Clausal interference during reference production

Similar documents
A DISSERTATION SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF MINNESOTA BY. Kaitlin Rose Johnson

Linguistic Variation across Sports Category of Press Reportage from British Newspapers: a Diachronic Multidimensional Analysis

Language specific preferences in anaphor resolution: Exposure or gricean maxims?

Morphosyntactic and Referential Cues to the Identification of Generic Statements

A Corpus-Based Study of Demonstratives in German, Russian and English

Frequency and pragmatically unmarked word order *

Anaphoric pronouns for topic devices: theoretical claims and acquisitional evidence

Processing as a Source of Accessibility Effects on Variation

AN EXPERIMENTAL APPROACH TO NEW AND OLD INFORMATION IN TURKISH LOCATIVES AND EXISTENTIALS

Running head: DELAY AND PROSPECTIVE MEMORY 1

Copyright and moral rights for this thesis are retained by the author

A content-addressable pointer mechanism underlies comprehension of verb-phrase ellipsis q

Realization of Textual Cohesion and Coherence in Business Letters through Presupposition 1

SCHEMA ACTIVATION IN MEMORY FOR PROSE 1. Michael A. R. Townsend State University of New York at Albany

Control and Boundedness

The Role of Test Expectancy in the Build-Up of Proactive Interference in Long-Term Memory

Information Structure and Referential Givenness/Newness: How Much Belongs in the Grammar?

Argument structure and theta roles

The Discourse Effects of the Indefinite Demonstrative dieser in German

Underlying and Surface Grammatical Relations in Greek consider

Approaches to control phenomena handout Obligatory control and morphological case: Icelandic and Basque

Formulaic Language and Fluency: ESL Teaching Applications

Annotating (Anaphoric) Ambiguity 1 INTRODUCTION. Paper presentend at Corpus Linguistics 2005, University of Birmingham, England

Abstractions and the Brain

Interactive Corpus Annotation of Anaphor Using NLP Algorithms

Universal Grammar 2. Universal Grammar 1. Forms and functions 1. Universal Grammar 3. Conceptual and surface structure of complex clauses

Constraints on metalinguistic anaphora

Levels of processing: Qualitative differences or task-demand differences?

Writing a composition

The Discourse Anaphoric Properties of Connectives

The Effect of Extensive Reading on Developing the Grammatical. Accuracy of the EFL Freshmen at Al Al-Bayt University

CAAP. Content Analysis Report. Sample College. Institution Code: 9011 Institution Type: 4-Year Subgroup: none Test Date: Spring 2011

The College Board Redesigned SAT Grade 12

Proof Theory for Syntacticians

Second Language Acquisition in Adults: From Research to Practice

To appear in The TESOL encyclopedia of ELT (Wiley-Blackwell) 1 RECASTING. Kazuya Saito. Birkbeck, University of London

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Why PPP won t (and shouldn t) go away

Typing versus thinking aloud when reading: Implications for computer-based assessment and training tools

Good Enough Language Processing: A Satisficing Approach

Rote rehearsal and spacing effects in the free recall of pure and mixed lists. By: Peter P.J.L. Verkoeijen and Peter F. Delaney

The Effects of Strategic Planning and Topic Familiarity on Iranian Intermediate EFL Learners Written Performance in TBLT

Ch VI- SENTENCE PATTERNS.

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 143 ( 2014 ) CY-ICER Teacher intervention in the process of L2 writing acquisition

Ideology and corpora in two languages. Rachelle Freake Queen Mary, University of London

Understanding the Relationship between Comprehension and Production

ON THE COGNITIVE STATUS OF ANTECEDENTS IN SPANISH DISCOURSE ANAPHORA

Strategies for Solving Fraction Tasks and Their Link to Algebraic Thinking

UCLA Issues in Applied Linguistics

Pragmatic Functions of Discourse Markers: A Review of Related Literature

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

Describing Motion Events in Adult L2 Spanish Narratives

Eye Movements in Speech Technologies: an overview of current research

Phenomena of gender attraction in Polish *

Case government vs Case agreement: modelling Modern Greek case attraction phenomena in LFG

An Evaluation of the Interactive-Activation Model Using Masked Partial-Word Priming. Jason R. Perry. University of Western Ontario. Stephen J.

Modal Verbs for the Advice Move in Advice Columns

Good-Enough Representations in Language Comprehension

LEXICAL COHESION ANALYSIS OF THE ARTICLE WHAT IS A GOOD RESEARCH PROJECT? BY BRIAN PALTRIDGE A JOURNAL ARTICLE

Syntactic Ambiguity Resolution in Sentence Processing: New Evidence from a Morphologically Rich Language

Rubric for Scoring English 1 Unit 1, Rhetorical Analysis

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 154 ( 2014 )

Gricean Communication and Transmission of Thoughts

Lecture 9. The Semantic Typology of Indefinites

THE EFFECTS OF TASK COMPLEXITY ALONG RESOURCE-DIRECTING AND RESOURCE-DISPERSING FACTORS ON EFL LEARNERS WRITTEN PERFORMANCE

The Language of Football England vs. Germany (working title) by Elmar Thalhammer. Abstract

Written by: YULI AMRIA (RRA1B210085) ABSTRACT. Key words: ability, possessive pronouns, and possessive adjectives INTRODUCTION

school students to improve communication skills

Cross-linguistic aspects in child L2 acquisition

Reference As A Cohesive Device

AN INTRODUCTION (2 ND ED.) (LONDON, BLOOMSBURY ACADEMIC PP. VI, 282)

Advanced Grammar in Use

Lecture 2: Quantifiers and Approximation

DIDACTIC APPROACH FOR DEVELOPMENT OF THE JOB LANGUAGE KIT FOR MIGRANTS

Improving Advanced Learners' Communication Skills Through Paragraph Reading and Writing. Mika MIYASONE

SCIENCE DISCOURSE 1. Peer Discourse and Science Achievement. Richard Therrien. K-12 Science Supervisor. New Haven Public Schools

Shyness and Technology Use in High School Students. Lynne Henderson, Ph. D., Visiting Scholar, Stanford

Propositional Anaphora in English: The relationship between so and discourse

Specifying Logic Programs in Controlled Natural Language

Artemeva, N 2006 Approaches to Leaning Genre: a bibliographical essay. Artemeva & Freedman

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

THE FU CTIO OF ACCUSATIVE CASE I MO GOLIA *

LQVSumm: A Corpus of Linguistic Quality Violations in Multi-Document Summarization

AGENDA LEARNING THEORIES LEARNING THEORIES. Advanced Learning Theories 2/22/2016

Lower and Upper Secondary

Getting the Story Right: Making Computer-Generated Stories More Entertaining

The New Theory of Disuse Predicts Retrieval Enhanced Suggestibility (RES)

Age Effects on Syntactic Control in. Second Language Learning

Principal vacancies and appointments

The Effect of Discourse Markers on the Speaking Production of EFL Students. Iman Moradimanesh

Monitoring Metacognitive abilities in children: A comparison of children between the ages of 5 to 7 years and 8 to 11 years

Ontologies vs. classification systems

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 98 ( 2014 ) International Conference on Current Trends in ELT

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

THE ROLE OF TOOL AND TEACHER MEDIATIONS IN THE CONSTRUCTION OF MEANINGS FOR REFLECTION

Accreditation in Europe. Zürcher Fachhochschule

Comparison Between Three Memory Tests: Cued Recall, Priming and Saving Closed-Head Injured Patients and Controls

Word Stress and Intonation: Introduction

POLITICAL SCIENCE 315 INTERNATIONAL RELATIONS

Loughton School s curriculum evening. 28 th February 2017

Transcription:

Clausal interference during reference production Roger P. G. van Gompel (r.p.g.vangompel@dundee.ac.uk) School of Psychology, University of Dundee Dundee, DD1 4HN, United Kingdom Kumiko Fukumura (kumiko.fukumura@strath.ac.uk) School of Psychological Science and Health, University of Strathclyde Glasgow, G1 1QE, United Kingdom Abstract Four sentence completion experiments investigated how language producers refer back to human characters when they are mentioned in different clauses in the preceding sentence. Participants produced fewer pronouns and more names when the characters were mentioned in the same type of clause (both main or both subordinate) than when they were mentioned in different clause types. This suggests that clauses with the same status interfere, thereby making the discourse entities less accessible. Participants also used more pronouns and fewer names when the antecedent was in a main than a subordinate clause and when it was the most recent rather than a more distant clause, indicating that main and recent clauses are most accessible. Keywords: Language production; Reference; Pronouns; Names; Interference; Competition; Clause status; Main and subordinate clause; Recency. Introduction A general assumption in both theoretical linguistics and psycholinguistics is that language producers choice of anaphoric expressions is strongly affected by the salience or accessibility of the referent (e.g., Ariel, 1990; Arnold, 2008; Fukumura & Van Gompel, 2010; Givón, 1983; Grosz, Joshi, & Weinstein, 1995; Gundel, Hedberg, & Zacharski, 1993). If the referent is highly salient or accessible, language producers favour anaphoric expressions such as pronouns that are short and provide little information about the referent, whereas if the referent is less accessible, they tend to use more informative expressions such as full names or definite noun phrases. Various saliency factors have been shown to influence the choice of anaphor. For example, speakers tend to produce more pronouns and fewer names when the referent is the subject than the object in the preceding sentence (Fletcher, 1984; Fukumura & Van Gompel, 2010; Stevenson, Kleinman, & Crawley, 1994), when the referent was more recently mentioned in the preceding discourse (Ariel, 1990; Givón, 1983), when the referent is animate rather than inanimate (Fukumura & Van Gompel, 2011) and when there is no competitor in the linguistic or visual context than when there is (Arnold & Griffin, 2007; Fukumura, Van Gompel, & Pickering, 2010). One other factor that has frequently been argued to affect salience is clausal status, that is, the distinction between main and subordinate clauses. It is often assumed that main clauses provide the main assertion or foregrounded information in the sentence, whereas subordinate clauses contain presupposed, backgrounded information (Levinson, 1983; Matthiessen & Thompson, 1988; Miltsakaki, 2002). This is supported by experimental evidence. In a study by Morrow (1985), participants tended to choose a character in the main rather than subordinate clause as the antecedent of an ambiguous pronoun. In line with this, Cooreman and Sanford (1996) and Price (2008) found that reading times were faster for sentences with pronouns that referred to a character in the main than the subordinate clause. Furthermore, several studies have found that participants referred more often to characters in the main than the subordinate clause (Cooreman & Sanford, 1996; Harris & Bates, 2002; Price, 2008). However, none of these studies examined whether clausal status also affects the choice of anaphoric expression. The current study addresses the question of how clausal status influences anaphor choice. Assuming that information in the main clause is more salient than in the subordinate clause, language producers should produce more reduced expressions and fewer explicit expressions for characters in the main than the subordinate clause. However, clausal status may affect anaphor choice in other ways too. Recent research suggests that the choice of anaphor is affected by similarity-based interference between the referent and competitor entities. There is much evidence that language producers tend to avoid pronouns when the referent has the same gender as another character (the competitor) in the context (Arnold & Griffin, 2007; Clancy, 1980; Fletcher, 1984; Fukumura et al., 2010). Fukumura, Hyönä, and Scholfield (in press) argued that this is not purely due to ambiguity avoidance. They showed that this effect occurred even in Finnish, where pronouns are not marked for gender, so pronouns are ambiguous regardless of whether the competitor has the same or a different gender. They concluded that a competitor that is semantically similar to the referent due to its gender competes with the referent, reducing the referent s activation. This, in turn, results in more explicit anaphors. Other studies provide further evidence for similarity-based interference. Fukumura and Van Gompel (2011) showed that participants produced more explicit referring expressions (more definite noun phrases, fewer pronouns) when both the referent and a competitor were animate or both were inanimate than when they differed in animacy. Fukumura, Van Gompel, Harley, & Pickering (2011) found that speakers produced more explicit referring expressions when they referred to a person

who was in the same situation as a competitor (e.g., both on a horse) than when this person was in a different situation. All these studies investigated interference due to semantic features of the referent and competitor, or semantic properties of the context. However, other types of similarity may also cause interference, including similarity between the clauses in which the referent and competitor are mentioned. For example, in (1a), both Rachel and Douglas are in a main clause, whereas in (1b), Rachel is in the main clause and Douglas is in a subordinate clause. 1a. Main-main clause: Rachel explored the cave and Douglas was lying on the 1b. Main-sub clause: Rachel explored the cave while Douglas was lying on the If clausal interference occurs, then information in the first clause should be less accessible when the second clause is also a main clause (1a) than when it is not (1b). Therefore, when language producers refer to Rachel, they should use fewer pronouns and more repeated names in (1a) than (1b). Clausal interference also predicts stronger interference from Rachel for Douglas in (1a) than (1b), making Douglas less accessible in (1a) than (1b). However, Douglas in (1a) is in a main clause whereas it is in a subordinate clause in (1b), and this should make it more accessible in (1a) than in (1b). Because the clausal interference and clause status effects go in opposite directions, they may cancel each other out, resulting in no differences for reference to Douglas. An alternative account of how clausal similarity may affect anaphor choice is the relative saliency account (cf. Fukumura & Van Gompel, 2010). According to this account, the saliency of the referent is not only determined by factors affecting its own saliency, but also by factors influencing the saliency of competitors: The higher the saliency of the competitors, the lower the relative saliency of the referent. This also predicts that Rachel should be more accessible in (1a) than (1b). Because Douglas is in the main clause in (1a), it should be more salient than in (1b), where it is in the subordinate clause. Therefore, the relative saliency of Rachel should be lower in (1a) than (1b). This should result in fewer pronouns and more names for Rachel in (1a) than (1b). Although this is the same prediction as that of the clausal interference account, the relative saliency account makes a different prediction for Douglas. Because Rachel is in the main clause in both cases, it should not affect the relative saliency of Douglas in (1a/b), so language producers should use more pronouns to refer to Douglas in (1a), where he is mentioned in the main clause, than (1b) when he is mentioned in the subordinate clause. Experiment 1 We tested the predictions of the clausal interference and relative saliency accounts using sentences such as (1). Forty participants were asked to provide a written continuation to the sentences, starting with a new sentence. Forty experimental materials were used, randomly interspersed between 86 fillers with a variety of structures. In addition to conditions (1a) and (1b), we had two additional conditions in which the words in the first and second clause were reversed to counterbalance for the specific words used in the two clauses. The conditions were manipulated as withinsubject and item variables. Figure 1 shows the percentages of pronouns and repeated names for references to name 1 (Rachel in (1)) and name 2 (Douglas) in the main-main (1a) and main-subordinate (1b) conditions. Continuations in which the first word did not refer to either name were excluded. Figure 1: Results from Experiment 1. By-subject and item analyses of variance were conducted on arcsine transformed proportions of pronoun responses. We observed no main effect of recency (name 1 vs. 2) or clause structure (main-main vs. main-sub), but crucially, there was an interaction between these two variables (F1(1, 36) = 17.6, p <.001; F2(1, 35) = 4.2, p =.048). When referring to name 1, participants produced fewer pronouns in the main-main than main-sub condition (F1 = 20.9, p <.001; F2 = 6.3, p =.017), as predicted by both the clausal interference and the relative saliency account. In contrast, there was no effect of clause structure when referring to name 2 (F1 = 1.3, p =.26; F2 < 1). This is consistent with the clausal interference account, but does not support the relative saliency account, because the latter predicts that more pronouns should be used for name 2 in the main-main than main-subordinate condition. Experiment 2 To further test the clausal interference and relative saliency accounts, we compared the main-main condition (2a) with a subordinate-main clause condition (2b). 2a. Main-main clause: Rachel explored the cave and Douglas was lying on the 2b. Sub-main clause:

While Rachel explored the cave Douglas was lying on the According to the clausal interference account, interference for Douglas should be larger in (2a) than (2b). In both conditions, Douglas is in a main clause, but it should get stronger interference from Rachel, when Rachel is also in a main clause (2a) than when Rachel is in a subordinate clause (2b). This should result in fewer pronouns and more names for Douglas in (2a) than (2b). Clausal interference for Rachel should also be larger in (2a) than (2b). However, Rachel is in the main clause in (2a) and in the subordinate clause in (2b), so the clause status effect is in the opposite direction from the clausal interference effect. The results from Experiment 1 suggest that they may cancel each other out. The relative saliency account also predicts fewer pronouns for Douglas in (2a) than (2b). This is because the competitor Rachel is more salient in (2a), where it is in the main clause, than in (2b), where it is in the subordinate clause. As a result, the relative saliency of Douglas should be lower in (2a) than (2b). In contrast, the relative saliency of Rachel is higher in (2a) than (2b) because while competition from Douglas is the same in both conditions, Rachel is in the main clause in (2a) but in the subordinate clause in (2b). Therefore, participants should produce more pronouns for Rachel in (2a) than (2b). Twenty-four participants took part. The method, design and analyses of the experiment were the same as before. Figure 2 shows the results by condition. because it predicted more pronouns for name 1 (Rachel) in the main-main than sub-main condition. Experiment 3 The clausal interference account explains the absence of an effect for name 2 in Experiment 1 and name 1 in Experiment 2 by assuming that the effects of clausal interference and clause status go in opposite directions and cancel each other out. In Experiment 3, we directly compared reference to main and subordinate clauses to provide evidence for an effect of clause status on the choice of anaphor. Although previous studies suggest that information in the main clause is more accessible than in the subordinate clause (Cooreman & Sanford, 1996; Harris & Bates, 2002; Price, 2008), we are not aware of research that shows how this affects anaphor choice. We used conditions with either a main-subordinate (3a) or subordinate-main clause order (3b). 3a. Main-sub order: Rachel discovered the cave while Douglas was lying on the 3b. Sub-main order: While Rachel discovered the cave Douglas was lying on the Twenty-four participants took part. The method, design and analyses were the same as in the previous experiments. Figure 3 shows the results by condition. Figure 2: Results from Experiment 2. We observed a main effect of recency, with more pronouns for the name in the more recent clause: F1 = 15.6, p <.001; F2 = 83.7, p <.001. When referring to name 2, participants produced fewer pronouns in the main-main than main-sub condition: F1 = 4.7, p =.043; F2 = 1.1, p =.31. When referring to name 2, there was no effect of clause structure: Fs < 1. These results are consistent with Experiment 1 and support the clausal interference account. The relative saliency account does not explain the results, Figure 3: Results from Experiment 3. We observed a main effect of clause status: Participants used more pronouns when they referred to the name in the main than the subordinate clause: F1= 8.1, p =.010; F2 = 10.6, p =.003. There was also an effect of recency with more pronouns for the second than first clause: F1 = 11.9, p =.003; F2(1, 31) = 3.4, p =.076. Finally, there was an interaction between the two variables: In the submain order, there were more pronouns for the second than the first name (F1 =16.5, p <.001; F2 =11.3, p =.002), whereas there was no effect in the main-sub order (Fs < 1). Together, the results suggest that both clause status and

recency affect the choice of anaphor: Both variables make name 2 more accessible than name 1 in the sub-main order, whereas the effects cancel each other out in the main-sub order. Experiment 4 Experiment 4 investigated whether clausal interference is stronger when the two clauses are syntactically closely linked. When two clauses are linked by a coordinating connective (4a), their mental representation in the syntactic and discourse structure may be closer than when there is no connective (4b). As a result, interference between the two referents Rachel and Douglas may be larger in (4a) than (4b). This should result in a main effect of clause linkage: The mean proportion of pronouns for the two referents should be lower in (4a) than (4b). 4a. Two coordinated clauses, strong link: Rachel discovered the cave and Douglas was lying on the 4b. Two sentences, no link: Rachel discovered the cave. Douglas was lying on the In addition, we should get a recency effect, as in Experiment 3. This recency effect may be stronger when the two clauses are not linked by a connective (4b) and are therefore in separate processing units than when there is a connective (4a). This predicts an interaction between presence/absence of the connective and reference to the first or second name: More pronouns for name 2 in (4b) than (4a), but fewer pronouns for name 1 in (4b) than (4a). Thirty-two participants took part. The method, design and analyses were the same as before. Figure 4 shows the results by condition. Figure 4: Results from Experiment 4. The analyses showed a main effect of recency with more pronouns and fewer names for name 2 than 1: F1 = 20.9, p <.001; F2 = 32.4, p <.001. There was also a weak interaction between recency and the presence/absence of the connective: F1 = 5.9, p =.022, F2 = 1.7, p =.20, indicating that the recency effect was slightly larger when the two names were in different sentences than in two coordinated clauses. Importantly, there was no main effect of clause linkage (F1 < 1; F2 = 1.5, p =.22): Overall, there were similar proportions of pronouns in two coordinated clauses and two sentences conditions. Thus, there was no evidence that interference is stronger when the clauses are more tightly linked. Conclusions Together, the experiments show that clausal structure affects the choice of anaphoric expression in several ways. First, when clause status is controlled (main-main conditions) or counterbalanced (mean of main-sub and sub-main conditions), language producers choose more pronouns and fewer names for referents in a more recent than a distant clause. This recency effect is slightly weaker when a coordinating connective links the two clauses than when there is no connective. Second, Experiment 3 showed that they also use more pronouns for referents in a main than a subordinate clause. Finally, and probably most interesting, they produce fewer pronouns when the referent is in a clause that is of the same type as another clause in the preceding sentence (both main or both subordinate) than when it is in a clause of a different type. This suggests that two clauses of the same type interfere and thereby reduce the accessibility of the entities that are mentioned in them. As a result, language producers use more explicit expressions. One possibility is that clausal interference occurs if two clauses are syntactically similar. Alternatively, the interference effect may occur because of similarity in information structure: Main clauses contain foregrounded information, whereas subordinated clauses contain backgrounded information (Levinson, 1983; Matthiessen & Thompson, 1988; Miltsakaki, 2002). In either case, our experiments provide evidence that interference during reference production not only occurs due to similarity of semantic properties between the referent and competitors, but also due to non-semantic properties. References Ariel, M. (1990). Accessing noun phrase antecedents. London: Routledge. Arnold, J. E. (2008). Reference production: Productioninternal and addressee-oriented processes. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23, 495 527. Arnold, J. E., & Griffin, Z. M. (2007). The effect of additional characters on choice of referring expression: Everyone counts. Journal of Memory and Language, 56, 521 536. Clancy, P. M. (1980). Referential choice in English and Japanese narrative discourse. In W. L. Chafe (Ed.), The pear stories: Cognitive, cultural and linguistic aspects of narrative production (pp. 127 202). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Cooreman, A. & Sanford, A. J. (1996). Focus and syntactic coordination in discourse. Edinburgh: HCRC publications.

Fletcher, C. R. (1984). Markedness and topic continuity in discourse processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 487 493. Fukumura, K., Hyönä, J, & Scholfield, M. (in press). Gender affects semantic competition: the effect of gender in a non-gender marking language. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition. Fukumura, K., & Van Gompel, R. P. G. (2010). Choosing anaphoric expressions: Do people take into account likelihood of reference? Journal of Memory and Language, 62, 52 66. Fukumura, K. & Van Gompel, R. P. G. (2011). The effect of animacy on the choice of referring expression. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26, 1472 1504. Fukumura, K., Van Gompel, R. P. G., Harley, T., & Pickering, M.J. (2011). How does similarity-based interference affect the choice of referring expression? Journal of Memory and Language, 65, 331 344. Fukumura, K., Van Gompel, R. P. G., & Pickering, M. J. (2010). The use of visual context during the production of referring expressions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 1700 1715. Givón, T. (1983). Topic continuity in discourse: An introduction. In T. Givón (Ed.), Topic continuity in discourse: A quantitative cross-language study (pp. 1 41). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Grosz, B. J., Joshi, A. K., & Weinstein, S. (1995). Centering: A framework for modelling the local coherence of discourse. Computational Linguistics, 21, 203 225. Gundel, J. K., Hedberg, N., & Zacharski, R. (1993). Cognitive status and the form of anaphoric expressions in discourse. Language, 69, 274 307. Harris, C. L. & Bates, E. A. (2002). Clausal backgrounding and promominal reference: A functionalist approach to c- command. Language and Cognitive Processes, 17, 237 269. Levinson, Stephen C. (1983). Pragmatics. Cambridge Textbooks in Linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Matthiessen, C. & Thompson, S. A. (1988). The structure of discourse and 'subordination'. In J. Haiman & S. A. Thompson (Eds.), Clause combining in grammar and discourse language (pp. 275 329). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Miltsakaki (2002). Toward an aposynthesis of topic continuity and intrasentential anaphora. Computational Linguistics, 28, 319 355. Morrow, D. G. (1985). Prominent characters and events organize narrative understanding. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 304 319. Price, J. M. (2008) The use of focus cues in healthy ageing. PhD thesis, University of Glasgow. Stevenson, R. J., Crawley, R. A., & Kleinman, D. (1994). Thematic roles, focus and the representation of events. Language and Cognitive Processes, 9, 519 548.