Perception in Optimality Theory: The frugality of the base Patricia Donegan and Paul Lassettre University of Hawai i at Manoa

Similar documents
Parallel Evaluation in Stratal OT * Adam Baker University of Arizona

**Note: this is slightly different from the original (mainly in format). I would be happy to send you a hard copy.**

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

An argument from speech pathology

Pobrane z czasopisma New Horizons in English Studies Data: 18/11/ :52:20. New Horizons in English Studies 1/2016

Universal contrastive analysis as a learning principle in CAPT

The Perception of Nasalized Vowels in American English: An Investigation of On-line Use of Vowel Nasalization in Lexical Access

Markedness and Complex Stops: Evidence from Simplification Processes 1. Nick Danis Rutgers University

Precedence Constraints and Opacity

Listener-oriented phonology

Underlying Representations

Lexical phonology. Marc van Oostendorp. December 6, Until now, we have presented phonological theory as if it is a monolithic

The Odd-Parity Parsing Problem 1 Brett Hyde Washington University May 2008

The presence of interpretable but ungrammatical sentences corresponds to mismatches between interpretive and productive parsing.

SOUND STRUCTURE REPRESENTATION, REPAIR AND WELL-FORMEDNESS: GRAMMAR IN SPOKEN LANGUAGE PRODUCTION. Adam B. Buchwald

1. REFLEXES: Ask questions about coughing, swallowing, of water as fast as possible (note! Not suitable for all

Speech Recognition at ICSI: Broadcast News and beyond

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

1 st Quarter (September, October, November) August/September Strand Topic Standard Notes Reading for Literature

Language Acquisition by Identical vs. Fraternal SLI Twins * Karin Stromswold & Jay I. Rifkin

NCU IISR English-Korean and English-Chinese Named Entity Transliteration Using Different Grapheme Segmentation Approaches

REVIEW OF CONNECTED SPEECH

A Level Playing-Field: Perceptibility and Inflection in English Compounds. Robert Kirchner and Elena Nicoladis (U. Alberta)

Florida Reading Endorsement Alignment Matrix Competency 1

LING 329 : MORPHOLOGY

Som and Optimality Theory

The Strong Minimalist Thesis and Bounded Optimality

Linguistics 220 Phonology: distributions and the concept of the phoneme. John Alderete, Simon Fraser University

Consonants: articulation and transcription

DOWNSTEP IN SUPYIRE* Robert Carlson Societe Internationale de Linguistique, Mali

Phonological Processing for Urdu Text to Speech System

COMPUTATIONAL COMPLEXITY OF LEFT-ASSOCIATIVE GRAMMAR

Derivations (MP) and Evaluations (OT) *

Canadian raising with language-specific weighted constraints Joe Pater, University of Massachusetts Amherst

Towards a Robuster Interpretive Parsing

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

I propose an analysis of thorny patterns of reduplication in the unrelated languages Saisiyat

First Grade Curriculum Highlights: In alignment with the Common Core Standards

CLASSIFICATION OF PROGRAM Critical Elements Analysis 1. High Priority Items Phonemic Awareness Instruction

Acoustic correlates of stress and their use in diagnosing syllable fusion in Tongan. James White & Marc Garellek UCLA

An Interactive Intelligent Language Tutor Over The Internet

Manner assimilation in Uyghur

The analysis starts with the phonetic vowel and consonant charts based on the dataset:

An Introduction to the Minimalist Program

Lecture 1: Machine Learning Basics

Taught Throughout the Year Foundational Skills Reading Writing Language RF.1.2 Demonstrate understanding of spoken words,

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Perceived speech rate: the effects of. articulation rate and speaking style in spontaneous speech. Jacques Koreman. Saarland University

Contrastiveness and diachronic variation in Chinese nasal codas. Tsz-Him Tsui The Ohio State University

Learning and Teaching

Spanish progressive aspect in stochastic OT

A Neural Network GUI Tested on Text-To-Phoneme Mapping

Introduction to HPSG. Introduction. Historical Overview. The HPSG architecture. Signature. Linguistic Objects. Descriptions.

On the nature of voicing assimilation(s)

Different Task Type and the Perception of the English Interdental Fricatives

Books Effective Literacy Y5-8 Learning Through Talk Y4-8 Switch onto Spelling Spelling Under Scrutiny

Ph.D. Linguistics, University of Arizona. Dissertation: Confluence in phonology: evidence from Micronesian reduplication Director: Diana Archangeli

The Journey to Vowelerria VOWEL ERRORS: THE LOST WORLD OF SPEECH INTERVENTION. Preparation: Education. Preparation: Education. Preparation: Education

The phonological grammar is probabilistic: New evidence pitting abstract representation against analogy

Designing a Rubric to Assess the Modelling Phase of Student Design Projects in Upper Year Engineering Courses

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

5. Margi (Chadic, Nigeria): H, L, R (Williams 1973, Hoffmann 1963)

Partial Class Behavior and Nasal Place Assimilation*

Stochastic Phonology Janet B. Pierrehumbert Department of Linguistics Northwestern University Evanston, IL Introduction

Learning Methods in Multilingual Speech Recognition

Criterion Met? Primary Supporting Y N Reading Street Comprehensive. Publisher Citations

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Voiced-voiceless distinction in alaryngeal speech - acoustic and articula

Rhythm-typology revisited.

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

The optimal placement of up and ab A comparison 1

English Language and Applied Linguistics. Module Descriptions 2017/18

Rachel E. Baker, Ann R. Bradlow. Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA

Program in Linguistics. Academic Year Assessment Report

Rover Races Grades: 3-5 Prep Time: ~45 Minutes Lesson Time: ~105 minutes

Learning Disability Functional Capacity Evaluation. Dear Doctor,

Houghton Mifflin Reading Correlation to the Common Core Standards for English Language Arts (Grade1)

On Human Computer Interaction, HCI. Dr. Saif al Zahir Electrical and Computer Engineering Department UBC

SOME MINIMAL NOTES ON MINIMALISM *

Revisiting the role of prosody in early language acquisition. Megha Sundara UCLA Phonetics Lab

STUDIES WITH FABRICATED SWITCHBOARD DATA: EXPLORING SOURCES OF MODEL-DATA MISMATCH

Constraining X-Bar: Theta Theory

Linguistics. Undergraduate. Departmental Honors. Graduate. Faculty. Linguistics 1

Phonetics. The Sound of Language

Eli Yamamoto, Satoshi Nakamura, Kiyohiro Shikano. Graduate School of Information Science, Nara Institute of Science & Technology

THE INFLUENCE OF TASK DEMANDS ON FAMILIARITY EFFECTS IN VISUAL WORD RECOGNITION: A COHORT MODEL PERSPECTIVE DISSERTATION

Language Acquisition Fall 2010/Winter Lexical Categories. Afra Alishahi, Heiner Drenhaus

DIBELS Next BENCHMARK ASSESSMENTS

A Fact in Historical Phonology from the Viewpoint of Generative Phonology: The Underlying Schwa in Old English

Practice Examination IREB

Word Stress and Intonation: Introduction

Informatics 2A: Language Complexity and the. Inf2A: Chomsky Hierarchy

9.85 Cognition in Infancy and Early Childhood. Lecture 7: Number

Demonstration of problems of lexical stress on the pronunciation Turkish English teachers and teacher trainees by computer

have to be modeled) or isolated words. Output of the system is a grapheme-tophoneme conversion system which takes as its input the spelling of words,

GCSE English Language 2012 An investigation into the outcomes for candidates in Wales

SOFTWARE EVALUATION TOOL

Evolution of Symbolisation in Chimpanzees and Neural Nets

Ministry of Education General Administration for Private Education ELT Supervision

Portuguese Vowel Harmony: A Comparative Analysis and the Superiority of Autosegmental Representations

Transcription:

Perception in Optimality Theory: The frugality of the base Patricia Donegan and Paul Lassettre University of Hawai i at Manoa One thread of the Optimality Theory (OT, Prince and Smolensky 1993) literature on acquisition analyzes how children accurately perceive sounds they cannot yet accurately pronounce (Gnanadesikan 1995, Smolensky 1996). The elegant OT analysis uses a single grammar for perception and production. However, the OT account of perception seems to predict that adults will perceive phonetic distinctions with high accuracy, regardless of what those adults can produce. Any adult who has ever studied an unfamiliar language knows that this is not true: distinctions that are perfectly clear to speakers of one language can be incomprehensible to others. Smolensky (1996) notes this, and, in passing, proposes that adults cannot reliably perceive some phonetic distinctions because they cannot use those distinctions to retrieve different lexical items. If speakers do not use some feature distinctively in their own language, that feature will be underspecified in lexical representations. Perceptual mergers occur when phonetically distinct items match the same underspecified lexical entry. We agree that perceptual mergers result from mapping different surface structures onto a single representation. However, we argue that underspecification does not answer the original objection, and propose an alternate model. In the standard model, structural well-formedness constraints evaluate only surface representations. In our model, perceptually grounded structural constraints evaluate the underlying representation as well. The result is a grammar that maps perceptually homophonous surface representations onto identical underlying representations. Our model accounts for the facts of both child and adult phonological perception in terms of constraint ranking, without recourse to a third layer of representation. 1. Modeling perception in OT: Smolensky 1996. Smolensky 1996 presents a model of linguistic perception, building on previous work such as Prince and Smolensky (1993:175 196) and Gnanadesikan (1995). Smolensky s model accounts for the widely-noted discrepancy between linguistic production and perception in young children, who are able to hear the difference between sounds that they cannot pronounce distinctly, using a single grammar for both production and perception. The model makes crucial use of two representations, UNDERLYING REPRESENTATION (UR) and SURFACE REPRESENTATION (SR). Production is modeled as mapping a given UR onto an unknown SR, and perception is modeled as mapping a given SR onto an unknown UR. The discrepancy between children s production and perception corresponds in the model to a more accurate mapping from SR to UR than from UR to SR.

*[+pal] MAX /+pal/ DEP [+pal] *[+pal] MAX /+pal/ DEP [+pal] a. /f s/,[f ] * *! e. /k s/,[k s] b. /f /,[f ] * f. /k /,[k s] *! Perception: given [f ] Perception: given [k s] c. /f /,[f s] * g. /k s/,[k s] d. /f /,[f ] *! h. /k s/,[k ] *! * Production: given /f / Production: given /k s/ Tableau 1. After Smolensky: accurate perception, inaccurate production. An optimality theoretic grammar evaluates candidates for whichever representation is to be discovered. Two types of constraints are used, STRUCTURAL CONSTRAINTS and FAITHFULNESS CONSTRAINTS. Structural constraints evaluate the well-formedness of SRs only. Faithfulness constraints evaluate URs and SRs with respect to each other; cases where UR and SR differ are less optimal than where UR and SR are alike. Thus, the evaluation function does not treat UR and SR equally: UR is evaluated only with respect to its faithfulness to SR, while SR is evaluated with respect both to its faithfulness to UR and to its inherent wellformedness. Moreover, in the early stages of acquisition, structural constraints are assumed to outrank faithfulness constraints (Tesar and Smolensky 1996). In Smolensky s model, the unequal ranking of the two types of constraint, and their unequal treatment of the two representations, underlies the discrepancy between perception and production. Tableau 1 shows the grammar of a child who pronounces fish as [f s], but rejects the same pronunciation by adults, according to our interpretation of Smolensky s model, and using our own notation. The same child pronounces kiss as [k s], and accepts that pronunciation by adults. Candidates are shown as ordered pairs, in which the known member of the pair is fixed and the unknown varies freely. We interpret faithfulness as UR-SR correspondence (McCarthy and Prince 1994, McCarthy 1995). MAX /feature/ is violated when the specified feature in UR does not have an identical correspondent in SR, and DEP [feature] is violated when the specified feature in SR does not have an identical correspondent in UR. In production, the URs /f / and /k s/ are given. Candidates are ordered pairs of UR and SR in which the SR varies freely. The candidate set contains all possible SRs but just one type of UR. The highest ranked of the three constraints shown is a structural constraint, which evaluates the SR. The harmonic ordering of the candidate set will therefore be affected by the well-formedness of the SR, and the winning candidates are [k s] and the unfaithful but well-formed [f s]. In perception, the two SRs [f ] and [k s] are given. As above, candidates

are ordered pairs, but now it is the SR that is fixed. The candidate set includes all possible URs but just one type of SR. All candidates incur equal violations of the structural constraints, and therefore the well-formedness of the SR does not affect the harmonic ordering of the candidate set. Faithfulness constraints, although ranked low, are the only constraints that affect the harmonic ordering of the candidate set, and the winning candidate is the one where the UR and the SR are most alike. The child s ability to hear the difference between [ ] and [s] is reflected in the model by the faithful mapping from [f ] and [k s] to /f / and /k s/. The same child s inability to pronounce / /distinctly from /s/ is reflected by the same grammar s mapping /f / onto [f s]. 2. Perception and richness of the base. RICHNESS OF THE BASE is a term by which Prince and Smolensky (1993:191) refer to the basic OT assumption that the candidate set is unlimited. The implication is that systematic gaps and omissions in a speaker s outputs cannot be explained by gaps and omissions in the input, but must be explained by the phonology itself, in the guise of the optimality theoretic evaluation function. Gaps and omissions in a language s lexicon reflect the surface patterns, not the other way around. Richness of the base is a reiteration of the fundamental OT claim that the locus of explanatory action (Prince and Smolensky 1993:3) is the ranked constraint hierarchy. For example, Standard Hawaiian has no closed syllables. This is not explained by the fact that Standard Hawaiian has no underlying strings that could be parsed into closed syllables has no underlying consonant that is not followed by a vowel. Rather, Standard Hawaiian phonology is such that every possible underlying string, including strings that contain such things as consonant clusters and final Cs, will be parsed into surface open syllables. The explanation lies in the constraint ranking, not in the inputs. Any lack of underlying consonant clusters and final consonants is merely a projection of the surface pattern. Again, Korean has no surface contrast between [l] and [r]. The distribution of these liquids is predictable: [l] occurs syllable finally and [r] occurs elsewhere. This is explained if Korean phonology neutralizes /l/ and /r/ in all environments. The lack of an underlying contrast between the two liquids is due to their surface neutralization. However, as Smolensky notes, the neutralization of Korean [l] and [r] is not just neutralization in production. Korean speakers also neutralize [l] and [r] in perception, tending to perceive English bear and bail, for example, as homophones. The OT model with faithful mapping from SR to UR predicts that these sounds will be perceptually as distinct for Korean speakers as [ ] and [s] (or [l] and [r]) are for the child acquiring English. In a footnote, Smolensky proposes that this effect is due to lexical retrieval: There are underspecified LEXICAL REPRESENTATIONS (LRs). Features that

STRUCTURE FAITH a. /ber/,[ber] * c. /ber/,[bel] * *! b. /bel/,[ber] * *! d. /bel/,[bel] * STRUCTURE FAITH Lexical retrieval: /ber/ bel /bel/ bel Tableau 2. Korean: faithful URs, homophony at lexical retrieval. are not used distinctively in a given language are absent from the LR. In lexical retrieval, a UR is matched to an LR. Because of underspecification, some distinct URs may match the same LR. Failure to perceive a distinction is reflected in this model as matching single LR. This is the case with [l] and [r] in Korean: Korean speakers hear bear and bail as homophones because Korean speakers cannot match them with distinct LRs (tableau 2). This model is indirectly faithful to richness of the base. Perception of homophony gaps and omissions in the perceptual pattern is explained by gaps and omissions underspecified features in the LR. This is not in the spirit of the strongest interpretation of richness of the base, although gaps and omissions in the LR are ultimately explained in terms of the constraint hierarchy, by way of patterns in SRs. 3. Nasal vowels in American English: a paradox. The underspecified LR model does not account for all perceptual phenomena. For example, under this model the LRs of many American English dialects must include nasal vowels. However, speakers of such dialects do not perceive the large number of contrasts entailed by this analysis, or at least not more reliably than Korean speakers distinguish [r] and [l]. American English speakers ordinarily pronounce say, sate, sane, and saint as [sei ], [sei t], [se n], and [se t]. There does not exist any fifth lexical item not homophonous with say, sate, sane, or saint and ordinarily pronounced [se nt], [se ], [sei n], or [sei nt]. No such lexical item is possible. The phonology determines how nasalized vowels are distributed on the surface, by mapping the eight logically possible URs /sei /, /sei t/, /sei n/, /sei nt/, /se /, /se t/, /se n/, and /se nt/, onto the four attested SRs [sei ], [sei t], [se n], and [se t]. Some SRs differ only by a nasalized vowel, and are not neutralized in perception: sate [sei t] and saint [se t], debt [d t] and dent [d t], lip [l p] and limp [l p], duck [d k] and dunk [d k], and so on. Underlying representations are faithful to SRs, and LRs are not distinct from URs. Vowel nasalization is specified in LR, or it is underspecified: there is no other option. If vowel nasalization were underspecified in LR, debt and dent and pairs like them would be perceived as homo-

*V[ nas]n *NC DEP [+nas] *V[+nas] MAX /C/ MAX /+nas/ a. /d t/,[d t] b. /d t/,[d t] *! c. /d t/,[d t] *! * d. /d t/,[d t] * e. /d nt/,[d t] * *! f. /d nt/,[d t] * *! /d [ nas] t/ d [ nas] t /d [+nas] t/ d [ nas] t phones (tableau 3). Therefore, vowel nasalization must be specified in LR to account for these minimal pairs. If this were correct, American English speakers would reliably distinguish the eight srs [sei ], [sei t], [sei n], [sei nt], [se ], [se t], [se n], and [se nt], whether or not they can produce them. We do not believe American English speakers can distinguish all eight any more reliably than Korean speakers can distinguish bear and bail. No scheme of lexical underspecification will condition a realistic pattern of perceptual mergers and distinctions. The standard OT model predicted that adult perception would be highly accurate, more accurate than adult production. The underspecified LR model was intended to account for cases where this prediction was incorrect. Although it is pleasingly simple, and it accounts for Korean perception of liquids, the underspecified LR model cannot account for more complicated examples of the same type, such as American English perception of vowel nasalization. It does not, therefore, solve the problem it was intended to solve. 4. Systematically unfaithful URs. With a modification to the way some structural constraints evaluate representations, the nasalization paradox is resolved. We recognize two classes of structural constraints, which can be distinguished formally and functionally, and also by their phonetic grounding. One of these classes of constraint should evaluate UR as well as SR, while the other class should continue to evaluate only SR. There is no need for a separate lexical representation. One class we call SEGMENTAL WELL-FORMEDNESS (SEGWF) constraints, although we might equally well term them ENHANCEMENT or FORTITIVE constraints. Formally, these constraints refer to simultaneous occurrences, or co- (1) a. *[feature] b. feature * feature i k

occurrences, of distinctive features, although their exact expression depends on the phonological theory used. They may be expressed as constraints on licensing or feature geometry. Our notation is shown in (1). Empirical evidence for SEGWF constraints comes from context-free phonological phenomena, including crosslinguistic patterning of phoneme inventories, unconditioned historical sound shifts, and context-free stylistic variation, as observed in, say, hyperarticulated speech. SEGWF constraints are phonetically grounded in perception, although this does not rule out articulatory grounding as well. That is, using a method such as Hayes s inductive grounding (Hayes 1996), these constraints emerge from perceptual distinctness rather than articulatory difficulty. The other class we call SEQUENTIAL WELL-FORMEDNESS (SEQWF) constraints, although another possible name might be LENITIVE constraints. Formally, these constraints refer to sequences of features (2). Empirical evidence for these constraints comes from context-sensitive phonological phenomena such as assimilation and reduction, and conditioned historical sound shifts. These constraints are grounded in articulation, and function to increase articulatory ease. (2) *[feature i ][feature k ] SEGWF constraints evaluate URs, while SEQWF constraints evaluate SRs only. The result is that in perception URs are not fully faithful to given SRs, although the mappings from SR to UR and from UR to SR are still asymmetrical. Perception of contrast is modeled as mapping of distinct SRs onto distinct URs, as before. Perception of homophony, however, is modeled as mapping of distinct SRs directly onto the same UR. Tableau 4 shows how this resolves the English vowel nasalization paradox. The only SEGWF constraint is *V [+nas], which is violated by any nasal vowel. This is sufficient to change the optimal UR given [d t] from /d t/ to /d nt/, given the ranking DEP [+nas], *V [+nas]» MAX /C/. Thus [d t] and [d t] are associated with distinct URs without allowing underlyingly nasalized vowels in English. This Tableau 4: English Perception 2 *V[ nas]n *NC DEP [+nas] *V[+nas] MAX /C/ MAX /+nas/ a. /d t/,[d t] b. /d t/,[d t] *! c. /d t/,[d t] *! * d. /d t/,[d t] **! e. /d nt/,[d t] **! * f. /d nt/,[d t] * *

STRUCTURE SEQWF SEGWF a. /ber/,[ber] * **! c. /ber/,[bel] * *! b. /bel/,[ber] * * d. /bel/,[bel] * Tableau 5. Korean: homophony at UR. constraint ranking will lead to all surface occurrences of [V ] being perceived as /VN/ by English speakers. This model is capable of handling production and perception of nasalized vowels in English simply and without paradox. The Korean case is resolved in the same way. Bear [ber] and bail [bel] are mapped directly onto the same UR. Like Smolensky, we model the perception of homophony as mapping onto the same representation. Unlike Smolensky, we derive that mapping directly from an optimality theoretic constraint hierarchy evaluating a candidate set. That phonology neutralizes liquids in both perception and production. It should be noted that our model allows the possibility of children learning English who cannot hear the difference between [ ] and [s]. If structural constraints outrank faithfulness initially, children will need to rerank constraints in order to perceive the distinction. This seems to be empirically contradicted by numerous reports that very young children are capable of perceiving any phonetic distinction whatsoever. However, such children are very young, under nine months, while the children at the early stages of language discussed by Smolensky 1996 and Gnanadesikan 1995 are over eighteen months. It is not necessary to assume that perception in the two groups of children works exactly the same. Our model is of linguistic perception, but we assume that both representations, SR as well as UR, are percepts. The surface representation has already undergone significant cognitive processing, and is not a representation of the acoustic wave. Rather, it is a representation of what is heard. We suggest that very young children perceive all possible phonetic distinctions because they treat STRUCTURE SEQWF SEGWF *[+pal] MAX /+pal/ DEP [+pal] DEP [+pal] *[+pal] a. /f s/,[f ] * * e. /f s/,[f ] *! * b. /f /,[f ] **! f. /f /,[f ] ** c. /k s/,[k s] g. /k s/,[k s] d. /k /,[k s] *! * h. /k /,[k s] *! * Tableau 6. Earlier and later linguistic perception. MAX /+pal/

linguistic stimuli similar to other sounds. They respond only to differences in SR. Adults may be conditioned to do the same thing. Korean speakers may well judge that bear and bail do not sound exactly alike even while unable to apprehend the difference linguistically. 5. Conclusion: Frugality of the base. Gaps, omissions, and other patterns in URs are not mere projections of surface patterns. The input to perception must be just as unlimited as the input to production: richness of the base is bidirectional. Gaps and omissions in perception can be explained by the grammar itself, as the result of the interaction of faithfulness and perceptually grounded structural constraints. It is for this reason that we say the base also is frugal. References. Gnanadesikan, Amalia. 1995. Markedness and faithfulness constraints in child phonology. ROA. University of Massachusetts, Amherst. Hayes, Bruce. 1996. Phonetically driven phonology: The role of Optimality Theory and inductive grounding. ROA. UCLA. McCarthy, John. 1995. Extensions of faithfulness: Rotuman revisited. ROA. University of Massachusetts, Amherst. McCarthy, John, and Alan Prince. 1994. Faithfulness and reduplicative identity. ROA. University of Massachusetts, Amherst, and Rutgers University. Ohala, John J., and Manjari Ohala. 1995. Speech perception and lexical representation: The role of vowel nasalization in Hindi and English. In Phonology and phonetic evidence: Papers in laboratory phonology IV, edited by Bruce Connell and Amalia Arvaniti, 41 60. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Prince, Alan, and Paul Smolensky. 1993. Optimality Theory: Constraint interaction in generative grammar. ms. Rutgers University and University of Colorado at Boulder. Smolensky, Paul. 1996. On the comprehension/production dilemma in child language. Linguistic Inquiry 27. Tesar, Bruce, and Paul Smolensky. 1996. Learnability in Optimality Theory (short version). ROA. Johns Hopkins University.