Adina Schreiber Tues. 3/13/07 Trueswell, Sekerina, Hill, and Logrip

Similar documents
Good Enough Language Processing: A Satisficing Approach

Copyright and moral rights for this thesis are retained by the author

The presence of interpretable but ungrammatical sentences corresponds to mismatches between interpretive and productive parsing.

Phenomena of gender attraction in Polish *

Good-Enough Representations in Language Comprehension

Aging and the Use of Context in Ambiguity Resolution: Complex Changes From Simple Slowing

Which verb classes and why? Research questions: Semantic Basis Hypothesis (SBH) What verb classes? Why the truth of the SBH matters

The Perception of Nasalized Vowels in American English: An Investigation of On-line Use of Vowel Nasalization in Lexical Access

11/29/2010. Statistical Parsing. Statistical Parsing. Simple PCFG for ATIS English. Syntactic Disambiguation

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

Syntactic Ambiguity Resolution in Sentence Processing: New Evidence from a Morphologically Rich Language

An Interactive Intelligent Language Tutor Over The Internet

THE INFLUENCE OF TASK DEMANDS ON FAMILIARITY EFFECTS IN VISUAL WORD RECOGNITION: A COHORT MODEL PERSPECTIVE DISSERTATION

Language Learning and Development. ISSN: (Print) (Online) Journal homepage:

A Bootstrapping Model of Frequency and Context Effects in Word Learning

Eye Movements in Speech Technologies: an overview of current research

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

Natural Language Processing. George Konidaris

Ambiguity in the Brain: What Brain Imaging Reveals About the Processing of Syntactically Ambiguous Sentences

Guidelines for Writing an Internship Report

Parsing of part-of-speech tagged Assamese Texts

Approaches to control phenomena handout Obligatory control and morphological case: Icelandic and Basque

SOFTWARE EVALUATION TOOL

Effects of speaker gaze on spoken language comprehension: Task matters

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

SEMAFOR: Frame Argument Resolution with Log-Linear Models

Linking object names and object categories: Words (but not tones) facilitate object categorization in 6- and 12-month-olds

Possessive have and (have) got in New Zealand English Heidi Quinn, University of Canterbury, New Zealand

Syntax Parsing 1. Grammars and parsing 2. Top-down and bottom-up parsing 3. Chart parsers 4. Bottom-up chart parsing 5. The Earley Algorithm

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

Modeling Attachment Decisions with a Probabilistic Parser: The Case of Head Final Structures

Argument structure and theta roles

AN ANALYSIS OF GRAMMTICAL ERRORS MADE BY THE SECOND YEAR STUDENTS OF SMAN 5 PADANG IN WRITING PAST EXPERIENCES

An Evaluation of the Interactive-Activation Model Using Masked Partial-Word Priming. Jason R. Perry. University of Western Ontario. Stephen J.

Lecture 2: Quantifiers and Approximation

University of Groningen. Verbs in spoken sentence processing de Goede, Dieuwke

Generation of Referring Expressions: Managing Structural Ambiguities

Specification and Evaluation of Machine Translation Toy Systems - Criteria for laboratory assignments

Constraining X-Bar: Theta Theory

NAME: East Carolina University PSYC Developmental Psychology Dr. Eppler & Dr. Ironsmith

CS 598 Natural Language Processing

An Introduction to the Minimalist Program

Some Principles of Automated Natural Language Information Extraction

Using computational modeling in language acquisition research

MENTORING. Tips, Techniques, and Best Practices

AN EXPERIMENTAL APPROACH TO NEW AND OLD INFORMATION IN TURKISH LOCATIVES AND EXISTENTIALS

Introduction to HPSG. Introduction. Historical Overview. The HPSG architecture. Signature. Linguistic Objects. Descriptions.

1/20 idea. We ll spend an extra hour on 1/21. based on assigned readings. so you ll be ready to discuss them in class

Target Language Preposition Selection an Experiment with Transformation-Based Learning and Aligned Bilingual Data

Case government vs Case agreement: modelling Modern Greek case attraction phenomena in LFG

Objectives. Chapter 2: The Representation of Knowledge. Expert Systems: Principles and Programming, Fourth Edition

Aspectual Classes of Verb Phrases

Universal Grammar 2. Universal Grammar 1. Forms and functions 1. Universal Grammar 3. Conceptual and surface structure of complex clauses

Ambiguities and anomalies: What can eye-movements and event-related potentials reveal about second language sentence processing?

Morphosyntactic and Referential Cues to the Identification of Generic Statements

Author: Justyna Kowalczys Stowarzyszenie Angielski w Medycynie (PL) Feb 2015

Inleiding Taalkunde. Docent: Paola Monachesi. Blok 4, 2001/ Syntax 2. 2 Phrases and constituent structure 2. 3 A minigrammar of Italian 3

Construction Grammar. University of Jena.

Science Fair Project Handbook

L1 and L2 acquisition. Holger Diessel

Chunk Parsing for Base Noun Phrases using Regular Expressions. Let s first let the variable s0 be the sentence tree of the first sentence.

The Discourse Anaphoric Properties of Connectives

Machine Learning from Garden Path Sentences: The Application of Computational Linguistics

Kelli Allen. Vicki Nieter. Jeanna Scheve. Foreword by Gregory J. Kaiser

Loughton School s curriculum evening. 28 th February 2017

Sample Goals and Benchmarks

Unit 3. Design Activity. Overview. Purpose. Profile

Chapter 4: Valence & Agreement CSLI Publications

A Usage-Based Approach to Recursion in Sentence Processing

Language Acquisition Fall 2010/Winter Lexical Categories. Afra Alishahi, Heiner Drenhaus

Individual Differences & Item Effects: How to test them, & how to test them well

Language acquisition: acquiring some aspects of syntax.

Major Milestones, Team Activities, and Individual Deliverables

Using dialogue context to improve parsing performance in dialogue systems

Character Stream Parsing of Mixed-lingual Text

A content-addressable pointer mechanism underlies comprehension of verb-phrase ellipsis q

Update on Soar-based language processing

Correspondence between the DRDP (2015) and the California Preschool Learning Foundations. Foundations (PLF) in Language and Literacy

LQVSumm: A Corpus of Linguistic Quality Violations in Multi-Document Summarization

Lexical Access during Sentence Comprehension (Re)Consideration of Context Effects

Assessing System Agreement and Instance Difficulty in the Lexical Sample Tasks of SENSEVAL-2

Grounding Language for Interactive Task Learning

Enhancing Unlexicalized Parsing Performance using a Wide Coverage Lexicon, Fuzzy Tag-set Mapping, and EM-HMM-based Lexical Probabilities

Formulaic Language and Fluency: ESL Teaching Applications

Language Acquisition by Identical vs. Fraternal SLI Twins * Karin Stromswold & Jay I. Rifkin

Senior Stenographer / Senior Typist Series (including equivalent Secretary titles)

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Milton Public Schools Special Education Programs & Supports

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

SCHEMA ACTIVATION IN MEMORY FOR PROSE 1. Michael A. R. Townsend State University of New York at Albany

The Smart/Empire TIPSTER IR System

Theoretical Syntax Winter Answers to practice problems

AQUA: An Ontology-Driven Question Answering System

Compositional Semantics

Organizing Comprehensive Literacy Assessment: How to Get Started

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

Course Law Enforcement II. Unit I Careers in Law Enforcement

Red Flags of Conflict

Writing a composition

Introduction to Questionnaire Design

Transcription:

Adina Schreiber Tues. 3/13/07 Trueswell, Sekerina, Hill, and Logrip The Kindergarten-path Effect: Studying Sentence Processing in Young Children *How do children process language in real time? Do they coordinate multiple sources of information during interpretation? -Adults don t take into account discourse/pragmatics principles to solve temporary syntactic ambiguities, and they show little to no ability to revise initial parsing commitment. - It s rapid and context-sensitive. - Adults make initial mistakes that they later revise. Garden-path phenomenon. - listeners can make a choice to go down a path when they encounter an ambiguity, then they can later retrace their steps and go back to the intended *What kinds of information take priority? Consider these sentences: 1. Anne hit the thief with the stick. 2. Anne hit the thief with the wart. *How do adults arrive at these conclusions? - They use lexically specific syntactic information, semantic plausibility, frequency of lexical co-occurrence, and referential context. - They use statistical regularities pertaining to the syntax of a language, not broad structural heuristics. - They coordinate the linguistic properties of the message with information from the context to determine processing commitments. Constraint-based lexical theory- assumes a constraint-satisfaction approach to ambiguity resolution. Multiple sources of information can be used to converge on one Referential Principle- relevant sources of information from the context can affect processing commitments, especially when lexical properties of the stimulus are relatively neutral. Children s On-line Language Processing Abilities *How do children process the PP-attachment ambiguity? *Do they use the Referential Principle? -Anaphoric (ie something that refers back) mapping processes appeared to be well mastered in all age groups, but developmental differences were observed for pronouns - 5 year olds rely more on pragmatic plausibility than lexical factors (such as gendermarking) in their assignment of pronominal co-reference. - Children have knowledge of contextual factors associated with the Referential Principle, and this knowledge can interfere with the understanding of restrictive relative

clauses. (Though some studies have found weaker or no effect of referential factors on relative clause comprehensions.) Experiment 1: The Frog(s), Horse, Napkins, and Box Experiment Referents: possible objects to be moved 1-Referent: one frog, one horse 2-Referents: two frogs, one on a napkin, one not. Context: either one or two referents Target: the frog on the napkin, the correct one to move Incorrect/Other: the frog on the table or the horse, the wrong animal to move Incorrect Destination: the empty napkin Ambiguous Phrase: Put the frog on the napkin in the box. Non-ambiguous Phrase: Put the frog that is on the napkin in the box. Modifier: The interpretation that on the napkin tells you which frog to move Destination: The interpretation that on the napkin tells you where to put a frog NP Attachment: Like modifier it refers to on the napkin being a sister of the noun frog VP Attachment: Like destination, it refers to on the napkin being a sister of the verb put Course Grain Data: what the subjects picked, where they put it, etc. Fine Grain Data: where their eyes looked, for how long, when, etc. Cross Checking: comparing Course Grain conclusions with Fine Grain conclusions Onset Phoneme: first sound of the words, especially the relevant ones, like frog vs. horse -Adults used the Referential Preference; they considered the empty napkin only when the sentence was Ambiguous and it was a 1-Referent trial. -Children looked at the empty napkin for 1 or 2 referent ambiguous trials. Data showed an effect of ambiguity, and no interaction between ambiguity and context. Children didn t use the Referential Principle, but rather had a bias for the destination They were often unable to recover from their initial mistake Observations: -1-Referent trial mistakes almost always had the correct animal, but incorrect destination -2-Referent trials had almost an equal number of correct animals moved as incorrect animals moved. -Children adopt a single strategy (structural or interpretive) for dealing with ambiguity. Course Grain Data -Five year olds prefer the Destination -Subjects usually directed gaze at incorrect destination during ambiguous trials, regardless of referential context. -Subjects were at chance levels when selecting Target over Other animal in tworeferent trials, suggesting that on the napkin was not treated as a Modifier and therefore was uninformative in determining a possible referent.

Fine Grain Data - Children looked more at the Incorrect Destination about 300 ms after the onset of the word napkin, mostly under the Ambiguous conditions. - This occurs in both 1-Referent and 2-Referent trials, suggesting insensitivity to the Referential Principle. - Ambiguity had an effect, context did not, and the two factors didn t interact. Fine Grain analyses revealed looks to the Incorrect Destination occurred more in the Ambiguous sentences, independent of Context, even at the earliest stages of processing. Reliable signs of considering the Incorrect Destination on Ambiguous trials began when subjects heard approximately 300 ms of the word napkin. Cross checking with Course Grain analysis: 91.1% of the time, the two analyses agreed. Recognition of Direct Object Noun Frog (and Referential Ambiguity) - The 1-Referent conditions showed a divergence between fixations on the Target and Other animals, with the higher probability on the Target, telling us the correct referent was established soon after disambiguating phonemic material was perceived. - In all 1-Referent contexts, the onset phoneme was different for the Target and Other animal (frog vs. horse.) - In the 2-Referent Unambiguous contexts, a similar divergence between fixations on the Target and Other was delayed in the 2-Referent contexts, until the onset of the word napkin, when fixations on the Target would increase. Statistics 1. Prior to any phonemic information to discriminate between the two animals, there were no significant differences in looking times to the Target and Other animals. 2. Upon hearing frog and before hearing napkin more time was spent looking at the Target animal in the 1-referent context, but not the 2-referent context. 3. After hearing napkin subjects fixated more on the Target in all conditions but the Two-referent ambiguous condition. Thus the phrase on the napkin was taken as a Modifier in the 2-Referent Unambiguous condition, helping to distinguish the Target from the Other. It was NOT taken as a Modifier in the 2-Referent context, resulting in continued competition between the two possible Referents. Summary of the Fine Grain Analyses -It indicated that children s first interpretation of the ambiguous phrase on the napkin tends to be a Destination rather than a Modifier. -More looks to the Incorrect Referent were found in the ambiguous trials than the unambiguous after they heard napkin. -Children s assignment of interpretation (correct or otherwise) is highly incremental. -Referential competition between the Target and the Other animals was consistently resolved at the points in speech where phonemic information could help distinguish the two referents: at frog in the 1-Referent conditions, and at napkin in the 2-Referent Unambiguous condition.

-The 2-Referent Ambiguous condition showed no resolution, suggesting the inability to take the ambiguous phrase on the napkin as a Modifier. Eye Movements for Correct and Incorrect Trials -Early looks to the Target or Other animal would correlate with which of the two animals were moved first in a trial in Ambiguous Environments. Subjects were choosing the referent based on which animal they happened to look at first. Experiment 2: Adults -Adults experienced a garden path in for the Ambiguous 1-Referent trials. -There were increased looks to the Incorrect Destination for those trials only. -Interaction between context and ambiguity. Fine Grain Analysis -Increased looks to the Incorrect Destination occur shortly after the onset of the word napkin, restricted to the 1-Referent Ambiguous trials, implying an initial Destination - Sensitivity to the Referential Principle is essentially immediate. - Ambiguity and Context interact. - Disambiguation occurs just after the onset of the word frog in the 1-Referent Contexts. - Looks to the Target animal are greater in the 1-Referent Unambiguous than the 1- Referent Ambiguous trials, because subjects are inspecting the Incorrect Destination in the latter but not the former. - The 2-Referent Contexts showed more consideration of the Other. -Divergence was early, especially in the 2-Referent conditions, showing that subjects were using the preposition on as a clue. Statistics: Before the word frog, there was no difference between looking at the Target or Other, in any run. -Between frog and napkin, the subjects looked more at frogs. - On helped, but not reliably. -After napkin, subjects looked more at the Target in all conditions. This tells us The Modifier interpretation was pursued in both Ambiguous and Non-Ambiguous conditions, distinguishing the Target from the Other. Comparison with 5 Year Olds -There was a reliable triple interaction between Context, Ambiguity and Age. -Adults were affected by Context and Ambiguity, Children only by Ambiguity. - Both groups showed rapid incremental - They differed in how they handled temporary syntactic ambiguity.

- Adults resolved it with the Referential Principle, 5 year olds did not. - Adults chose the Modifier in 2-Reference contexts, children always preferred the Destination option. - They chose Destination when the context indicated that the Modifier was unnecessary (1-Referent context.) - The preferences came early, just after hearing the phrase on the napkin. -Adults recovered from incorrect interpretations, children did not. -Adults had very few errors. - Children s choice of referent in 2-Referent Ambiguous trials was random. Explanations for VP Attachment Preference for the Younger Group Explanation 1: Children parse according to some principle, such as choose the least complicated structure. Maybe as people age, their Minimal Attachment parsing commitment (based on lexical and context factors) becomes faster. Explanation 2: Lexical properties of a child s input matters. They might be using syntactic or semantic knowledge of verbs and possible arguments. This makes the Destination option more likely because put the frog on the napkin is the most likely syntactic alternative given the lexical input. Young children exhibit strong sensitivity to lexically specific syntactic preferences. Also, patterns of incorrect interpretations are best accounted for by assuming emerging sensitivity to lexically specific properties, such as animacy, subcategory, and control information. Wiggle Frog Experiment Lexically specific syntactic biases play a role in children s processing commitments, but there must be other constraints leading to a preference for the Destination Insensitivity to the Referential Principle in the Youngest Parsers -Verb specific syntactic and semantic properties present in the stimuli so strongly supported the Destination interpretation that referential factors were unable to impact processing preferences. -It s possible that children have the Referential Principle, but can t use it in certain circumstances. Summary and Closing Marks -Five year olds have a language processing system that relies more heavily on local linguistic factors to inform parsing preferences, along with a general inability to reverse commitments to initial interpretations. They have a highly incremental processing system. Word recognition and referential resolution in syntactically unambiguous environments appears to proceed smoothly, showing patterns similar to adults. -Adults can use relevant contextual factors to inform parsing commitments, and they can revise.