INTD0112 Introduction to Linguistics

Similar documents
Linguistics 220 Phonology: distributions and the concept of the phoneme. John Alderete, Simon Fraser University

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

The analysis starts with the phonetic vowel and consonant charts based on the dataset:

Universal contrastive analysis as a learning principle in CAPT

Lexical phonology. Marc van Oostendorp. December 6, Until now, we have presented phonological theory as if it is a monolithic

Consonants: articulation and transcription

**Note: this is slightly different from the original (mainly in format). I would be happy to send you a hard copy.**

Handout #8. Neutralization

Underlying Representations

Pobrane z czasopisma New Horizons in English Studies Data: 18/11/ :52:20. New Horizons in English Studies 1/2016

The Perception of Nasalized Vowels in American English: An Investigation of On-line Use of Vowel Nasalization in Lexical Access

English Language and Applied Linguistics. Module Descriptions 2017/18

English for Life. B e g i n n e r. Lessons 1 4 Checklist Getting Started. Student s Book 3 Date. Workbook. MultiROM. Test 1 4

Phonological Processing for Urdu Text to Speech System

1 st Quarter (September, October, November) August/September Strand Topic Standard Notes Reading for Literature

DOWNSTEP IN SUPYIRE* Robert Carlson Societe Internationale de Linguistique, Mali

have to be modeled) or isolated words. Output of the system is a grapheme-tophoneme conversion system which takes as its input the spelling of words,

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

Florida Reading Endorsement Alignment Matrix Competency 1

Parallel Evaluation in Stratal OT * Adam Baker University of Arizona

ABSTRACT. Some children with speech sound disorders (SSD) have difficulty with literacyrelated

The Journey to Vowelerria VOWEL ERRORS: THE LOST WORLD OF SPEECH INTERVENTION. Preparation: Education. Preparation: Education. Preparation: Education

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Voiced-voiceless distinction in alaryngeal speech - acoustic and articula

DIBELS Next BENCHMARK ASSESSMENTS

A Neural Network GUI Tested on Text-To-Phoneme Mapping

Contrastiveness and diachronic variation in Chinese nasal codas. Tsz-Him Tsui The Ohio State University

On the nature of voicing assimilation(s)

LNGT0101 Introduction to Linguistics

Learning Methods in Multilingual Speech Recognition

Precedence Constraints and Opacity

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

Phonetics. The Sound of Language

A Fact in Historical Phonology from the Viewpoint of Generative Phonology: The Underlying Schwa in Old English

Linguistics. Undergraduate. Departmental Honors. Graduate. Faculty. Linguistics 1

Understanding and Supporting Dyslexia Godstone Village School. January 2017

DegreeWorks Advisor Reference Guide

University of Waterloo School of Accountancy. AFM 102: Introductory Management Accounting. Fall Term 2004: Section 4

The influence of orthographic transparency on word recognition. by dyslexic and normal readers

Phonology Revisited: Sor3ng Out the PH Factors in Reading and Spelling Development. Indiana, November, 2015

Using SAM Central With iread

Books Effective Literacy Y5-8 Learning Through Talk Y4-8 Switch onto Spelling Spelling Under Scrutiny

Rhythm-typology revisited.

Revisiting the role of prosody in early language acquisition. Megha Sundara UCLA Phonetics Lab

Tutorial on Paradigms

CAS LX 522 Syntax I. Long-distance wh-movement. Long distance wh-movement. Islands. Islands. Locality. NP Sea. NP Sea

Large Kindergarten Centers Icons

SEGMENTAL FEATURES IN SPONTANEOUS AND READ-ALOUD FINNISH

5. Margi (Chadic, Nigeria): H, L, R (Williams 1973, Hoffmann 1963)

Reading Horizons. A Look At Linguistic Readers. Nicholas P. Criscuolo APRIL Volume 10, Issue Article 5

Speech Recognition at ICSI: Broadcast News and beyond

Constraining X-Bar: Theta Theory

TASK 2: INSTRUCTION COMMENTARY

Disambiguation of Thai Personal Name from Online News Articles

ROSETTA STONE PRODUCT OVERVIEW

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Language Acquisition by Identical vs. Fraternal SLI Twins * Karin Stromswold & Jay I. Rifkin

SOUND STRUCTURE REPRESENTATION, REPAIR AND WELL-FORMEDNESS: GRAMMAR IN SPOKEN LANGUAGE PRODUCTION. Adam B. Buchwald

Correspondence between the DRDP (2015) and the California Preschool Learning Foundations. Foundations (PLF) in Language and Literacy

Theme 5. THEME 5: Let s Count!

Different Task Type and the Perception of the English Interdental Fricatives

1. REFLEXES: Ask questions about coughing, swallowing, of water as fast as possible (note! Not suitable for all

Phonemic Awareness. Jennifer Gondek Instructional Specialist for Inclusive Education TST BOCES

ADDIS ABABA UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF GRADUATE STUDIES MODELING IMPROVED AMHARIC SYLLBIFICATION ALGORITHM

MERRY CHRISTMAS Level: 5th year of Primary Education Grammar:

Using a Native Language Reference Grammar as a Language Learning Tool

Tears. Measurement - Capacity Make A Rhyme. Draw and Write. Life Science *Sign in. Notebooks OBJ: To introduce capacity, *Pledge of

L1 and L2 acquisition. Holger Diessel

A survey of intonation systems

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. VCV-sequencies in a preliminary text-to-speech system for female speech

Radical CV Phonology: the locational gesture *

Fisk Street Primary School

TEKS Comments Louisiana GLE

Consonant-Vowel Unity in Element Theory*

Using Proportions to Solve Percentage Problems I

Perceived speech rate: the effects of. articulation rate and speaking style in spontaneous speech. Jacques Koreman. Saarland University

Special Educational Needs Assessment for Learning. Phil Dexter, British Council, Teacher Development Adviser

On the Formation of Phoneme Categories in DNN Acoustic Models

Getting Started with MOODLE

EUROPEAN DAY OF LANGUAGES

Build on students informal understanding of sharing and proportionality to develop initial fraction concepts.

SELF: CONNECTING CAREERS TO PERSONAL INTERESTS. Essential Question: How Can I Connect My Interests to M y Work?

Tour. English Discoveries Online

CLASSIFICATION OF PROGRAM Critical Elements Analysis 1. High Priority Items Phonemic Awareness Instruction

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

Getting Started with Deliberate Practice

Cross Language Information Retrieval

NAME: East Carolina University PSYC Developmental Psychology Dr. Eppler & Dr. Ironsmith

Arabic Orthography vs. Arabic OCR

Stages of Literacy Ros Lugg

WiggleWorks Software Manual PDF0049 (PDF) Houghton Mifflin Harcourt Publishing Company

Progress Monitoring Assessment Tools

5 Guidelines for Learning to Spell

By Zorica Đukić, Secondary School of Pharmacy and Physiotherapy

Tutoring First-Year Writing Students at UNM

Dyslexia/dyslexic, 3, 9, 24, 97, 187, 189, 206, 217, , , 367, , , 397,

Unit 9. Teacher Guide. k l m n o p q r s t u v w x y z. Kindergarten Core Knowledge Language Arts New York Edition Skills Strand

First Grade Curriculum Highlights: In alignment with the Common Core Standards

Portuguese Vowel Harmony: A Comparative Analysis and the Superiority of Autosegmental Representations

READ 180 Next Generation Software Manual

ELA/ELD Standards Correlation Matrix for ELD Materials Grade 1 Reading

Transcription:

INTD0112 Introduction to Linguistics Lecture #8 Sept 30 th, 2009 Announcements Just a reminder: HW2 due on Friday in my mailbox or by e-mail by 12:30pm. Delay policy applies. First talk in the linguistics series is next Wednesday. There's a five-point of extra credit for writing a onepage report on the talk. Thai tongue twisters: http://learn-thai-podcast.com/blog/learn-thailessons/learn-thai-lesson-tongue-twisterspronunciation-training/ Phonology Phonology While phonetics studies how speech sounds are articulated, what their physical properties are, and how they are perceived, phonology studies the organization of speech sounds in a particular language. As it turns out, while two or more languages may have the same sounds, no two languages organize their sound inventories in the same way. [s] and [S] in Japanese vs. English In both English and Japanese we hear the sounds [s] and [S]: Japanese: [Simasu] do English: [slæs] slash In English, however, the two sounds can distinguish meaning, e.g., [Sor] shore vs. [sor] sore The occurrence of each sound is thus unpredictable in English. [s] and [S] in Japanese vs. English By contrast, in Japanese, we do not find pairs where [s] and [S] create a difference in meaning. Consider these Japanese words: [Simasu] do [higasi] east [sensei] teacher [san] three Can you make a guess where each of [s] and [S] occurs? 1

Phonology Phonology addresses these questions: Which sounds are predictable in a particular language? What is the phonetic context that allows us to predict the occurrence of these sounds? How can we formalize speakers phonological knowledge in rule notation? We discuss this today. Subconscious phonological knowledge Native speakers of a particular language typically treat certain sounds as being the same, even when they are phonetically different, e.g., the [l] in lay and play the [t] in top and stop But other sounds are considered to be distinct: the [l] and the [r] in light and right the [p] and the [b] in pan and ban Phonemes vs. allophones Minimal pairs Phonologists explain the difference by invoking a distinction between phonemes and allophones. A phoneme is a meaning-distinguishing sound, whereas an allophone is a phonetic variant of a particular phoneme. But how do we know if two sounds are two separate phonemes, or are simply phonetic variants of the same phoneme? Answer: Minimal pairs. A minimal pair is a set of two words that have the same sounds in all positions except one. Phonemes or allophones? So, bearing this in mind, let s go back and look at the examples we discussed earlier. Based on the minimal pair light and right, are the [l] and [r] phonemes or allophones in Based on the minimal pair pan and ban, are the [p] and [b] phonemes or allophones in Phonemes or allophones? Based on the minimal pair shore and sore (remember spelling is irrelevant to phonology), are the [s] and [S] phonemes or allophones in How about these further minimal pairs? seat and sit fool and full sip and zip leaf and leave 2

Phonemes or allophones? Now, let s consider the following minimal pairs: a. tar: [t h a ] vs. *[ta ] b. star: [sta ] vs. *[st h a ] Now, here s the question: Are the two sounds [t h ] and [t] phonemes or allophones in Since [t h ] and [t] are not contrastive in English, they are two allophones of the same phoneme, which we might represent here as /t/. (Notice the slash, rather than the square bracket, notation.) Phonemes or allophones? Let s consider another example: play: [pl ej] vs. [plej] Here s the question: Are [l ] and [l] phonemes or allophones in Right. Since they are not contrastive, then they are allophones of the same phoneme, which we may represent here as /l/. Phonemes or allophones? How about nasal vowels in Are they phonemes or allophones? First, let s try to find (or construct) a couple of minimal pairs: a. pin [p h In)] vs. *[p h In] b. pit [p h It] vs. *[p h I)t] Is the contrast here phonemic or allophonic? So, how do languages differ, then? One reason why human languages differ in their sound inventory is that what is considered a phoneme in one language is an allophone in another, and what is an allophone in one language is a phoneme in another. So, for example, the aspiration variation is allophonic in English, but not in Thai. Consider these data from Thai: [paa] forest [p h aa] to split [tam] to pound [t h am] to do [kat] to bite [k h at] to interrupt So, how do languages differ, then? Now, consider nasal vowels in French: gars [ga] lad gant [ga)] glove Are they phonemes or allophones? How about Akan, a Ghanaian language? [ka] bite [ka)] speak [tu] pull [tu)] den [pam] sew [pa)m] confederate Distribution: contrastive vs. complementary From all these examples, you should ve noticed that different allophones occur in different environments, that is, where one of them occurs, the other doesn t, and vice versa, which is not the case with phonemes. We say that allophones occur in complementary distribution, whereas phonemes occur in contrastive distribution. And this is one main distinction between a phoneme and an allophone. 3

Phonemes are abstract entities So, phonemes are meaning-distinguishing sounds, whereas allophones are phonetic variants of the same phoneme that occur in specific contexts. Notice that this means that phonemes are actually abstract entities in your head rather than actual sounds that come out of your mouth. Such physical sounds are allophones, the concrete manifestations of the abstract phonemes. Phonemes are abstract entities The psychological existence of phonemes can be noticed in native speakers slips of the tongue, e.g., key chain [ki tsejn] may come out as [tsi kejn], but never as [ti ksejn]. This shows that [ts] is stored in the mind as a single unit, just as [k] is. Note on transcription Remember the distinction between broad phonetic transcription and narrow phonetic transcription? We can now understand this better in terms of phonemic vs. phonetic transcription. In phonemic transcription, only phonemes are transcribed. In phonetic transcription, the allophones of each phoneme are transcribed. Steps for solving phonology problems Given two sounds and a set of data, the task is to determine if the two sounds are separate phonemes or allophones of the same phoneme. To do that, we proceed methodically. Minimal pairs? Step 1: See if there are any minimal pairs in the data where the two sounds in question are in contrastive distribution. If yes, then the two sounds are phonemes. If not, then proceed to step 2. Overlapping or complementary? Step 2: Find out if the two sounds are in overlapping or in complementary distribution. - If overlapping, then the two sounds are in free variation, hence allophones of the same phoneme. - If complementary, then the sounds are allophones of the same phoneme, in which case state the phonological environments in which each allophone occurs and then move to step 3. 4

Which is underlying, and which is derived? Step 3: Once you determine the environments in which each sound occurs, it is time to determine which one is the underlying form and which one is derived. In most cases, the sound that appears in more environments can be taken to represent the underlying phoneme. Write a rule! Step 4: Now, you are in a position to write a phonological rule that shows the process whereby the allophones are derived from the underlying phoneme. Some phonology problems Consider the following Finnish words: 1. [kudot] failures 2. [katot] roofs 3. [kate] cover 4. [kade] envious 5. [madon] of a worm 6. [ratas] wheel 7. [maton] of a rug 8. [radon] of a track Question: Are [t] and [d] two different phonemes or two allophones of the same phoneme in Finnish? Some phonology problems Now, consider these Tagalog words: 1. [datiŋ] to arrive 6. [dara/iŋ] will complain 2. [dami] amount 7. [marumi] dirty 3. [dumi] dirt 8. [marami] dirty 4. [daratiŋ] will arrive 9. [da/iŋ] to complain 5. [mandurukot] pickpocket 10. [mandukot] to go pickpocketing Question: Are [d] and [R] phonemes or allophones? Sindhi. Italian. Spanish. Russian. Some phonology problems Summary Phonemes are meaning-distinguishing sounds. They are unpredictable. They are abstract entities. Allophones are phonetic variants of the same phoneme. They are predictable. They are the physical sounds we produce. Phonemes become allophones via phonological processes, which are represented formally as phonological rules. We discuss these on Monday. 5

Phonological rules Formalizing our phonological knowledge Informally speaking, a phonological rule takes an underlying form as input, operates on it, and derives a surface form as output. The operation of the rule, however, is subject to a main restriction: it has to occur in a certain phonological environment. Abstractly, we can represent this in the following notation: X Y/ Z Basic definitions: the means changes to ; the slash / means in the environment of ; and the positions the input in the environment (that is before or after the relevant segments that determine the phonological change). What this rule simply says is that an input X is changed to Y when it occurs before Z. Suppose instead that we want to say that X changes to Y after (rather than before) Z. How do we do that? Well, a simple change will get us the required result: X Y / Z Suppose further we want to place a certain restriction on the occurrence of the segment. For example, that it has to occur syllableinitial or at a word boundary. Again, we can come up with two simple notations to indicate this: By convention, we will use σ to indicate a syllable boundary, and # to indicate a word boundary. Now, read the following rules. Can you figure out what they mean? X Y / σ X Y / # 6

In some cases an element in the environment may be optional. How do we represent that in the notation of our rules? Brackets will do the trick. Consider this rule. What does it mean? X Y / (Z) σ Sometimes we might have more than one context for the application of a rule. How do we indicate that using our rule notation? Braces come to the rescue, as in this rule: X Y / Z # The above rule simply means that X changes to Y either before Z or at word boundary. [l]-devoicing Ok, so why don t we look at some concrete examples to see how this works? Let s start with the rule for /l/ devoicing in English. Informally put, the rule says /l/ gets devoiced when following a syllable-initial voiceless stop. How do we represent this in phonological rule notation? /l/ [l ] / σ [voiceless stop] Aspiration How about aspiration of voiceless stops in Voiceless stops become aspirated in English when they occur syllableinitially. How do we represent that in formal rule notation in phonology? [voiceless stop] [aspirated] / σ Vowel nasalization Now, vowel nasalization: In English, vowels become nasalized when they are followed by a nasal consonant. Rule notation: V [nasal] / [nasal] Ok, but how about vowel nasalization in Scots Gaelic? Remember the rule? 7

Deletion How about deletion rules? For these, we use the symbol Ø in the output of the rule (i.e., after the arrow). For example, English speakers delete the [ə] in an open syllable when it is followed by a stressed syllable, as in police [pl iès]. How do we represent this in rule notation? [ə] Ø / C 0 σ C 0 V stressed Epenthesis The Ø comes in handy for phonological rules that insert sounds as well. The key difference here is that the Ø will be in the input to the rule. For example, in some English dialects, consonant clusters of [l] and another consonant are not allowed in syllable-final position. Speakers of these dialects, therefore insert a [ə] to fix the syllable, e.g., milk [milək]. In rule notation, this would be represented as: Ø [ə] / [l] C σ So, which one is derived from the other? Question: Given two allophones of one phoneme in the language, how do we decide which one is the underlying form and which one is the surface form? In other words, which one is derived from the other? As a case in point, we assumed that oral vowels in English get nasalized before nasal consonants. But what would go wrong if we assume instead that nasal vowels get oralized before nonnasal consonants? So, which one is derived from the other? The rule of thumb is this: The form that occurs in a larger number of phonetic contexts is most likely to be the underlying form. The form that is restricted in its occurrence to particular contexts is most likely to be a derived form. The underlying form, thus, is typically the elsewhere form. So, which one is derived from the other? For example, in English oral vowels occur initially, finally, as well as before nonnasal consonants. Nasal vowels, by contrast, occur only before nasal consonants. Conclusion: English vowels are underlyingly oral. Can you extend this reasoning to aspiration in Next class agenda Phonological rules and some phonology problems. Morphology: Chapter 3. 8