Understanding Children with Language Problems

Similar documents
Developing Grammar in Context

International Examinations. IGCSE English as a Second Language Teacher s book. Second edition Peter Lucantoni and Lydia Kellas

Advanced Grammar in Use

1 st Quarter (September, October, November) August/September Strand Topic Standard Notes Reading for Literature

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

Writing a composition

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

Houghton Mifflin Reading Correlation to the Common Core Standards for English Language Arts (Grade1)

Word Stress and Intonation: Introduction

ELA/ELD Standards Correlation Matrix for ELD Materials Grade 1 Reading

Taught Throughout the Year Foundational Skills Reading Writing Language RF.1.2 Demonstrate understanding of spoken words,

First Grade Curriculum Highlights: In alignment with the Common Core Standards

Florida Reading Endorsement Alignment Matrix Competency 1

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Characteristics of the Text Genre Informational Text Text Structure

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

Controlled vocabulary

MARK¹² Reading II (Adaptive Remediation)

Coast Academies Writing Framework Step 4. 1 of 7

CELTA. Syllabus and Assessment Guidelines. Third Edition. University of Cambridge ESOL Examinations 1 Hills Road Cambridge CB1 2EU United Kingdom

Year 4 National Curriculum requirements

More ESL Teaching Ideas

Primary English Curriculum Framework

English Language and Applied Linguistics. Module Descriptions 2017/18

BULATS A2 WORDLIST 2

Test Blueprint. Grade 3 Reading English Standards of Learning

Opportunities for Writing Title Key Stage 1 Key Stage 2 Narrative

Introduction to HPSG. Introduction. Historical Overview. The HPSG architecture. Signature. Linguistic Objects. Descriptions.

Literacy THE KEYS TO SUCCESS. Tips for Elementary School Parents (grades K-2)

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

English for Life. B e g i n n e r. Lessons 1 4 Checklist Getting Started. Student s Book 3 Date. Workbook. MultiROM. Test 1 4

Books Effective Literacy Y5-8 Learning Through Talk Y4-8 Switch onto Spelling Spelling Under Scrutiny

CS 598 Natural Language Processing

Language Acquisition by Identical vs. Fraternal SLI Twins * Karin Stromswold & Jay I. Rifkin

PolicePrep Comprehensive Guide to Canadian Police Officer Exams

MARK 12 Reading II (Adaptive Remediation)

2017 national curriculum tests. Key stage 1. English grammar, punctuation and spelling test mark schemes. Paper 1: spelling and Paper 2: questions

Language Change: Progress or Decay?

Characteristics of the Text Genre Realistic fi ction Text Structure

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

Reading Horizons. A Look At Linguistic Readers. Nicholas P. Criscuolo APRIL Volume 10, Issue Article 5

Grade 2 Unit 2 Working Together

Holy Family Catholic Primary School SPELLING POLICY

What the National Curriculum requires in reading at Y5 and Y6

EDEXCEL FUNCTIONAL SKILLS PILOT TEACHER S NOTES. Maths Level 2. Chapter 4. Working with measures

An Introduction to the Composition and Analysis of Greek Prose

Grade 6: Module 1: Unit 2: Lesson 5 Building Vocabulary: Working with Words about the Key Elements of Mythology

Language Acquisition Fall 2010/Winter Lexical Categories. Afra Alishahi, Heiner Drenhaus

HISTORY COURSE WORK GUIDE 1. LECTURES, TUTORIALS AND ASSESSMENT 2. GRADES/MARKS SCHEDULE

Teaching Vocabulary Summary. Erin Cathey. Middle Tennessee State University

Evolution of Symbolisation in Chimpanzees and Neural Nets

Considerations for Aligning Early Grades Curriculum with the Common Core

Dickinson ISD ELAR Year at a Glance 3rd Grade- 1st Nine Weeks

TABE 9&10. Revised 8/2013- with reference to College and Career Readiness Standards

The analysis starts with the phonetic vowel and consonant charts based on the dataset:

GOLD Objectives for Development & Learning: Birth Through Third Grade

5 th Grade Language Arts Curriculum Map

Unit 1: Scientific Investigation-Asking Questions

George s Marvelous Medicine

REVIEW OF CONNECTED SPEECH

Nancy Hennessy M.Ed. 1

Grade 5: Module 3A: Overview

Number Line Moves Dash -- 1st Grade. Michelle Eckstein

BASIC ENGLISH. Book GRAMMAR

Medium Term Plan English Year

Derivational and Inflectional Morphemes in Pak-Pak Language

National Literacy and Numeracy Framework for years 3/4

Lecture Notes on Mathematical Olympiad Courses

Grade 4: Module 2A: Unit 1: Lesson 3 Inferring: Who was John Allen?

THE PROMOTION OF SOCIAL AWARENESS

Guide to Teaching Computer Science

Cambridgeshire Community Services NHS Trust: delivering excellence in children and young people s health services

Table of Contents. Introduction Choral Reading How to Use This Book...5. Cloze Activities Correlation to TESOL Standards...

Senior Stenographer / Senior Typist Series (including equivalent Secretary titles)

Cambridge, New York, Melbourne, Madrid, Cape Town, Singapore, São Paulo, Delhi

Grade 8: Module 4: Unit 1: Lesson 8 Reading for Gist and Answering Text-Dependent Questions: Local Sustainable Food Chain

Reading Grammar Section and Lesson Writing Chapter and Lesson Identify a purpose for reading W1-LO; W2- LO; W3- LO; W4- LO; W5-

Helping Students Get to Where Ideas Can Find Them

WiggleWorks Software Manual PDF0049 (PDF) Houghton Mifflin Harcourt Publishing Company

Unvoiced Landmark Detection for Segment-based Mandarin Continuous Speech Recognition

P a g e 1. Grade 4. Grant funded by: MS Exemplar Unit English Language Arts Grade 4 Edition 1

Informatics 2A: Language Complexity and the. Inf2A: Chomsky Hierarchy

Text: envisionmath by Scott Foresman Addison Wesley. Course Description

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

Rhythm-typology revisited.

CDE: 1st Grade Reading, Writing, and Communicating Page 2 of 27

Copyright 2017 DataWORKS Educational Research. All rights reserved.

UNIT PLANNING TEMPLATE

Linguistics 220 Phonology: distributions and the concept of the phoneme. John Alderete, Simon Fraser University

Comprehension Recognize plot features of fairy tales, folk tales, fables, and myths.

NCU IISR English-Korean and English-Chinese Named Entity Transliteration Using Different Grapheme Segmentation Approaches

Chapter 5. The Components of Language and Reading Instruction

Benchmark Testing In Language Arts

LEXICAL COHESION ANALYSIS OF THE ARTICLE WHAT IS A GOOD RESEARCH PROJECT? BY BRIAN PALTRIDGE A JOURNAL ARTICLE

Name of Course: French 1 Middle School. Grade Level(s): 7 and 8 (half each) Unit 1

Some Principles of Automated Natural Language Information Extraction

Using a Native Language Reference Grammar as a Language Learning Tool

Construction Grammar. University of Jena.

AQUA: An Ontology-Driven Question Answering System

Ch VI- SENTENCE PATTERNS.

Transcription:

Understanding Children with Language Problems SHULA CHIAT City University, London

PUBLISHED BY THE PRESS SYNDICATE OF THE UNIVERSITY OF CAMBRIDGE The Pitt Building, Trumpington Street, Cambridge, United Kingdom CAMBRIDGE UNIVERSITY PRESS The Edinburgh Building, Cambridge CB2 2RU, UK http://www.cup.cam.ac.uk 40 West 20th Street, New York NY 10011-4211, USA http://www.cup.org 10 Stamford Road, Oakleigh, Melbourne 3166, Australia Shula Chiat 2000 This book is in copyright. Subject to statutory exception and to the provisions of relevant collective licensing agreements, no reproduction of any part may take place without the written permission of Cambridge University Press. First published 2000 Printed in the United Kingdom at the University Press, Cambridge Typeset in Photina 10/12pt [VN] A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library Library of Congress cataloguing in publication data Chiat, Shula. Understanding children with language problems / Shula Chiat. p. cm. (Cambridge approaches to linguistics) Includes bibliographical references. ISBN 0 521 57386 6. ISBN 0 521 57474 9 (pbk.) 1. Language disorders in children Case studies. 2. Speech disorders in children Case studies. 3. Children Language. I. Title. II. Series. RJ496.L35C46 2000 618.92 855 dc21 99-23267 CIP ISBN 0 521 57386 6 hardback ISBN 0 521 57474 9 paperback

Contents Acknowledgements Glossary of text conventions and symbols page ix xi Introduction 1 Part I Problems with words 1 What s in a word? 13 2 The child s road to words 17 3 Blocks on the road to words 33 4 Exploring the blockage 40 5 Dant always day dings : problems with phonology 66 6 Stip or step or slip or what?: problems with lexical processing 88 Part II Grappling with verb structure 7 Translating events 113 8 Growing verb structures 127 9 Shortfalls with verbs 144 10 Thing out. Tip in there : problems with verb processing 163 Part III Missing function morphemes 11 Filling out sentences 183 12 That one not working, see : problems with auxiliary verb processing 206

viii Contents Part IV Hidden meanings, baffling meanings 13 The roots of meaning 227 14 [æ] you don t tell nobody this? : strengths in pragmatic processing 243 15 I can speak Chinese. But I can t speak Chinese : problems in pragmatic processing 251 Endpoint and springboard 262 Further reading 267 References 269 Index 279

1 What s in a word? Seven-year-old Steven struggles to name a microphone: S: Oh this is tricky... when you on... it s when you re on... like on a concert... and you get this... micro... scope... no... like thing when you speak into it and it s even louder. (From Hayman 1996) Six-year-old Eamonn requests a favoured toy: E: I want that... deepersiper. SC: The what? E: [jəυ] ( = you know) the deeper... Where deedeepsiper? I dunno deedeepsiper is. Where s the deepbersiper? The deepdeepsiper? Eamonn s search is for the deep-sea diver. Children who have problems with language have problems with words. Typical descriptions of such children state that they are unable to understand words, find words, or say words, or that they omit words or use words in odd ways. Such descriptions are no more than a starting-point for looking at their difficulties. To probe these further, we need to consider what it is the child can t understand, find, say or put in the right place. This means knowing what words are. We can then consider what is involved in coming to understand, find, say and combine them, and what might stop a child from doing one or more of these things. While it seems obvious that intact language users know words, it is by no means obvious what it is that they know. Once we go beyond the obvious to question what it is that we know, we find 13

14 Problems with words that words are not simple things. Each word is a collection of rather special sorts of information. Take the word hair. To know the word hair is to have represented in your mind a connection between the following: Phonological information: the sound pattern of the word. In this case, a single syllable containing the sequence of sounds /hεə/ (in accents where the final r is not pronounced). Semantic information: the meaning to which the sound pattern can refer. In the case of /hεə/, this is the clump of stuff that grows on the human head or in varying quantities on other parts of human beings and on animals. Syntactic information: the position which the word may occupy in relation to other words. This information would include the fact that hair may be preceded by the determiner the and by adjectives such as long, and that it will occur in certain positions within the sentence such as before a verb ( Hair grows ), following certain verbs ( He cuts hair ) and so on. Each of these pieces of information is abstract in the sense that it is picked out or abstracted from another level of information. The phonology of the word is the sound pattern, and not the sound signal which occurs when the word is uttered. Nor does it include every detail of that sound signal. For example, knowing the word hair does not involve knowing the loudness with which it might be uttered. It does involve knowing the consonant vowel sequence /hεə/. This means picking out those features of the sound signal that differentiate it from other sequences of consonant and vowel such as /dεə/ and /ʃεə/ (dare and share). Similarly, the semantics of the word hair does not include every detail of the thing it might refer to. Knowledge of the word does not include the number or shades or length or density of hairs on a particular head. In summary, words are not concrete objects out there in the world. Their existence consists in a connection between phonological, semantic and syntactic information, and that information is picked out from the stream of information we receive.

What s in a word? 15 Different languages, of course, have different words. Most obviously, languages differ in the phonological form which they attach to a meaning. Part of learning a new language is learning the phonological forms of words. But languages also differ in the exact semantics which they attach to phonological forms. For example, the phonological form hair in English refers to the clump of stuff on the head and elsewhere on the body. In contrast, French uses a distinct phonological form for hair on the head (cheveux) and hair on other parts of the body (poil). Languages also differ in the syntactic properties they attach to particular phonological semantic pairings. In English, hair when referring to stuff on the head is not used with plural endings or with a plural verb: Mona Lisa s hair is long is fine, but Mona Lisa s hairs are long is not at least not if the intended reference is to hair on the head! In French, the converse is true, so that the French version of Mona Lisa s hair is long would make hair plural, and in fact mark the verb ( are ) and the adjective ( long ) as plural. Each word, then, is a phonological semantic syntactic complex. To know words is to have stored such complexes in our minds, in what is termed our mental vocabulary or lexicon. But knowing words involves more than knowing each such complex. Our mental vocabulary is not an undifferentiated collection of such complexes. Words share properties with other words. They may be phonologically similar to each other. Hair, for example, is phonologically similar to fare and chair and bear in sharing the rhyme /εə/ (the variable spelling of that rhyme being irrelevant). It is phonologically similar to house in sharing the initial consonant /h/. It is phonologically identical to hare, though spelled differently. Words such as hair and hare, which are phonologically identical but semantically distinct, are known as homophones. Phonological relationships between hair and other words Homophone hare Shared rhyme fare, chair, bear... Shared initial consonant house, hat, hen... Semantically, hair is most similar to words which refer to parts

16 Problems with words of the body such as skin and nail. It has more in common with words referring to concrete substances such as wire or soil or bread than to concrete entities such as table or book. It has even less in common with words referring to abstract things such as idea or message, or to places such as library or park, or to states such as joy or poverty, or to events such as revolution or accident. Syntactically, though, hair shares properties with all these words, which belong to the category noun. This means they may be preceded by a determiner and adjective: Det Adjective Noun the famous skin/nail/wire/soil/bread weird table/book new idea/message library/park joy/poverty revolution/accident But of all the above nouns, hair has most in common with skin, soil and bread, which are distinct from other nouns in certain respects. These nouns may, for example, occur without a determiner where other nouns may not: Hair should be kept clean Skin should be kept clean *Table should be kept clean *Library should be kept clean Nouns like hair (as on the head) form a subcategory known as mass nouns. They are distinguished from other nouns, known as count nouns, in a number of ways, one being the possibility of occurring without a determiner. Looking at just the word hair illustrates amply the idea that a word consists of connections between phonological, semantic and syntactic information, and is connected to other words by virtue of sharing some of that information.