Mòoré Tonal Polarity by Juliann Spencer Graduate Institute of Applied Linguistics Student

Similar documents
5. Margi (Chadic, Nigeria): H, L, R (Williams 1973, Hoffmann 1963)

Parallel Evaluation in Stratal OT * Adam Baker University of Arizona

DOWNSTEP IN SUPYIRE* Robert Carlson Societe Internationale de Linguistique, Mali

Som and Optimality Theory

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

The presence of interpretable but ungrammatical sentences corresponds to mismatches between interpretive and productive parsing.

Sari locative noun classes Contents

Houghton Mifflin Reading Correlation to the Common Core Standards for English Language Arts (Grade1)

Taught Throughout the Year Foundational Skills Reading Writing Language RF.1.2 Demonstrate understanding of spoken words,

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

The analysis starts with the phonetic vowel and consonant charts based on the dataset:

The optimal placement of up and ab A comparison 1

Precedence Constraints and Opacity

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

Manner assimilation in Uyghur

I propose an analysis of thorny patterns of reduplication in the unrelated languages Saisiyat

Word Stress and Intonation: Introduction

Using a Native Language Reference Grammar as a Language Learning Tool

Derivations (MP) and Evaluations (OT) *

TABE 9&10. Revised 8/2013- with reference to College and Career Readiness Standards

CS 598 Natural Language Processing

Lexical phonology. Marc van Oostendorp. December 6, Until now, we have presented phonological theory as if it is a monolithic

Constraining X-Bar: Theta Theory

The Odd-Parity Parsing Problem 1 Brett Hyde Washington University May 2008

LING 329 : MORPHOLOGY

Lexical specification of tone in North Germanic

BULATS A2 WORDLIST 2

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

Underlying Representations

1 st Quarter (September, October, November) August/September Strand Topic Standard Notes Reading for Literature

Writing a composition

Derivational and Inflectional Morphemes in Pak-Pak Language

5 Minimalism and Optimality Theory

First Grade Curriculum Highlights: In alignment with the Common Core Standards

Case government vs Case agreement: modelling Modern Greek case attraction phenomena in LFG

Underlying and Surface Grammatical Relations in Greek consider

An argument from speech pathology

Parsing of part-of-speech tagged Assamese Texts

Multiple case assignment and the English pseudo-passive *

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

Reading Grammar Section and Lesson Writing Chapter and Lesson Identify a purpose for reading W1-LO; W2- LO; W3- LO; W4- LO; W5-

MEASURING GENDER EQUALITY IN EDUCATION: LESSONS FROM 43 COUNTRIES

AN EXAMPLE OF THE GOMORY CUTTING PLANE ALGORITHM. max z = 3x 1 + 4x 2. 3x 1 x x x x N 2

Bitonal lexical pitch accents in the Limburgian dialect of Borgloon

Acoustic correlates of stress and their use in diagnosing syllable fusion in Tongan. James White & Marc Garellek UCLA

The Effect of Extensive Reading on Developing the Grammatical. Accuracy of the EFL Freshmen at Al Al-Bayt University

ELA/ELD Standards Correlation Matrix for ELD Materials Grade 1 Reading

Inleiding Taalkunde. Docent: Paola Monachesi. Blok 4, 2001/ Syntax 2. 2 Phrases and constituent structure 2. 3 A minigrammar of Italian 3

Linguistics 220 Phonology: distributions and the concept of the phoneme. John Alderete, Simon Fraser University

Introduction to HPSG. Introduction. Historical Overview. The HPSG architecture. Signature. Linguistic Objects. Descriptions.

The Acquisition of English Intonation by Native Greek Speakers

An Introduction to the Minimalist Program

Senior Stenographer / Senior Typist Series (including equivalent Secretary titles)

MARK 12 Reading II (Adaptive Remediation)

Rachel E. Baker, Ann R. Bradlow. Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA

Ternary rhythm in alignment theory René Kager Utrecht University

L1 Influence on L2 Intonation in Russian Speakers of English

Tutorial on Paradigms

The Acquisition of Person and Number Morphology Within the Verbal Domain in Early Greek

Grammars & Parsing, Part 1:

Listener-oriented phonology

What the National Curriculum requires in reading at Y5 and Y6

An Interface between Prosodic Phonology and Syntax in Kurdish

Markedness and Complex Stops: Evidence from Simplification Processes 1. Nick Danis Rutgers University

The Strong Minimalist Thesis and Bounded Optimality

Radical CV Phonology: the locational gesture *

The Structure of Relative Clauses in Maay Maay By Elly Zimmer

Books Effective Literacy Y5-8 Learning Through Talk Y4-8 Switch onto Spelling Spelling Under Scrutiny

Copyright by Niamh Eileen Kelly 2015

Sample Goals and Benchmarks

Name of Course: French 1 Middle School. Grade Level(s): 7 and 8 (half each) Unit 1

INSTANT VOCABULARY 6-10

High Tone in Moro: Effects of Prosodic Categories and Morphological Domains * Peter Jenks (Harvard University) and Sharon Rose (UC San Diego)

MOBILE OBJECT MARKERS IN MORO: THE ROLE OF TONE. University of California, Berkeley University of California, San Diego

Phonetic Knowledge in Tonal Adaptation: Mandarin and English. Loanwords in Lhasa Tibetan*

The Bruins I.C.E. School

**Note: this is slightly different from the original (mainly in format). I would be happy to send you a hard copy.**

Context Free Grammars. Many slides from Michael Collins

Dickinson ISD ELAR Year at a Glance 3rd Grade- 1st Nine Weeks

LANGUAGE DIVERSITY AND ECONOMIC DEVELOPMENT. Paul De Grauwe. University of Leuven

Opportunities for Writing Title Key Stage 1 Key Stage 2 Narrative

SKY Journal of Linguistics is published by the Linguistic Association of Finland (one issue per year). Notes for Contributors

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

South Carolina English Language Arts

Mercer County Schools

Weave the Critical Literacy Strands and Build Student Confidence to Read! Part 2

1/20 idea. We ll spend an extra hour on 1/21. based on assigned readings. so you ll be ready to discuss them in class

On the nature of voicing assimilation(s)

Rhythm-typology revisited.

Primary English Curriculum Framework

An Interactive Intelligent Language Tutor Over The Internet

More Morphology. Problem Set #1 is up: it s due next Thursday (1/19) fieldwork component: Figure out how negation is expressed in your language.

Cognitive Modeling. Tower of Hanoi: Description. Tower of Hanoi: The Task. Lecture 5: Models of Problem Solving. Frank Keller.

Control and Boundedness

Large Kindergarten Centers Icons

Considerations for Aligning Early Grades Curriculum with the Common Core

TEKS Comments Louisiana GLE

Informatics 2A: Language Complexity and the. Inf2A: Chomsky Hierarchy

The influence of metrical constraints on direct imitation across French varieties

The Relative Chronology of Accentual Phenomena in the Žiri Basin Local Dialect (of the Poljane Dialect)

Transcription:

Mòoré Tonal Polarity by Juliann Spencer Graduate Institute of Applied Linguistics Student ABSTRACT Mòoré, a tonal language spoken primarily in Burkina Faso, is governed by two high-ranking markedness constraints: tonal polarity and the spreading of high tones. High tone spreading is dominated by tonal polarity in Mòoré nouns, meaning that adjacent tone-bearing units never have identical tones within the same word. This paper describes the interaction between tonal polarity, the spreading of high tones, and other active constraints within the Mòoré language. 1. Introduction Mòoré [mos] is a Niger-Congo language spoken by about 5,000,000 people in Burkina Faso. There are also 17,000 speakers in Mali, and 19,700 speakers in Togo. Alternate names include Mole, Moose, More, Moshi and Mossi (Lewis 2009). The Mòoré language has two tonal levels: high and low (Kenstowicz 1994:390). Mòoré nouns show tonal polarity, as the tone of the number suffix is always the opposite of the tone of the root. Tonal polarity is demonstrated by the following data (Kenstowicz et al. 1988), in which high tone is marked with an acute accent and low tone is left unmarked. sg. pl. gloss kor-gó kor-dó sack roː-gó ro-tó house wób-go wób-do elephant láŋ-go lán-do hole tịː-gá tịː-sé tree keː-gá keː-sé green sáː-ga sáː-se broom wáŋ-ga wám-se hollow gob-ré gwab-á left hand tụb-ré tụb-á ear kúg-ri kúg-a stone bẹ d-re bẹ d-a big 2. Tonal Polarity and the Spreading of High Tones Tonal polarity is a markedness constraint, which can be stated as follows: POLARITY: Roots and suffixes have opposite tones. Another markedness constraint at work in Mòoré is the spreading of high tones. H-SPREAD: High tones spread to an adjacent low tone from left to right. 1

This process can be seen in the following data (Kenstowicz et al. 1988), where nouns are modified by adjectives and used in phrases with the verbs /ko/ give and /zá/ bring (Kenstowicz 1994:391). The word for sack, /korgó/, has a low tone on the first syllable. When it occurs after /ko/, which has a low tone, /korgó/ remains unchanged. However, when /korgó/ occurs after /zá/, which has a high tone, the high tone spreads from /zá/ to /korgó/, as shown in the following examples. ko sáːga ko korgó zá sáːga zá kór gó kor bẹ da kor keːgá zá bẹ da zá kéː gá give a broom give a sack bring a broom bring a sack big sacks green sack big brooms green broom Notice that on nouns having a HL sequence, with high tone on the root and low tone on the suffix, the high tone does not spread from the root to the suffix. H-SPREAD is therefore dominated by POLARITY, as shown in the following tableau. POLARITY» H-SPREAD /ko sáːga/ POLARITY H-SPREAD ì [ko sáːga] * [ko sáːgá] *! [ko saːga] *! * One may ask why POLARITY is necessary as a constraint, since the Obligatory Contour Principle (Leben 1973) also dictates that adjacent tones within the same word must not be identical. However, the OCP does not provide a satisfactory analysis of all examples. In the previous tableau, if POLARITY were replaced by the OCP, the optimal candidates would be [ko sáːgá] or [ko saːga], ungrammatical forms: OCP» H-SPREAD /ko sáːga/ OCP H-SPREAD [ko sáːga] * [ko sáːgá] [ko saːga] The ungrammatical candidate [ko sáːgá] does not necessarily violate the OCP because it could be considered to have a single high tone linked to both syllables. Similarly, [ko saːga] could simply have a single low tone linked to both syllables. POLARITY, as a constraint, is therefore preferable to the OCP. 2

3. Other Constraints in Mòoré In the phrase [zá kór gó], the final high tone is downstepped. When the high tone spreads from [zá] to the first syllable of [korgó], the faithfulness constraint MAX-TONE (Yip 2002) prohibits the original low tone from being deleted. MAX-TONE: Do not delete tones. However, if the low tone remained associated with the first syllable of [korgó], this would violate the markedness constraint NO CONTOUR (Yip 2002). NO CONTOUR: No TBU may be associated with more than one tone. By leaving the low tone intact, but disassociating it from its original tone-bearing unit, both MAX- TONE and NOCONTOUR are satisfied. The low tone remains as a floating tone, which causes a downstep in the following high tone, satisfying POLARITY as well. The floating low tone violates another markedness constraint, NOFLOAT (Yip 2002), but this constraint is dominated by both MAX-TONE and NOCONTOUR. NOFLOAT: Every tone must be associated with at least one TBU. MAX-TONE, NOCONTOUR» NOFLOAT Another potential output for /zá korgó/ would be the faithful candidate [zá korgó], which does not allow the high tone on /zá/ to spread. Such an output would prevent the violation of NOFLOAT, and at the same time satisfy both MAX-TONE and NOCONTOUR. The fact that *[zá korgó] is not realized as an output shows that NOFLOAT is dominated by H-SPREAD, in addition to MAXTONE and NOCONTOUR. Because POLARITY outranks H-SPREAD, it also outranks NOFLOAT by transitivity. POLARITY» H-SPREAD» NOFLOAT The rankings of POLARITY, H-SPREAD, MAX-TONE, NOCONTOUR, and NOFLOAT are shown in the following tableau, although it should be noted that POLARITY does not necessarily dominate MAX-TONE and NOCONTOUR. POLARITY» H-SPREAD» NOFLOAT H-SPREAD, MAX-TONE, NOCONTOUR» NOFLOAT /zá korgó/ POLARITY H-SPREAD MAX-TONE NOCONTOUR NOFLOAT ì [zá kór gó] * [zá kórgó] *! * [zá kôrgó] *! [zá korgó] *! Using the constraint IDENT-TONE instead of MAX-TONE and NOCONTOUR may initially appear to be a simpler analysis, since it replaces two constraints with one. IDENT-TONE: Tonal specifications may not change. However, unlike MAX-TONE and NOCONTOUR, IDENT-TONE would be violated by [zá kór gó], the winning candidate. The failure of IDENT-TONE to provide a satisfactory analysis is demonstrated in the following tableau. All outputs rank equally except for the correct one, which violates NOFLOAT, as well as fatally 3

violating IDENT-TONE twice. Regardless of the order in which the three constraints are ranked, [zá kór gó] will not be the winning candidate. /zá korgó/ H-SPREAD IDENT-TONE NOFLOAT [zá kór gó] **! * [zá kórgó] * [zá kôrgó] * [zá korgó] * The effects of MAX-TONE and NOCONTOUR are further demonstrated by the following data (Kenstowicz et al. 1988): nẹ d-a nẹ d korgó nẹ d sáːga naː-bá naːb kór gó naːb sáːga man man s sack man s broom chief chief s sack chief s broom Once again, high tones spread to an adjacent low tone from left to right, and a floating tone is still preferred over tone deletion or contour tones. However, note that the vowel of a suffix is deleted when it occurs in a phrase-medial position (Kenstowicz 1994:391). This deletion is caused by an additional markedness constraint that I am proposing: DELV. DELV: Delete suffixal vowels in a phrase-medial position. DELV outranks MAX-IO, a faithfulness constraint, as demonstrated by the following tableau: DELV» MAX-IO /nẹd-a korgo/ DELV MAX-IO ì [nẹd korgo] * [nẹda korgo] *! Even when the vowel is deleted, the tone associated with that vowel remains as a floating tone, confirming the hypothesis that MAX-TONE outranks NOFLOAT. H-SPREAD and NOCONTOUR also apply to these data, as demonstrated by the following tableau: 4

/nẹ d-a sáːga/ POLARITY H-SPREAD MAX-TONE NOCONTOUR NOFLOAT ì [nẹ d sáːga] * * [nẹ da sáːga] **! [nẹ dá sáːgá] *!* [nẹ dá sáːga] *! * [nẹ d sáːga] * *! [nẹ d sǎːga] * *! [nẹ d sáːgá] *! * Based on the previous tableau, it may appear that DELV is not necessary as a constraint, since all likely candidates can be eliminated by other constraints, particularly POLARITY and H-SPREAD. However, an examination of the form [naːb kór gó] yields evidence that DELV is indeed a necessary constraint to prevent incorrect outputs, particularly when the input has a LH tonal pattern and is therefore less likely to violate H-SPREAD when it does not violate POLARITY. As shown by the following tableau, if DELV were not a constraint, then the incorrect output [naːbá kór gó] would perfectly satisfy all constraints except NOFLOAT, just like the correct output [naːb kór gó] does. In order for [naːbá kór gó] to be eliminated in favor of [naːb kór gó], DELV must be included as a relevant constraint. Note that there is no evidence that DELV is dominated by any of the other constraints: /naː-bá korgó/ POLARITY H-SPREAD MAX-TONE NOCONTOUR NOFLOAT DELV ì [naːb kór gó] * [naːbá korgó] *! [naːbá kór gó] * *! [naːb korgó] *! [naːb kórgó] *! * [naːb kôrgó] *! 4. Conclusion In summary, POLARITY dominates H-SPREAD, which dominates NOFLOAT. MAX-TONE and NOCONTOUR also dominate NOFLOAT. DELV dominates MAX-IO, and DELV does not necessarily dominate NOFLOAT: POLARITY» H-SPREAD» NOFLOAT MAX-TONE, NO CONTOUR» NOFLOAT DELV» MAX-IO These rankings can be more clearly seen in the following diagram: 5

OCP POLARITY H SPREAD H-SPREAD MAX-TONE, NOCONTOUR MAX-TONE, NOCONTOUR NOFLOAT NOFLOAT DELV DELV MAX-IO References Kenstowicz, Michael. 1994. Phonology in generative grammar. Cambridge: Blackwell Publishers. Kenstowicz, Michael, Emmanuel Nikiema & Meterwa Ourso. 1988. Tonal polarity in two Gur languages. Studies in the Linguistic Sciences 18:77-104. Leben, William. 1973. Suprasegmental phonology. Cambridge: MIT Ph.D. dissertation. Lewis, M. Paul (ed.). 2009. Ethnologue: Languages of the world, 16th edn. Dallas: SIL International. http://www.ethnologue.com/. (26 May, 2010.) Yip, Moira. 2002. Tone. Cambridge Textbooks in Linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 6