial-Word Priming. Jason R. Perry. University of Western Ontario. Stephen J.

Specification and Evaluation of Machine Translation Toy Systems - Criteria for laboratory assignments

The Effect of Extensive Reading on Developing the Grammatical. Accuracy of the EFL Freshmen at Al Al-Bayt University

AN EXPERIMENTAL APPROACH TO NEW AND OLD INFORMATION IN TURKISH LOCATIVES AND EXISTENTIALS

Citation for published version (APA): Veenstra, M. J. A. (1998). Formalizing the minimalist program Groningen: s.n.

Pseudo-Passives as Adjectival Passives

Som and Optimality Theory

Agreement attraction in comprehension: representations and processes*

DIDACTIC MODEL BRIDGING A CONCEPT WITH PHENOMENA

The Smart/Empire TIPSTER IR System

Natural Language Processing. George Konidaris

11/29/2010. Statistical Parsing. Statistical Parsing. Simple PCFG for ATIS English. Syntactic Disambiguation

Eye Movements in Speech Technologies: an overview of current research

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

Acquiring verb agreement in HKSL: Optional or obligatory?

Merry-Go-Round. Science and Technology Grade 4: Understanding Structures and Mechanisms Pulleys and Gears. Language Grades 4-5: Oral Communication

Cross-linguistic aspects in child L2 acquisition

Chapter 4: Valence & Agreement CSLI Publications

Using computational modeling in language acquisition research

California Department of Education English Language Development Standards for Grade 8

To appear in The TESOL encyclopedia of ELT (Wiley-Blackwell) 1 RECASTING. Kazuya Saito. Birkbeck, University of London

THE ACQUISITION OF INFLECTIONAL MORPHEMES: THE PRIORITY OF PLURAL S

Describing Motion Events in Adult L2 Spanish Narratives

Specifying a shallow grammatical for parsing purposes

The Perception of Nasalized Vowels in American English: An Investigation of On-line Use of Vowel Nasalization in Lexical Access

Second Language Acquisition of Complex Structures: The Case of English Restrictive Relative Clauses

Target Language Preposition Selection an Experiment with Transformation-Based Learning and Aligned Bilingual Data

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

Rote rehearsal and spacing effects in the free recall of pure and mixed lists. By: Peter P.J.L. Verkoeijen and Peter F. Delaney

Type-driven semantic interpretation and feature dependencies in R-LFG

Frequency and pragmatically unmarked word order *

Modeling Attachment Decisions with a Probabilistic Parser: The Case of Head Final Structures

Individual Differences & Item Effects: How to test them, & how to test them well

THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF HUMANITIES & SOCIAL STUDIES

Aspectual Classes of Verb Phrases

THE PERCEPTIONS OF THE JAPANESE IMPERFECTIVE ASPECT MARKER TEIRU AMONG NATIVE SPEAKERS AND L2 LEARNERS OF JAPANESE

Syntactic and Lexical Simplification: The Impact on EFL Listening Comprehension at Low and High Language Proficiency Levels

Parsing of part-of-speech tagged Assamese Texts

Which verb classes and why? Research questions: Semantic Basis Hypothesis (SBH) What verb classes? Why the truth of the SBH matters

Ambiguities and anomalies: What can eye-movements and event-related potentials reveal about second language sentence processing?

Second Language Acquisition in Adults: From Research to Practice

Firms and Markets Saturdays Summer I 2014

Linguistic Variation across Sports Category of Press Reportage from British Newspapers: a Diachronic Multidimensional Analysis

THE INFLUENCE OF TASK DEMANDS ON FAMILIARITY EFFECTS IN VISUAL WORD RECOGNITION: A COHORT MODEL PERSPECTIVE DISSERTATION

LING 329 : MORPHOLOGY

Control and Boundedness

Lower and Upper Secondary

An Empirical and Computational Test of Linguistic Relativity

L1 and L2 acquisition. Holger Diessel

UC Berkeley Berkeley Undergraduate Journal of Classics

UCLA Issues in Applied Linguistics

Degeneracy results in canalisation of language structure: A computational model of word learning

Does the Difficulty of an Interruption Affect our Ability to Resume?

What the National Curriculum requires in reading at Y5 and Y6

Transcription:

Morphological Cues in Children s Processing of Ambiguous Sentences: a Study of Subject / Object Ambiguities in Greek Despina Papadopoulou & Ianthi Tsimpli Aristotle University of Thessaloniki 1. Introduction In this paper, we investigate the on-line processing of locally ambiguous sentences, in which the ambiguity is resolved by morphological means, namely by S-V agreement and Case on DPs. Moreover, we compare the parsing strategies employed by adult readers with those used by in order to examine the development of the parser. The method used is an on-line measure, more specifically a self-paced reading task. The structures we tested involve subject/object ambiguities, in which the verb of a pre-posed adverbial clause is optionally transitive and followed by a DP which could be either the object of the embedded verb (1a) or the subject of the main verb (1b): (1) a. While she was eating the pizza she fell on the floor. (1) b. While she was eating the pizza fell on the floor. Various parsing models (Frazier & Fodor, 1978; Frazier, 1987; Pritchett, 1988; 1992; Weinberg, 21) make the prediction that the object reading should be favored over the subject reading. The reason is either the operation of Late Closure, that is the preference of the parser to attach new constituents (i.e. the pizza) to the phrase currently being processed (in (1a) and (1b) the embedded verb), or the cost associated with the analysis of the DP the pizza as the subject of the main verb due to its attachment to a new thematic domain (the one defined by the main verb). The preference for the object reading has been empirically supported by the results of several on-line studies in English. So, in

an early eye-tracking study study, Frazier & Rayner (1982) found that English experience the garden-path effect when processing the main verb in sentences like (1b). Similarly, Mitchell (1987) obtained the same result, not only when the embedded verbs were optionally transitive but also when they were intransitive. The object reading preference for sentences like (1b) has been replicated by a recent eye-tracking study (Pickering & Traxler 23) with English. With respect to child sentence processing, Traxler (22) conducted three self-paced reading experiments, in which he examined the way 1- to 11-yearold English process sentences like (1b) under three conditions: in the first condition the DP following the embedded verb was a plausible object of the verb, in the second an implausible object and in the third condition the embedded verb was intransitive. All three tasks showed that the preferred to analyze the DP after the embedded verb as its object rather than as the subject of the main verb. Traxler interpreted this result as evidence for the priority of the syntactic processor over the thematic processor in child sentence processing. The fact that the seem to rely more on grammatical cues rather than on thematic/semantic/pragmatic cues has also been obtained by other studies on child sentence processing (see Felser et al., 23). The present study reports on the findings of two on-line experiments conducted with Greek and on sentences such as (1a) and (1b). In the first experiment, the ambiguity was resolved on the main verb by means of S-V agreement, while the DP following the embedded verb was unmarked for Case and, thus, could not provide any information about its syntactic function as Subject or Object. In the second experiment, the DP following the embedded verb was marked for either Accusative or Nominative case, thus, its syntactic function was explicit and disambiguated the sentences: (2) a. Kathos etroghe ta biskota epese sto patoma. while was-eating the cookies fell-3sg on-the floor While (s)he was eating the cookies (s)he fell on the floor. (2) b. Kathos etroghe tus ahinus epese sto patoma. while was-eating the-pl-acc sea-urchins-acc fell-3sg on-the floor While (s)he was eating the sea-urchins (s)he fell on the floor. 2. Method 2.1. Procedure The procedure used in both experiments was an on-line self-paced reading measure. The sentences were presented in a word-by-word fashion. Once the subjects read each word, they had to press the spacebar to proceed to the next one. The button presses resulted in the disappearance of the segment and the appearance of the next one (moving-window technique). In addition, the participants had to perform a grammaticality judgment task at the end of each

sentence, by pressing the button YES if they thought the sentence was grammatical and NO if they thought it was ungrammatical. 2.2. Materials Each experiment consisted of four versions. Each version comprised 96 sentences, half of which was grammatical and half ungrammatical. Our critical items were distributed across four conditions, which resulted from the manipulation of two variables with two levels each: the argument structure of the embedded verb (optionally transitive vs. intransitive verbs) and the syntactic function of the DP following the embedded verb (object of the embedded verb vs. subject of the main verb). The experimental items were twenty-four quartets, with one member of each quartet representing each condition. The three members of each quartet were grammatical and the fourth one was ungrammatical, as can be seen in (2) (5) below: (3) Optionally transitive verb; object reading () Kathos majireve ta makaronja kaike stin katsarola. while was-cooking the spaghetti-pl burnt-herself-3sg on the pot While (s)he was cooking the spaghetti (s)he burnt herself on the pot. (4) Optionally transitive verb; subject reading () Kathos majireve ta makaronja kaikan stin katsarola. while was-cooking the spaghetti-pl burnt-3pl in the pot While (s)he was cooking the spaghetti burnt in the pot. (5) Intransitive verb; object reading (ungrammatical; ) * Kathos etrehe ta makaronja kaike stin katsarola. while was-running the spaghetti-pl burnt-herself-3sg in the pot *While (s)he was running the spaghetti (s)he burnt herself on the pot. (6) Intransitive verb; subject reading () Kathos etrehe ta makaronja kaikan stin katsarola. while was-running the spaghetti-pl burnt-3pl in the pot While (s)he was running the spaghetti burnt in the pot. In the Case Experiment, the materials were similar with those of the Agreement Experiment, but they differed in the way the ambiguities were resolved. In this task, the ambiguity was resolved via the morphological Case (Accusative or Nominative) carried by the DP following the embedded verb: (7) Optionally transitive verb; object reading () Kathos majireve tus astakus kaike stin katsarola. while was-cooking the-acc lobsters-acc burnt-herself-3sg on the pot While (s)he was cooking the lobsters (s)he burnt herself on the pot. (8) Optionally transitive verb; subject reading () Kathos majireve i astaki kaikan stin katsarola.

while was-cooking the-nom lobsters-nom burnt-3pl in the pot While (s)he was cooking the lobsters burnt in the pot. (9) Intransitive verb; object reading (ungrammatical; ) * Kathos etrehe tus astakus kaike stin katsarola. while was running the-acc lobsters-acc burnt-herself-3sg in the pot *While (s)he was running the lobsters (s)he burnt herself on the pot. (1) Intransitive verb; subject reading () Kathos etrehe i astaki kaikan stin katsarola. while was running the-nom lobsters-nom burnt-3pl in the pot While (s)he was running the lobsters burnt in the pot. In each experimental version there were twenty-four critical sentences, six of which were ungrammatical. Each experimental version was designed so that each subject was exposed to all conditions and all materials but never saw the same experimental item more than once. 2.3. Subjects All the subjects participating in the experiments were naive with respect to the purpose of the study. The following Table presents more detailed information about the subjects: Table 1. The profile of the subjects Subjects Number Gender Age range 8 3 M; 5 F 2-4 years 53 24M; 29 F 1-11 years 2.4. Data analysis Only subjects who were accurate more than 6% were included in the data analyses. This resulted in the exclusion of one child. In addition, all erroneous responses to the grammaticality judgments as well as RTs above 2 ms and above 1.5 SD from the mean RT of each subject per segment were eliminated. 3. Agreement Experiment 3.1. Results The following Graph depicts the mean RTs for the disambiguating segment that is the main verb:

9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 Graph1. Main Verb: Mean RTs per group We ran a two-way repeated-measures ANOVA for each group separately with Verb type (transitive vs. intransitive verbs) and Syntactic function of the DP following the embedded verb (subject vs. object) as within-subjects variables. The adult data revealed a significant main effect of Syntactic function (F(1,37)=7,578; p<,1), due to the faster RTs for the subject reading, and a marginally significant interaction between the two variables (F(1,37)=3,479; p=,7). The latter finding is due to the fact that the ungrammatical condition () was read significantly slower than the one (t(38)=2,568; p<,2), whereas there were no significant differences between the and the condition. The child data showed a significant main effect of Verb type (F(1,25)=5,811; p<,3) due to the longer RTs for the ungrammatical condition. 3.2. Agreement results: Discussion First of all, the fact that the ungrammatical condition was read significantly longer than the grammatical ones points toward two directions. The first is that both groups successfully used S-V agreement information to process sentences on-line. The second is that, when encountering the main verb, the subjects had already accessed the argument structure of the embedded verb, and, hence, at this point the thematic processor was in operation. Moreover, our findings clearly show that Greek differs from English, in that the well-documented object reading preference in English is not found in the Greek data. This result indicates that there might be cross-linguistic variation in sentence processing due to morphological richness and associated properties of individual languages, e.g. word order variation. We propose that the observed differences between Greek and English are attributed to the fact that Greek

readers rely more on morphological cues when parsing Greek sentences than on parsing principles such as Late Closure. Furthermore, we maintain that the absence of any unambiguous morphological cues, like the DPs unmarked for case in the Agreement task, might have resulted in the generation of the two possible structures in parallel (c. Frazier & Clifton, 1996 for non-primary phrases). This is why the Greek readers do not exhibit a garden-path effect when the syntactic function of the DP becomes explicit. 4. Case Experiment 4.1. Results The following Graph presents the and s RTs on the Determiner: 6 5 4 3 2 1 Graph 2. Determiner: Main RTs per condition for both groups The statistical analyses were conducted as for the Agreement task. In the adult data, there was a significant interaction between Verb type and Syntactic function (F(1,39)=9,212; p<,1). Paired-samples t-tests showed that there was a significant difference only between the two transitive verb conditions (t1(39)=4,111; p<,1) but not between the intransitive ones. No significant effects were obtained from the child data.

The RTs on the Noun are reported in Graph 3: 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 Graph 3. Noun: Mean RTs per condition for both groups The adult data showed that the main effect of Syntactic function was significant (F(1,39)=4,786;p<,4) due to the slower RTs for the object reading than for the subject reading and that the interaction between Verb type and Syntactic function was significant (F(1,39)=13,77; p<,1). Paired-samples t- tests revealed the condition was significantly slower than the (t(39)=4,19; p<,1), whereas there was not a significant difference between the two transitive verb conditions. On the other hand, the child data yielded a significant main effect of Syntactic function (F(1,25)=4,181; p=,52), namely the sentences in which the DP turned out to be the subject of the main verb were read significantly faster than the ones in which the DP was the object of the embedded verb. 4.2. Case results: Discussion The results from the Case experiment indicate that the processing of the experimental sentences involves two stages, on the Determiner and the Noun respectively. The two-stage processing is found in both and. During the first stage, the adult data show a Late Closure effect, which does not seem to be affected by the argument structure of the verb. Note, however, that during the second stage, the object preference disappears while the grammaticality effect is evident. The latter indicates that the argument structure of the embedded verb is available to the readers at this stage. On the other hand, the fact that the object reading preference disappears implies that the gardenpath effect manifested on the Determiner is not as conscious as is in English. During the first stage (i.e. the Determiner), the child data are similar with the adult in that no grammaticality effect is obtained while at the same time

show no preference for subject or object reading. Thus, for Late Closure effects are not shown at all. During the second stage, the thematic processor is available as indicated by the grammaticality effect. However, unlike, show a preference for the subject reading which appears to be an early closure effect. As we will see in the accuracy data, the fast RTs for the subject reading have adverse effects on accuracy. 4. Grammaticality Judgements: RTs & Accuracy In this section, we report on the data from the judgement task. In Graph 4, the response times for the Agreement Experiment are presented per group: 13 12 11 1 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 Graph 4. Agreement Experiment: Mean RTs on GJs per group The responded to the condition significantly faster than to the condition (t(37)=2,386; p<,3), which resembles a garden-path effect albeit at the recall stage. The responded more slowly to the transitive than to the intransitive verbs (F(1,25)=6,74; p<,2), which might be attributed to the construction of two parallel structures in the on-line processing of the DP unmarked for case.

Graph 5 shows the RTs for the Case task: 12 11 1 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 Graph 5. Case Experiment: Mean RTs on GJs per group The results showed no significant effects for either the or the. Graph 6 presents the data on accurate responses for the Agreement task for each group: 1 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 Graph 6. Agreement Experiment: Accuracy Percentages per group There were no significant effects in either group, although overall were more accurate than the.

Graph 7 depicts the accuracy data for the Case task: 1 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 Graph 7. Case Experiment: Accuracy percentages per group No significant effects were obtained in the adult data. The child data revealed that the condition was responded to significantly less accurately than the condition (t(25)=2,899; p<,1). Given the s preference for the subject reading found in the RTs for both the Noun and the GJs we are presented with a speed-accuracy trade-off effect. 5. Conclusions The results from our study suggest that in highly inflected languages morphological cues override syntactically based parsing strategies like Late Closure at the initial stage of parsing. In such languages, morphologically unmarked words might result in the generation of parallel structures, evidenced in the child and adult data from the Agreement experiment. In the Case task, we found evidence that the thematic processor operates on the Noun, i.e. a lexical category but not on the Determiner. Thus, the grammaticality effect found in the Agreement experiment on the main verb, also a lexical category, is accounted for. The difference between child and adult processing is (a) that no garden-path effects are found in the child data in either experiment and (b) that show an early closure effect in the Case experiment which leads to a drop in their accuracy rates in the condition. The child findings are consistent with the claim that child processing is based on grammatical properties. In our study, rely more on morphological cues for ambiguity resolution rather than on heuristics, like Late Closure, used by on the Determiner in the Case experiment.

References Felser, C. Marinis, Th., & Clahsen, H. 23. Children's Processing of Ambiguous Sentences: A Study of Relative Clause Attachment. Language Acquisition 11(3): 127-163. Frazier, L. 1987. Theories of sentence processing. In J. Garfield (ed.), Modularity in Knowledge Representation and Natural Language Understanding (pp. 291-37). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Frazier, L., & Clifton, C. 1996. Construal. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Frazier, L., & Fodor, J.D. 1978. The sausage machine: a new two-stage parsing model. Cognition 6: 291-325. Frazier, L., & Rayner, K. 1982. Making and correcting errors during sentence comprehension: eye movements in the analysis of structurally ambiguous sentences. Cognitive Psychology 14: 178-269. Mitchell, D.C. 1987. Lexical guidance in human parsing: Locus and processing characteristics. In M. Coltheart (ed.), Attention and Performance XII: The Psychology of Reading (pp. 61-618). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Pickering, M.J., & Traxler, M. J. 23. Plausibility and recovery from garden-paths: An eye-tracking study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 24, 94-961. Pritchett, B.L. 1988. Garden-path phenomena and the grammatical basis of language processing. Language 64: 539-576. Pritchett, B.L. 1992. Parsing with grammar: Islands, heads, and garden paths. In H. Goodluck, & M. Rochemont (eds.), Island Constraints: Theory, Acquisition, and Processing (pp. 321-349). Dordrecht: Kluwer. Traxler, M. 22. Plausibility and subcategorization preference in 's processing of temporarily ambiguous sentences: Evidence from self-paced reading. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 55A, 75-96. Weinberg, A. 21. A Minimalist Theory of Human Sentence Processing. In S.D. Epstein and N. Hornstein (eds), Working Minimalist (pp. 283-314). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.