Commentary on the article: From sense of number to sense of magnitude The role of continuous magnitudes in numerical cognition

Similar documents
9.85 Cognition in Infancy and Early Childhood. Lecture 7: Number

Lecture 2: Quantifiers and Approximation

Evidence for distinct magnitude systems for symbolic and non-symbolic number

Linking object names and object categories: Words (but not tones) facilitate object categorization in 6- and 12-month-olds

Summary / Response. Karl Smith, Accelerations Educational Software. Page 1 of 8

1 3-5 = Subtraction - a binary operation

DO YOU HAVE THESE CONCERNS?

Mathematics Education

Extending Place Value with Whole Numbers to 1,000,000

Inquiry Learning Methodologies and the Disposition to Energy Systems Problem Solving

A cautionary note is research still caught up in an implementer approach to the teacher?

Exact Equality and Successor Function : Two Keys Concepts on the Path towards Understanding Exact Numbers

Document number: 2013/ Programs Committee 6/2014 (July) Agenda Item 42.0 Bachelor of Engineering with Honours in Software Engineering

Build on students informal understanding of sharing and proportionality to develop initial fraction concepts.

+32 (0)

CLASSIFICATION OF PROGRAM Critical Elements Analysis 1. High Priority Items Phonemic Awareness Instruction

Unraveling symbolic number processing and the implications for its association with mathematics. Delphine Sasanguie

The KAM project: Mathematics in vocational subjects*

Grade 6: Correlated to AGS Basic Math Skills

NCSC Alternate Assessments and Instructional Materials Based on Common Core State Standards

SAT MATH PREP:

Tun your everyday simulation activity into research

ASSESSMENT OF STUDENT LEARNING OUTCOMES WITHIN ACADEMIC PROGRAMS AT WEST CHESTER UNIVERSITY

Genevieve L. Hartman, Ph.D.

Pedagogical Content Knowledge for Teaching Primary Mathematics: A Case Study of Two Teachers

Alpha provides an overall measure of the internal reliability of the test. The Coefficient Alphas for the STEP are:

Focus of the Unit: Much of this unit focuses on extending previous skills of multiplication and division to multi-digit whole numbers.

BENCHMARK TREND COMPARISON REPORT:

Strategies for Solving Fraction Tasks and Their Link to Algebraic Thinking

Missouri Mathematics Grade-Level Expectations

Stages of Literacy Ros Lugg

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

Aviation English Training: How long Does it Take?

A Game-based Assessment of Children s Choices to Seek Feedback and to Revise

By Merrill Harmin, Ph.D.

How to Judge the Quality of an Objective Classroom Test

Robert S. Unnasch, Ph.D.

University of Toronto

Software Maintenance

Lecture 1: Machine Learning Basics

On Human Computer Interaction, HCI. Dr. Saif al Zahir Electrical and Computer Engineering Department UBC

CALCULUS I Math mclauh/classes/calculusi/ SYLLABUS Fall, 2003

Visual processing speed: effects of auditory input on

Beyond Classroom Solutions: New Design Perspectives for Online Learning Excellence

A Case-Based Approach To Imitation Learning in Robotic Agents

Special Education Program Continuum

Backwards Numbers: A Study of Place Value. Catherine Perez

Copyright Corwin 2015

OPTIMIZATINON OF TRAINING SETS FOR HEBBIAN-LEARNING- BASED CLASSIFIERS

Physical Features of Humans

Page 1 of 11. Curriculum Map: Grade 4 Math Course: Math 4 Sub-topic: General. Grade(s): None specified

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

The Task. A Guide for Tutors in the Rutgers Writing Centers Written and edited by Michael Goeller and Karen Kalteissen

Montana Content Standards for Mathematics Grade 3. Montana Content Standards for Mathematical Practices and Mathematics Content Adopted November 2011

INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY

Concept Acquisition Without Representation William Dylan Sabo

AGS THE GREAT REVIEW GAME FOR PRE-ALGEBRA (CD) CORRELATED TO CALIFORNIA CONTENT STANDARDS

Scoring Notes for Secondary Social Studies CBAs (Grades 6 12)

Rendezvous with Comet Halley Next Generation of Science Standards

South Carolina English Language Arts

Arizona s College and Career Ready Standards Mathematics

First Grade Standards

5 Early years providers

Abstract Rule Learning for Visual Sequences in 8- and 11-Month-Olds

Vision for Science Education A Framework for K-12 Science Education: Practices, Crosscutting Concepts, and Core Ideas

Introducing the New Iowa Assessments Mathematics Levels 12 14

Department of Communication Criteria for Promotion and Tenure College of Business and Technology Eastern Kentucky University

South Carolina College- and Career-Ready Standards for Mathematics. Standards Unpacking Documents Grade 5

Oakland Unified School District English/ Language Arts Course Syllabus

A Note on Structuring Employability Skills for Accounting Students

Introduction. 1. Evidence-informed teaching Prelude

NCEO Technical Report 27

Notes on The Sciences of the Artificial Adapted from a shorter document written for course (Deciding What to Design) 1

Lecturer Promotion Process (November 8, 2016)

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

GCSE Mathematics B (Linear) Mark Scheme for November Component J567/04: Mathematics Paper 4 (Higher) General Certificate of Secondary Education

Promotion and Tenure Guidelines. School of Social Work

Department of Anthropology ANTH 1027A/001: Introduction to Linguistics Dr. Olga Kharytonava Course Outline Fall 2017

lourdes gazca, American University in Puebla, Mexico

Evaluation of Hybrid Online Instruction in Sport Management

Rule Learning With Negation: Issues Regarding Effectiveness

What is Thinking (Cognition)?

Multiple Intelligences 1

SPATIAL SENSE : TRANSLATING CURRICULUM INNOVATION INTO CLASSROOM PRACTICE

Rule Learning with Negation: Issues Regarding Effectiveness

Within the design domain, Seels and Richey (1994) identify four sub domains of theory and practice (p. 29). These sub domains are:

Speech Recognition at ICSI: Broadcast News and beyond

Sector Differences in Student Learning: Differences in Achievement Gains Across School Years and During the Summer

Declaration of competencies

Anglia Ruskin University Assessment Offences

The Singapore Copyright Act applies to the use of this document.

TIPS PORTAL TRAINING DOCUMENTATION

Early Warning System Implementation Guide

Systematic reviews in theory and practice for library and information studies

The Round Earth Project. Collaborative VR for Elementary School Kids

Unit 3. Design Activity. Overview. Purpose. Profile

CONSTRUCTION OF AN ACHIEVEMENT TEST Introduction One of the important duties of a teacher is to observe the student in the classroom, laboratory and

Assessment and Evaluation

Essentials of Ability Testing. Joni Lakin Assistant Professor Educational Foundations, Leadership, and Technology

Word learning as Bayesian inference

Transcription:

Commentary on the article: From sense of number to sense of magnitude The role of continuous magnitudes in numerical cognition 1. Target Article Authors: Leibovich, Katzin, Harel and Henik 2. Word counts: Word count abstract: 60 Word count main text: 911 Word count references: 309 Word count total: 1389 3. Commentary Title: Infants and number: give credit where credit is due. 4. Author Names: Sophie Savelkouls; Sara Cordes 5. Institution: Boston College 6. Institutional Mailing address: Boston College Department of Psychology 300 McGuinn Hall 140 Commonwealth Ave. Chestnut Hill, MA 02467 7. Institutional Telephone number: 617-552-4112 8. Email addresses: savelkou@bc.edu; cordess@bc.edu 9. Home page URL: https://www2.bc.edu/sara-cordes/lab/

Abstract Leibovich et al. overlook numerous human infant studies pointing to an early emerging number sense. These studies have carefully manipulated continuous magnitudes in the context of a numerical task revealing that infants can discriminate number when extent is controlled, that infants fail to track extent cues with precision, and that infants find changes in extent less salient than numerical changes.

In presenting their case for an acquired number sense dependent upon continuous magnitudes, Leibovich and colleagues overlook a substantial literature revealing that continuous magnitudes across sets of multiple objects - are relatively difficult to track (Barth, 2008). Importantly, the authors rely primarily upon data from adults to support their developmental model, a population shown to invoke advanced estimation strategies while concurrently shying away from more intuitive strategies (Siegler & Booth, 2005). As such, adult data are more likely to reflect learned notions and strategies with regards to number, not intuitions. Evidence from early human development is required to appropriately address questions of inherent abilities. Notably, however, the authors disregard numerous findings with preverbal infants contradicting their claims of the primacy of continuous magnitudes. In particular, these studies reveal infants: (1) can discriminate number when continuous extent is controlled, (2) are relatively poor discriminators of continuous extent, and (3) are more likely to attend to changes in number compared to changes in continuous extent. Similarly to Leibovich et al., we review evidence only pertaining to large, non-symbolic numerosities (>4). First, while it is true that infant abilities to detect numerical changes can be influenced by continuous extent cues (e.g., Cantrell, Boyer, Cordes, & Smith, 2015), habituation studies reveal infants are capable of tracking number independent of these cues. The presence of purely numerical abilities early in development is consistent with a number sense account. Leibovich et al. discount infant habituation methods, arguing that inherent confounds between number and extent prevent studies from isolating numerical abilities. The authors fail to acknowledge a number of sophisticated designs that have taken advantage of the initial habituation phase to dissociate number and continuous extent (Xu & Spelke, 2000). In these designs, infants are

habituated to a series of exemplars of a single numerosity while extent cues are systematically varied; in subsequent test trials, the degree of change in extent across novel and familiar number test trials is equated. As such, these designs allow for one and only one dimension of discrimination: number. Under these circumstances when extent is not a reliable cue for discrimination infants have repeatedly succeeded in detecting changes in number (e.g. Lipton & Spelke, 2003, 2004; Xu & Arriaga, 2007; Xu & Spelke, 2000). If early numerical abilities were entirely dependent upon continuous magnitudes, these robust findings would not be possible. Second, infants track continuous extent with relatively poor precision. If it is the case that infants rely upon continuous magnitudes when comparing sets, then infants should be at least as good at detecting changes in continuous extent as they are at detecting changes in number. This is not the case. Studies that have examined infant abilities to track continuous extent across multiple items have consistently found that infants are remarkably limited in their ability to discriminate extent. In particular, 6-7 month olds require as much as a 4-fold change in cumulative area (Brannon, Abbott, & Lutz, 2004; Cordes & Brannon, 2008) or individual element size (Cordes & Brannon, 2011) and a 3-fold change in contour length (Starr & Brannon, 2015) in order to successfully detect a change in these continuous variables. In contrast, parallel studies isolating numerical abilities reveal that infants of this age reliably discriminate strikingly smaller changes in number (2-fold; Xu & Spelke, 2000). That is, infants notice 2-fold changes in number (when extent is deconfounded), but require the size of individual items in an array to quadruple in size in order to detect a change in extent. Although infants are capable of tracking continuous extent, they are remarkably poor at doing so.

Third, infants find numerical changes to be more salient than extent changes. If infants relied upon continuous extent representations for tracking quantity in the world, this would not only entail a precise ability to track changes in extent, but a greater proclivity to notice these changes over changes in number. Looking-time studies directly pitting changes in number against changes in continuous extent however, demonstrate that infants more readily attend to numerical changes. Cordes and Brannon (2009) pitted number against contour length and cumulative surface area and found that in both cases, 7-month old infants dishabituated to changes in number but not to changes in continuous extent. Additionally, Libertus, Starr, and Brannon (2014) found that even when a 1:3 ratio change in number was pitted against a 1:10 ratio change in cumulative area in a change detection paradigm, infants still did not show a preference for continuous extent, despite this dramatically larger change in the cumulative area. If numerical intuitions rested upon continuous magnitudes, infants would more readily notice changes in extent. They do not. In sum, we argue that a careful reading of the infant literature provides strong support for an intuitive number sense. The finding that infants can track continuous extent in numerical tasks does not undermine evidence from other studies revealing that infants attend to number independent of extent. Yet preverbal infants robust successes on numerical tasks do destabilize claims of an acquired number sense. Moreover, data revealing that infants are relatively poor discriminators of continuous extent and that infants find numerical changes more salient than extent changes weaken assertions that magnitude-tracking underlies numerical processing, posing a significant challenge to the neo-piagetian model posed by Leibovich et al. Whether,

over the course of development into adulthood, representations of number and magnitude become more closely intertwined, remains to be determined; however we believe that the infant data firmly point to an early and intuitive number sense.

References Barth, H. (2008). Do mental magnitudes form part of the foundation for natural number concepts? Don t count them out yet. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31(6), 644 645. http://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x08005591 Brannon, E. M., Abbott, S., & Lutz, D. J. (2004). Number bias for the discrimination of large visual sets in infancy. Cognition, 93(2), B59-68. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2004.01.004 Cantrell, L., Boyer, T. W., Cordes, S., & Smith, L. B. (2015). Signal clarity: an account of the variability in infant quantity discrimination tasks. Developmental Science, 18(6), 877 893. http://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12283 Cordes, S., & Brannon, E. M. (2008). The difficulties of representing continuous extent in infancy: Using number is just easier. Child Development, 79(2), 476 489. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01137.x Cordes, S., & Brannon, E. M. (2009). The relative salience of discrete and continuous quantity in young infants. Developmental Science, 12(3), 453 463. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00781.x Cordes, S., & Brannon, E. M. (2011). Attending to One of Many: When Infants are Surprisingly Poor at Discriminating an Item s Size. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 65. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00065 Libertus, M. E., Starr, A., & Brannon, E. M. (2014). Number trumps area for 7-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 50(1), 108 12. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0032986 Lipton, J. S., & Spelke, E. S. (2003). Origins of Number Sense: Large-number discrimination in Human Infants. Psychological Science, 14(5), 396 401. http://doi.org/10.1111/1467-

9280.01453 Lipton, J. S., & Spelke, E. S. (2004). Discrimination of Large and Small Numerosities by Human Infants. Infancy, 5(3), 271 290. http://doi.org/10.1207/s15327078in0503_2 Siegler, R. S., & Booth, J. L. (2005). Development of numerical estimation. In Handbook of mathematical cognition (pp. 197 212). Starr, A., & Brannon, E. M. (2015). Evidence against continuous variables driving numerical discrimination in infancy. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 923. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00923 Xu, F., & Arriaga, R. I. (2007). Number discrimination in 10-month-old infants. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25(1), 103 108. http://doi.org/10.1348/026151005x90704 Xu, F., & Spelke, E. S. (2000). Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants. Cognition, 74(1), 1 11. http://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-0277(99)00066-9