BUILDING A TWO AXES PROCESS MODEL OF UNDERSTANDING MATHEMATICS*

Similar documents
THEORETICAL CONSIDERATIONS

Proof Theory for Syntacticians

SPATIAL SENSE : TRANSLATING CURRICULUM INNOVATION INTO CLASSROOM PRACTICE

Abstractions and the Brain

THE ROLE OF TOOL AND TEACHER MEDIATIONS IN THE CONSTRUCTION OF MEANINGS FOR REFLECTION

Enhancing Van Hiele s level of geometric understanding using Geometer s Sketchpad Introduction Research purpose Significance of study

PRODUCT COMPLEXITY: A NEW MODELLING COURSE IN THE INDUSTRIAL DESIGN PROGRAM AT THE UNIVERSITY OF TWENTE

Digital Fabrication and Aunt Sarah: Enabling Quadratic Explorations via Technology. Michael L. Connell University of Houston - Downtown

Introductory thoughts on numeracy

FROM QUASI-VARIABLE THINKING TO ALGEBRAIC THINKING: A STUDY WITH GRADE 4 STUDENTS 1

A cautionary note is research still caught up in an implementer approach to the teacher?

Geo Risk Scan Getting grips on geotechnical risks

Strategies for Solving Fraction Tasks and Their Link to Algebraic Thinking

Extending Place Value with Whole Numbers to 1,000,000

Innovative Methods for Teaching Engineering Courses

University of Groningen. Systemen, planning, netwerken Bosman, Aart

Learning from and Adapting the Theory of Realistic Mathematics education

AGS THE GREAT REVIEW GAME FOR PRE-ALGEBRA (CD) CORRELATED TO CALIFORNIA CONTENT STANDARDS

Grade 6: Correlated to AGS Basic Math Skills

Introduction. 1. Evidence-informed teaching Prelude

IBM Software Group. Mastering Requirements Management with Use Cases Module 6: Define the System

DIDACTIC MODEL BRIDGING A CONCEPT WITH PHENOMENA

On-Line Data Analytics

Classify: by elimination Road signs

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

AGENDA LEARNING THEORIES LEARNING THEORIES. Advanced Learning Theories 2/22/2016

MASTER S THESIS GUIDE MASTER S PROGRAMME IN COMMUNICATION SCIENCE

COMPETENCY-BASED STATISTICS COURSES WITH FLEXIBLE LEARNING MATERIALS

The Use of Concept Maps in the Physics Teacher Education 1

Montana Content Standards for Mathematics Grade 3. Montana Content Standards for Mathematical Practices and Mathematics Content Adopted November 2011

1.11 I Know What Do You Know?

A GENERIC SPLIT PROCESS MODEL FOR ASSET MANAGEMENT DECISION-MAKING

AP Calculus AB. Nevada Academic Standards that are assessable at the local level only.

Objectives. Chapter 2: The Representation of Knowledge. Expert Systems: Principles and Programming, Fourth Edition

Arizona s College and Career Ready Standards Mathematics

Evolution of Symbolisation in Chimpanzees and Neural Nets

Update on Standards and Educator Evaluation

ISSN X. RUSC VOL. 8 No 1 Universitat Oberta de Catalunya Barcelona, January 2011 ISSN X

Maximizing Learning Through Course Alignment and Experience with Different Types of Knowledge

Concept Acquisition Without Representation William Dylan Sabo

Math-U-See Correlation with the Common Core State Standards for Mathematical Content for Third Grade

Enhancing Students Understanding Statistics with TinkerPlots: Problem-Based Learning Approach

2 nd grade Task 5 Half and Half

Unpacking a Standard: Making Dinner with Student Differences in Mind

Lecture Notes on Mathematical Olympiad Courses

Missouri Mathematics Grade-Level Expectations

prehending general textbooks, but are unable to compensate these problems on the micro level in comprehending mathematical texts.

Visual CP Representation of Knowledge

UPPER SECONDARY CURRICULUM OPTIONS AND LABOR MARKET PERFORMANCE: EVIDENCE FROM A GRADUATES SURVEY IN GREECE

Defining Numeracy the story continues David Kaye LLU+ London South Bank University

Developing True/False Test Sheet Generating System with Diagnosing Basic Cognitive Ability

Ontologies vs. classification systems

Ministry of Education General Administration for Private Education ELT Supervision

INNOWIZ: A GUIDING FRAMEWORK FOR PROJECTS IN INDUSTRIAL DESIGN EDUCATION

Statewide Framework Document for:

An ICT environment to assess and support students mathematical problem-solving performance in non-routine puzzle-like word problems

Blended Learning Module Design Template

CONCEPT MAPS AS A DEVICE FOR LEARNING DATABASE CONCEPTS

(Still) Unskilled and Unaware of It?

WHY SOLVE PROBLEMS? INTERVIEWING COLLEGE FACULTY ABOUT THE LEARNING AND TEACHING OF PROBLEM SOLVING

NUMBERS AND OPERATIONS

PROCESS USE CASES: USE CASES IDENTIFICATION

Ex-Post Evaluation of Japanese Technical Cooperation Project

Development of an IT Curriculum. Dr. Jochen Koubek Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin Technische Universität Berlin 2008

1. Professional learning communities Prelude. 4.2 Introduction

Are representations to be provided or generated in primary mathematics education? Effects on transfer

Grade 2: Using a Number Line to Order and Compare Numbers Place Value Horizontal Content Strand

THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF

TU-E2090 Research Assignment in Operations Management and Services

OPTIMIZATINON OF TRAINING SETS FOR HEBBIAN-LEARNING- BASED CLASSIFIERS

Notes on The Sciences of the Artificial Adapted from a shorter document written for course (Deciding What to Design) 1

Operational Knowledge Management: a way to manage competence

Shared Mental Models

Achievement Level Descriptors for American Literature and Composition

Physics 270: Experimental Physics

Reducing Abstraction When Learning Graph Theory

Grade 7 - Expansion of the Hudson s Bay Company: Contributions of Aboriginal Peoples in Canada

Inquiry Learning Methodologies and the Disposition to Energy Systems Problem Solving

Self Study Report Computer Science

Analysis of Students Incorrect Answer on Two- Dimensional Shape Lesson Unit of the Third- Grade of a Primary School

REFERENCE FRAMEWORK FOR THE TRAINING OF COOPERATING TEACHERS AND UNIVERSITY SUPERVISORS. (Abridged version)

Concept mapping instrumental support for problem solving

A Case-Based Approach To Imitation Learning in Robotic Agents

Classroom Assessment Techniques (CATs; Angelo & Cross, 1993)

Summary results (year 1-3)

The Effect of Extensive Reading on Developing the Grammatical. Accuracy of the EFL Freshmen at Al Al-Bayt University

MYCIN. The MYCIN Task

Mathematics Scoring Guide for Sample Test 2005

A R "! I,,, !~ii ii! A ow ' r.-ii ' i ' JA' V5, 9. MiN, ;

DEVELOPING GEOMETRIC THINKING SKILLS THROUGH DYNAMIC DIAGRAM TRANSFORMATIONS

Primary Teachers Perceptions of Their Knowledge and Understanding of Measurement

Florida Mathematics Standards for Geometry Honors (CPalms # )

Bruno D Amore: Elementi di Didattica della Matematica. Bologna: Pitagora Editrice II ed Hermann Maier, Regensburg (Germany)

Mathematics Success Level E

MARKETING FOR THE BOP WORKSHOP

Scenario Design for Training Systems in Crisis Management: Training Resilience Capabilities

Professional Development Guideline for Instruction Professional Practice of English Pre-Service Teachers in Suan Sunandha Rajabhat University

Kentucky s Standards for Teaching and Learning. Kentucky s Learning Goals and Academic Expectations

BENCHMARK TREND COMPARISON REPORT:

Foundations of Knowledge Representation in Cyc

Transcription:

HIROSHIMA JOURNAL OF MATHEMATICS EDUCATION 1 : 63-73, 1993 BUILDING A TWO AXES PROCESS MODEL OF UNDERSTANDING MATHEMATICS* Masataka Koyama Hiroshima University, Japan (Received November 24, 1992) Abstract The purpose of this study is to make clear what kind of characteristics a model of understanding mathematics should have so as to be useful and effective in mathematics education. The models of understanding presented in preceding papers are classified into two large categories, i. e. "aspect model" and "process model". Focusing on the process of understanding mathematics, reflective thinking plays an important role to develop children's understanding, or to progress children's thinking from one level to a higher level of understanding. As a theoretical framework, a process model consisting of two axes is presented for further studies. The vertical axis in the model implies levels of understanding and the horizontal axis implies learning stages. At any level of understanding, there are three stages, i.e. intuitive, reflective and analytic stage. INTRODUCTION The word "understanding" is frequently used in descriptions of aims of teaching mathematics in the Course of Study (Ministry of Education, 1989) and in the teaching practices of mathematics in Japan. Placing emphasis on children's understanding is obviously desirable in mathematics education, but what it means is not clear. Moreover, what mathematics teachers should do to help children develop their understanding of mathematics has not been made sufficiently clear. These are essen- *Paper presented at the Sixteenth Annual Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (PME16), University of New Hampshire, USA, August 6-ll, 1992.

54 Masataka Koyama tial and critical problems need to be addressed. The key to the solution of these problems, in my opinion, is to capture what it means for children to understand mathematics and to make clear the mechanism which enables children's understanding of mathematics to develop. In other words, what we need is to "understand" understanding. It is, however, not an easy task and we need our great effort to do it. In fact, as Hirabayashi (1987) describes, the American history of research in mathematics education seems to be the struggling with interpretations of understanding. The problem of understanding is still a main issue being addressed by some researchers, especially those from cognitive psychology in PME. As a result of their work, various models of understanding as the frameworks for describing aspects or processes of children's understanding of mathematics have been developed (Skemp, 1976, 1979, 1982; Byers and Herscovics, 1977; Davis, 1978; Herscovics and Bergeron, 1983, 1984, 1985, 1988; Pirie and Kieren, 1989a, 1989b). The purpose of this study is to address what kind of characteristics a model of understanding should have so as to be useful and effective in mathematics education. In order to achieve this purpose, in this paper, past research related to models of understanding mathematics are summarized and the fundamental conception of understanding mathematics is described. Then, basic components substantially common to the process models of understanding mathematics are discussed. Finally, I present as a theoretical framework a process model consisting of two axes, called "a two axes process model" of understanding mathematics. FUNDAMENTAL CONCEPTION OF UNDERSTANDING MATHEMATICS What do we mean by understanding? According to Skemp (1971), to understand something means to assimilate it into an appropriate schema (p. 43). Haylock (1982) answers this question in the following way: a simple but useful model for discussing understanding in mathematics is that to understand something means to make (cognitive) connections (p. 54). These explanations of understanding are (cognitive) psychological and imply that to understand something is to cognitively connect it to a previous understanding which is called a schema or a cognitive structure. We could say that a schema or cognitive structure is a model of a nerve net in the brain of human beings. In this sense, to understand something is substantially an individual,

A Two Axes Process Model of Understanding 65 internal (mental) activity. Moreover, comparing the Piagetian cognitive structures with the Kantian schemata and categories, Dubinsky and Lewin (1986) theorize that the Piagetian cognitive structures are constructed from the outset and undergo systematic changes of increasing differentiation and hierarchic integration (p. 59). This suggests that understanding as defined above is not a static activity as all-or-nothing but a complex dynamic phenomenon which could change in accordance with the construction and reconstruction of cognitive structures. Therefore, accepting the conception of understanding mathematics as an internal (mental) dynamic activity, we necessarily need some methods to externalize children's understanding of mathematics. A retrospective method, an observation method, an interview method, and a combination of these methods are promising and useful methods for externalizing understanding. It is, however, impossible to directly see understanding when defined as a mental activity. Therefore, we need some theoretical framework. According to the definition of model by Gentner (1983), the theoretical framework for making clear aspects or processes of understanding mathematics could be called a model which has a mental activity of understanding as its prototype. In that sense, any model is indispensable for making clear understanding and the significance of building a model can be found in this point. As mentioned in the previous section, various models of understanding mathematics have been proposed and presented in the preceding papers. These models are, for example, including a discrimination of "relational and instrumental understanding" (Skemp, 1976), "a tetrahedral model" (Byers and Herscovics, 1977), "a 2X3 matrix model" (Skemp, 1979), "a 2 X4 matrix model" (Skemp, 1982), "a constructivist model" (Herscovics and Bergeron, 1983), "a two-tiered model" (Herscovics and Bergeron, 1988) and "a transcendent recursive model" (Pirie and Kieren, 1989b). As Pirie and Kieren (1989b) point out, models can be classified into two large categories. The one is "aspect model" which focuses on the various kinds of understanding and the other is "process modes" which focuses on the dynamic processes of understanding. The models presented in Skemp (1976, 1979, 1982) and Byers and Herscovics (1977) belong to the former and the models in Herscovics and Bergeron (1983, 1988) and Pirie and Kieren (1989) belong to the latter. We need both aspect model and process model in order to develop children's understanding in mathematics education. These models seem to be build mainly to describe the real aspects or processes of children's understanding and they are very useful for us to grasp them. It is, however, not sufficient to describe the real aspects or processes of children's

66 Masataka Koyama understanding. Mathematics education, by its nature, should be organized by both teaching activity and learning activity. Therefore, a model of understanding which is useful and effective in the teaching and learning mathematics should have prescriptive as well as descriptive characteristics. Namely, the model is expected to have the prescriptive characteristic also in the sense that it can suggest us didactical principles regarding to the following questions. What kind of didactical situations are necessary and how should we set them up to help children understand mathematics? To which direction should we guide children, in developing their understanding of mathematics? BASIC COMPONENTS OF PROCESS MODEL In order to build a model of understanding, we must elucidate the processes of children's understanding in mathematics. In this section, focusing on a process model, weexplore basic components of children's understanding. For theoretically exploring them, we examine process models of understanding (Herscovics and Bergeron, 1983, 1988; Pirie and Kieren, 1989) and a model of learning mathematics (van Hiele and van Hiele-Geldof, 1958; van Hiele, 1986). Herscovics and Bergeron have been addressing to the difficult task of building and modifying a model of understanding in the processes of mathematical concept formation. They built "a constructivist model" of understanding mathematical concepts based on the constructivist assumption that children construct mathematical concepts. The constructivist model consists of four levels of understanding: the first one, that of intuition, a second one involving procedures, the third dealing with abstraction, and a last level, that of formalization (Herscovics and Bergeron, 1983, p. 77). Then they modified this model and presented an extended model of understanding. This extended model is called "a two-tiered model", one tier identifing three different levels of understanding of the preliminary physical concepts, the other tier identifing three distinct constituent parts of the comprehension of mathematical concepts (Herscovics and Bergeron, 1988, p. 15). Their fundamental conception underlying this model is that the understanding of a mathematical concept must rest on the understanding of the preliminary physical concepts (p. 20). Pirie and Kieren (1989) stress that what is needed is an incisive way of viewing the whole process of gaining understanding (p.7). And they present "a transcendent recursive model" of understanding which consists of eight levels: doing, image making, image having, property noticing, formalizing, observing, structuring, and

A Two Axes Process Model of Understanding 67 inventing. Their fundamental conception of understanding underlying the model and the important characteristic of the model are succinctly and clearly represented in the following quoted passage. Mathematical understanding can be characterized as levelled but non-linear. It is a recursive phenomenon and recursion is seen to occur when thinking moves between levels of sophistication. Indeed each level of understanding is contained within succeeding levels. Any particular level is dependent on the forms and processes within and, further, is constrainted by those without. (Pirie and Kieren, 1989, p. 8) We can see that these models of understanding are process models which have prescriptive as well as descriptive characteristics and involve some levels of understanding. There is, however, an objection to the levels of understanding. In fact, examining the Herscovics and Bergeron model for understanding mathematical concepts, Sierpinska (1990) argues that what is classified here, in fact, are the levels of children's mathematical knowledge, not their acts of understanding (p. 28). This criticism is based on a different notion of understanding, that understanding is an act (of grasping the meaning) and not a process or way of knowing. It is worth notice but in my opinion there must be some levels, even if those are levels of children's mathematical knowledge, in the processes of children's understanding of mathematics. The process model of understanding mathematics should involve some hierarchical levels so as to be useful and effective in the teaching and learning mathematics. The hierarchy of levels of understanding can be typically seen in the transcendent recursive model illustrated in Figure 1 (Pirie and Kieren, 1989, p. 8). It reminds us of the van Hieles' theory of levels of thinking in learning geometry which was presented in their doctoral dissertation (cf. van Hiele and van Hiele-Geldof, 1958). In the theory five hierarchical levels of thinking are identified and five learning stages for progressing from one level to a higher level are involved (van Hiele, 1986). We notice that these models are very similar to each other in two respects. The one is levels themselves set up and the other is the idea of progressing from a level to a outer (higher) level. The first similarity can be recognized more clearly by illustrating the van Hiele model in Figure 2 (Koyama, 1988a). In fact, ignoring somewhat the difference in the scope and domain of learning mathematics, each of the two levels indicated by a thick

68 Masataka Koyama circle in Figure 1 correspond to the level in Figure 2 as follows: (Doing, Image Making) ~ l (Concrete Object*2, Geometrical Figure) (Image Having, Property Noticing) ~ (Geometrical Figure*, Property) (Formalizing, Observing) - (Property*, Proposition) (Structuring, Inventing) ~ (Proposition*, Logic) The second similarity is more important in a process model than the first, because it is concerned with the crucial idea of developing children's understanding of mathematics. The idea of developing children's understanding in the Pirie and Kieren model in recursion, whereas, in the van Hiele model it is objedification or explicitation. These ideas seem to be substantially similar and might be stated, in other words, as reflective abstraction or reflective thinking. We can say that in the process of understanding mathematics reflective thinking plays an important role to develop children's understanding, or to progress their thinking from a level to a higher level of understanding. Therefore these models suggest to us that a process model should have learning stages involving reflective thinking. Fig. 1 The sign -- indicates the correspondence between levels. The sign * indicates an object of thinking in each level.

A Two Axes Process Model of Understanding 69 Fifth Level fourth Level Third Level Second Level First Level Fig.2 The sign t indicates the objectification of a way of unconscious thinking. After all, we identify two such basic components of a process model as hierarchical levels and learning stages. In the next section, a process model with these two basic components is presented as a theoretical framework for developing children's understanding in the teaching and learning of mathematics. A TWO AXES PROCESS MODEL In order to build a process model which can prescribe as well as describe how the process of children's understanding of mathematics should progress, we must give serious consideration to the following questions. Through what levels should children's understanding progress? How do children develop their thinking in each level of understanding? Relating to the first question, as already discussed, levels involved in the Pirie and Kieren model and the van Hiele model can be regarded as possible answers. In mathematical thinking process, logical thinking and intuition are complementary and closely interrelated. The interaction between logical thinking and intuition in a level has the energy to make another interaction in a higher level possible. Focusing on, for example, the long term process of understanding, we could identify the hierarchical levels in accordance with the characteristic which each object of intuition has

70 Masataka Koyama (Koyama, 1988b). First Level; Intuition of mathematical entities Second Level; Intuition of properties of those entities Third Level; Intuition of relations of those properties Fourth Level; Intuition of relations of those propositions Although we need to examine these levels and modify them in accordance with mathematical concepts intended in the teaching and learning mathematics, they form a vertical axis of the process model of understanding. Relating to the second question, learning stages involved in the van Hiele theory (van Hiele, 1986) and in Dienes theory (Dienes, 1960, 1963, 1970) are very suggestive. On the one hand, in the van Hiele theory five stages in the learning process leading to a higher level are discerned; information, guided orientation, explicitation, free orientation, and integration (van Hiele, 1986, pp. 53-54). On the other hand, in the Dienes theory six stages in the mathematics learning are offered based on four principles, the dynamic, constructivity, mathematical variability and perceptual variability principle (Dienes, 1960, p. 44); free play, rule-bound play, exploration of isomorphic structure, representation, symbolization and formalization (Dienes, 1963, 1970). The stages in these two models can be roughly corresponded as follows: information to free play, guided orientation to rule-bound play, explicitation to exploration of isomorphic structure and representation, free orientation to symbolization, and integration to formalization respectively. According to Wittmann (1981), these corresponding stages are classified into three categories. He emphasizes that three types of activities are necessary in order to develop a balance of intuitive, reflective and formal thinking, based on the assumption that mathematics teaching should be modelled according to the processes of doing mathematics (Wittmann, 1981, p. 395). I have modified Wittmann's definitions of three activities a little in order to form a horizontal axis of the process mode. At any level of understanding, there are three stages, i.e. intuitive, reflective, and analytic stage. Intuitive Stage; Children are provided opportunities for manipulating concrete objects, or operating on mathematical concepts or relations acquired in a previous level. At this stage they do intuitive thinking. Reflective Stage; Children are stimulated and encouraged to pay attention to their own manipulating or operating activities, to be aware of them and their consequences, and to represent them in terms of diagrams, figures or language. At this stage they do reflective thinking. Analytic Stage; Children elaborate their representations to be mathematical ones

A Two Axes Process Model of Understanding 71 using mathematical terms, verify the consequences by means of other examples or cases, or analyze the relations among consequences in order to integrate them as a whole. At this stage they do analytical thinking and at the end they could progress their understanding to a next higher level. Through these three stages (not necessarily linear) children's understanding could progress from one level to a higher level in the teaching and learning of mathematics. As a result, the process model of understanding consists of two axes, called "a two axes process model". In a two axes process model the vertical axis is formed by four hierarchical levels of understanding and the horizontal axis is formed by three stages in any level. BY WAY OF CONCLUSION A model of understanding mathematics should have prescriptive as well as descriptive characteristics so as to be useful and effective in mathematics education as the integration of teaching and learning activities. Based on the assumption, basic components commonto the process models presented in preceding research were explored and two basic components, i.e. hierarchical levels of understanding and learning stages for developing, were identified. By using these components as its two axes, a two axes process model was built to elucidate the process of children's understanding in mathematics education. The validity of this model can be assured indirectly to some extent by corroborative evidences in preceding research related to models of understanding. But the model is a theoretical one and a means to an end. Therefore, by using this model, to grasp the real processes of children's understanding in the teaching and learning certain mathematical concepts and to elabolate or modify it is left as an important task. REFERENCES Byers, V. and Herscovics N., (1977), Understanding School Mathematics. Mathematics Teaching, No. 81, 24-27. Davis, E.J., (1978), A Model for Understanding Understanding in Mathematics. Arithmetic Teacher, Vol.26, No.l, 13-17. Dienes, Z. P., (1960), Building Up Mathematics. Hutchinson Educational Ltd., London.

72 Masataka Koyama Dienes, Z. P., (1963), An Experimental Study of Mathematics-Learning. Hutchinson & Co. Ltd., London. Dienes, Z. P., (1970), Les Six Etapes de L'apprentissage des Structure. Educational Studies in Mathematics, Vol. 3, No. 1, 12-42. Dubinsky, E. and Lewin, P., (1986), Reflective Abstraction and Mathematics Education: The Genetic Decomposition of Induction and Compactness. The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, Vol. 5, 55-92. Gentner, D., (1983), Structure-Mapping: A Theoretical Framework for Analogy. Cognitive Science, Vol. 7, 155-170. Haylock, D.W., (1982), Understanding in Mathematics: Making Connections. Mathematics Teaching, No. 98, 54-56. Herscovics, N. and Bergeron, J. C, (1983), Models of Understanding. Zentralblatt fur Didaktik der Mathematik, Jahrgang 15, Heft 2, 75-83. Herscovics, N. and Bergeron, J. C, (1984), A Constructivist vs a Formalist Approach in the Teaching of Mathematics. Proceedings of the 8th PME, 190-196. Herscovics, N. and Bergeron, J. C, (1985), A Constructivist vs a Formalist Approach in the Teaching of Even-Odd Number Concept at the Elementary Level. Proceedings of the 9th PME, 459-464. Herscovics, N. and Bergeron, J. C, (1988), An Extended Model of Understanding. Proceedings of the 10th PME-NA, 15-22. Hirabayashi, I., (1987), The Activistic Development of Mathematics Education, (a doctoral dissertation, Hiroshima University, 1979). Toyokan, Tokyo, (in Japanese) Koyama, M., (1988a), A Study on Intuition in Mathematics Education. In Hirabayashi, I. (ed)., Perspectives in Mathematics Education, Seibunsha, Tokyo, 175-193. (in Japanese) Koyama, M., (1988b), On the Characteristic Intuition in Each Level of Mathematical Thinking. Bulletin of WJASME: Research in Mathematics Education, No. 14, 9-15. (in Japanese) Ministry of Education in Japan, (1989), The Course of Study for Elementary Schools. Tokyo, (in Japanese) Pirie, S. and Kieren, T., (1989a), Through the Recursive Eye: Mathematical Understanding as a Dynamic Phenomenon. Proceedings of the 13th PME, Vol. 3, 119-126. Pirie, S. and Kieren, T., (1989b), A Recursive Theory of Mathematical Understanding. For the Learning of Mathematics, Vol. 9, No. 3, 7-ll.

A Two Axes Process Model of Understanding 73 Sierpinska, A., (1990), Some Remarks on Understanding in Mathematics. For the Learning of Mathematics, Vol. 10, No. 3, 24-36. Skemp, R. R., (1971), The Psychology of Learning Mathematics. Penguin Books Ltd., Middlesex. Skemp, R. R., (1976), Relational Understanding and Instrumental Understanding. Mathematics Teaching, No. 77, 20-26. Skemp, R. R., (1979), Goals of Learning and Qualities of Understanding. Mathematics Teaching, No. 88, 44-49. Skemp, R. R., (1982), Symbolic Understanding. Mathematics Teaching, No. 99, 59-61. van Hiele, P. M. and van Hiele-Geldof, D., (1958), A Method of Initiation into Geometry at Secondary Schools. In Freudenthal, H. (ed.), Report on Methods of Initiation into Geometry, Groningen Wolters, 67-80. van Hiele, P. M., (1986), Structure and Insight: A Theory of Mathematics Education. Academic Press, Inc., Florida. Wittmann, E., (1981), The Complementary Roles of Intuitive and Reflective Thinking in Mathematics Teaching. Educational Studies in Mathematics, Vol.12, 389-397. Department of Mathematics Education Faculty of Education Hiroshima University 1-1 -2 Kagamiyama Higashi-Hiroshima, 724 Japa n