Hypothesis: We expected that, relative to mastery praise, socialcomparison. Abstract STUDY 2 STUDY 1. Method. Introduction

Similar documents
The role of self- and social directed goals in a problem-based, collaborative learning context

Sex Differences in Self-Efficacy and Attributions: Influence of Performance Feedback

Science Diaries: A Brief Writing Intervention to Improve Motivation to Learn Science. Matthew L. Bernacki

self-regulated learning Boekaerts, 1997, 1999; Pintrich, 1999a, 2000; Wolters, 1998; Zimmerman, 2000

A Game-based Assessment of Children s Choices to Seek Feedback and to Revise

Paper presented at the ERA-AARE Joint Conference, Singapore, November, 1996.

predictors of later school success. However, research has failed to address how different

Applying Motivation Theories to the Design of Educational Technology

Greek Teachers Attitudes toward the Inclusion of Students with Special Educational Needs

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 171 ( 2015 ) ICEEPSY 2014

Maximizing Learning Through Course Alignment and Experience with Different Types of Knowledge

Ph.D. in Behavior Analysis Ph.d. i atferdsanalyse

Learning and Teaching

The Agile Mindset. Linda Rising.

Learning By Asking: How Children Ask Questions To Achieve Efficient Search

Effects of Trainer Expressiveness, Organization, and Trainee Goal Orientation on Training Outcomes

MOTIVATIONAL AND SELF-REGULATED LEARNING COMPONENTS OF CLASSROOM ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE

Self Study Report Computer Science

Conceptual and Procedural Knowledge of a Mathematics Problem: Their Measurement and Their Causal Interrelations

1GOOD LEADERSHIP IS IMPORTANT. Principal Effectiveness and Leadership in an Era of Accountability: What Research Says

Inside the mind of a learner

Module 12. Machine Learning. Version 2 CSE IIT, Kharagpur

VOL. 3, NO. 5, May 2012 ISSN Journal of Emerging Trends in Computing and Information Sciences CIS Journal. All rights reserved.

Unraveling symbolic number processing and the implications for its association with mathematics. Delphine Sasanguie

K5 Math Practice. Free Pilot Proposal Jan -Jun Boost Confidence Increase Scores Get Ahead. Studypad, Inc.

Debriefing in Simulation Train-the-Trainer. Darren P. Lacroix Educational Services Laerdal Medical America s

A Decision Tree Analysis of the Transfer Student Emma Gunu, MS Research Analyst Robert M Roe, PhD Executive Director of Institutional Research and

TALKING. Sam, an eighth-grade student in Amy s class, IN CLASS Remembering What Is Important About Classroom Talk

University of Toronto Mississauga Degree Level Expectations. Preamble

CONSISTENCY OF TRAINING AND THE LEARNING EXPERIENCE

Essentials of Ability Testing. Joni Lakin Assistant Professor Educational Foundations, Leadership, and Technology

Document number: 2013/ Programs Committee 6/2014 (July) Agenda Item 42.0 Bachelor of Engineering with Honours in Software Engineering

A CORRELATIONAL STUDY OF THE MOTIVATION AND ENGAGEMENT IN TEACHERS: EXPERIENCE AND EFFECTIVENESS

Effects of Anonymity and Accountability During Online Peer Assessment

The My Class Activities Instrument as Used in Saturday Enrichment Program Evaluation

Effective Pre-school and Primary Education 3-11 Project (EPPE 3-11)

Alpha provides an overall measure of the internal reliability of the test. The Coefficient Alphas for the STEP are:

EDUC-E328 Science in the Elementary Schools

Developing a Language for Assessing Creativity: a taxonomy to support student learning and assessment

15 super powers you never knew you had

The Good Judgment Project: A large scale test of different methods of combining expert predictions

Results In. Planning Questions. Tony Frontier Five Levers to Improve Learning 1

An Empirical Analysis of the Effects of Mexican American Studies Participation on Student Achievement within Tucson Unified School District

SCIENCE TEACHERS EFFICACY BELIEFS, MASTERY-FOCUSED INSTRUCTION, AND STUDENTS EFFICACY BELIEFS: A MULTILEVEL STRUCTURAL EQUATION MODEL. Belle B.

SOC 175. Australian Society. Contents. S3 External Sociology

Exploration. CS : Deep Reinforcement Learning Sergey Levine

MOTIVATION FOR READING AND UPPER PRIMARY SCHOOL STUDENTS ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT IN READING IN KENYA

12- A whirlwind tour of statistics

Testing protects against proactive interference in face name learning

CHAPTER 5: COMPARABILITY OF WRITTEN QUESTIONNAIRE DATA AND INTERVIEW DATA

Social Emotional Learning in High School: How Three Urban High Schools Engage, Educate, and Empower Youth

R01 NIH Grants. John E. Lochman, PhD, ABPP Center for Prevention of Youth Behavior Problems Department of Psychology

Math Intervention "SMART" Project (Student Mathematical Analysis and Reasoning with Technology)

UDL AND LANGUAGE ARTS LESSON OVERVIEW

DESIGN-BASED LEARNING IN INFORMATION SYSTEMS: THE ROLE OF KNOWLEDGE AND MOTIVATION ON LEARNING AND DESIGN OUTCOMES

A Bootstrapping Model of Frequency and Context Effects in Word Learning

INTRODUCTION TO DECISION ANALYSIS (Economics ) Prof. Klaus Nehring Spring Syllabus

Collegiate Academies Response to Livingston School Facility RFA Submitted January 23, 2015

Ideas for Plenary Session. Erskine

A. What is research? B. Types of research

The Role of a Theoretical Framework. what the researcher will look for and how data will be sorted. Making a theoretical framework

What effect does science club have on pupil attitudes, engagement and attainment? Dr S.J. Nolan, The Perse School, June 2014

Case study Norway case 1

Effect of Word Complexity on L2 Vocabulary Learning

Entrepreneurial Discovery and the Demmert/Klein Experiment: Additional Evidence from Germany

Sociology 521: Social Statistics and Quantitative Methods I Spring Wed. 2 5, Kap 305 Computer Lab. Course Website

Access Center Assessment Report

ZHANG Xiaojun, XIONG Xiaoliang School of Finance and Business English, Wuhan Yangtze Business University, P.R.China,

Developing efficacy beliefs in the classroom.

Second Step Suite and the Whole School, Whole Community, Whole Child (WSCC) Model

REDUCING STRESS AND BUILDING RESILIENCY IN STUDENTS

Natural Learning in Higher Education

Strategy Abandonment Effects in Cued Recall

The Development of Altruistic Behavior: Helping in Children and Chimpanzees

An Empirical and Computational Test of Linguistic Relativity

National Standards for Foreign Language Education

SIMPLY THE BEST! AND MINDSETS. (Growth or fixed?)

SSIS SEL Edition Overview Fall 2017

Seven Keys to a Positive Learning Environment in Your Classroom. Study Guide

Presentation Format Effects in a Levels-of-Processing Task

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

Analyzing the Usage of IT in SMEs

Quantitative analysis with statistics (and ponies) (Some slides, pony-based examples from Blase Ur)

GETTING THE MOST OF OUT OF BRAINSTORMING GROUPS

What Teachers Are Saying

Recommended Guidelines for the Diagnosis of Children with Learning Disabilities

Stimulating Techniques in Micro Teaching. Puan Ng Swee Teng Ketua Program Kursus Lanjutan U48 Kolej Sains Kesihatan Bersekutu, SAS, Ulu Kinta

Impact Of Mindset Interventions During Morning Meeting On Student Motivation And Classroom Community

Medical Complexity: A Pragmatic Theory

Examining the Use of Play Activities to Increase Appropriate Classroom Behaviors

Professional Teachers Strategies for Promoting Positive Behaviour in Schools

STA 225: Introductory Statistics (CT)

Undergraduates Views of K-12 Teaching as a Career Choice

MMOG Game-Based Knowledge Conversion: An Ecological View Of Mutualistic Co-Presence

MEASURING GENDER EQUALITY IN EDUCATION: LESSONS FROM 43 COUNTRIES

NORTH CAROLINA STATE BOARD OF EDUCATION Policy Manual

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 191 ( 2015 ) WCES Why Do Students Choose To Study Information And Communications Technology?

Analyzing Linguistically Appropriate IEP Goals in Dual Language Programs

Effect of Cognitive Apprenticeship Instructional Method on Auto-Mechanics Students

Cooper Upper Elementary School

Transcription:

Social-Comparison Praise Undermines Intrinsic Motivation When Children Later Doubt Their Ability Jennifer Henderlong Corpus, Kelly E. Love, & Christin M. Ogle Reed College Abstract How does praise for normative excellence versus task mastery affect 4th- and 5thgrade children s intrinsic motivation? The two studies reported here show that mastery praise enhances motivation, and that social-comparison praise undermines motivation when ambiguity about children s performance is later introduced. Introduction Performing at the top of one s class may be highly motivating, but what are the consequences of highlighting children s normative superiority through praise? The achievement goals literature indicates that mastery goals are often more adaptive than goals focusing on normative excellence, especially in the elementaryschool classroom (e.g., Dweck, 1999; Midgley & Urdan, 2001). The praise literature, however, is mixed (see Henderlong & Lepper, 2002). Mastery praise appears to enhance intrinsic motivation relative to neutral feedback, but social-comparison praise does as well (e.g., Deci, 1971; Koestner et al., 1990). Existing studies are limited because (1) dependent measures have been collected when children have no reason to question their performance, and (2) socialcomparison and mastery praise have not been directly compared. Social-comparison praise may create vulnerabilities when children are less certain about their performance in the future, much as praise for intelligence has been shown to leave children vulnerable in the face of failure (Henderlong & Lepper, 2000; Mueller & Dweck, 1998). Hypothesis: We expected that, relative to mastery praise, socialcomparison praise would dampen intrinsic motivation and increase vulnerabilities in the face of subsequent ambiguous feedback. Participants: 86 4 th - and 5 th -grade children (51 girls, 35 boys). Procedure: Children worked on a set of novel puzzles and were given a high score (>80%) and praise (see below). Those in the Neutral Feedback Condition were not given any additional feedback. Social-Comparison Condition: That s among the best work I ve seen from someone your age! ; Most kids don t do as well as that! Mastery Condition: You ve really learned how to solve these! ; You ve really become an expert at these! Next, children worked on a drawing task but did not receive any feedback, in order to create ambiguity about their performance. Children completed a 9-item self-report scale of intrinsic motivation, assessing their curiosity, challenge-seeking, and mastery goals (Lepper, Corpus, & Iyengar, in press). Finally, as a behavioral measure of intrinsic motivation, children selected either a long or short version of a task similar to the initial praised task. Results STUDY 1 Children reported the highest levels of intrinsic motivation in the mastery praise condition, followed by those in the neutral feedback and social-comparison praise conditions, F (2, 80) = 4.03, p <.05. (Fig.1) Children were more likely to select the long versus short task in the mastery praise condition (63%) compared to the socialcomparison praise (30%) or neutral feedback (29%) conditions, χ 2 (2, N = 86) = 8.92, p <.05. (Fig. 2) STUDY 2 Would social-comparison praise still have a detrimental effect if children were given unambiguous feedback that their performance continued to be outstanding? Participants: 78 4th- & 5th-grade children (35 girls, 43 boys). Procedure: Same as Study 1, with two exceptions. Children were told that they earned a really good score on the drawing task and were given the opportunity to look through a folder of other scored drawings that confirmed their strong performance. Results Social-comparison praise no longer had a detrimental effect on intrinsic motivation, relative to mastery praise and neutral feedback, on either self-reported or behavioral measures of motivation (Figs. 1 & 2). 4 3.8 3.6 3.4 3.2 Social- Comparison Figure 1 Mastery Study 1 Study 2 Neutral Feedback 75 60 45 30 15 0 Figure 2 Study 1 Study 2 Social Comparison Mastery Neutral Feedback Analyses combining data from the two studies confirmed that children doubted their performance on the drawing task more in Study 1 than Study 2, t(162) = 5.97, p <.001. Moreover, there were significant Feedback Condition x Study interactions for both selfreported and behavioral intrinsic motivation. Conclusions Mastery praise led to greater intrinsic motivation than social-comparison praise only when uncertainty about performance was subsequently introduced. Praising children for normative superiority may send the message that personal competence is measured by outperforming peers, rather than skill development. This message is harmful when children are in situations that lead them to doubt their ability. References Deci, E. L. (1971). Effects of externally mediated rewards on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 18, 105-115. Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. Philadelphia: Psychology Press. Henderlong, J., & Lepper, M. R. (2002). The effects of praise on children s intrinsic motivation: A review and synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 774-795. Henderlong, J., & Lepper, M. R. (2000, April). The effects of praise on children s motivation: Person, product, and process feedback. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, New Orleans, Louisiana. Koestner, R., Zuckerman, M., & Olsson, J. (1990). Attributional style, comparison focus of praise, and intrinsic motivation. Journal of Research in Personality, 24, 87-100. Lepper, M. R.,Corpus, J. H., & Iyengar, S. S. (in press). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivational orienations in the classroom: Age differences and academic correlates. To appear in: Journal of Educational Psychology. Midgley, C. M., & Urdan, T. (2001). Academic self-handicapping and achievement goals: A further examination. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 26, 61-75. Mueller, C. M., & Dweck, C. S. (1998). Praise for intelligence can undermine children s motivation and performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 33-52. Presented at the 2005 biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development in Atlanta, GA. We thank Tracy Tomlinson, Penelope Stanton, Annie Block, and the Portland Public Schools Office of Research and Evaluation for their support. Please direct correspondence to Jennifer Henderlong Corpus, Department of Psychology, Reed College, 3203 SE Woodstock Blvd., Portland OR 97202. Email: jennifer.henderlong.corpus@reed.edu.

Social-Comparison Praise Undermines Intrinsic Motivation When Children Later Doubt Their Ability Jennifer Henderlong Corpus, Kelly E. Love, & Christin M. Ogle Department of Psychology Reed College Presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development April 2005, Atlanta, GA Theoretical Framework In an academic context, what is more thrilling than to learn that you have earned the top score in the class, that you are smarter than others, or that you were the first to solve a tough problem? To some, there is no higher praise than normative superiority. Of course, there are other ways to express approval, and even to convey competence. Rather than praising students for excellence relative to others, one might focus on more mastery-oriented achievements, such as their acquisition of skills, developing expertise, or conceptual understanding. Different dimensions of praise such as these have been the subject of a growing body of research revealing that subtle variations in the framing of praise can lead to similar, complementary, or even opposing consequences for intrinsic motivation (see Henderlong & Lepper, 2002, for a review). The motivational effectiveness of social-comparison versus mastery praise, however, has not yet been empirically tested. Although not necessarily in the domain of praise, the distinction between normative excellence and individual task mastery has been the focus of several empirical investigations directed at understanding students goals in the classroom. In general, researchers have contrasted performance goals that involve a focus on demonstrating competence or avoiding demonstrations of incompetence, with mastery goals that involve a focus on enhancing competence and acquiring skills (Ames, 1992; Dweck, 1986; Nicholls, 1984). Mastery goals in the elementary-school classroom have been associated with a host of positive achievement outcomes such as persistence in the face of failure, positive affect, self-efficacy, effective learning strategies, and intrinsic motivation (see Ames, 1992; Dweck, 1999; Dweck & Leggett, 1988; Pintrich, 2000). Performance goals, on the other hand, have typically been associated with more negative outcomes such as learned helplessness, negative affect, superficial learning strategies, challenge avoidance, and self-handicapping (Ames, 1992; Dweck, 1999; Elliott & Dweck, 1988; Midgley & Urdan, 2001; Pintrich, 2000). In recent years, a multiple goals perspective has emerged that casts doubt on a uniformly negative view of performance goals (see Elliot & McGregor, 2001; Harackiewicz, Barron, & Elliot, 1998; Harackiewicz, Barron, Pintrich, Elliot, & Thrash, 2002). Notwithstanding this debate, investigators continue to agree on the largely adaptive value of mastery goals in the elementary-school classroom. Thus, to the extent that goals highlighting individual task mastery are adaptive, and that goals highlighting interpersonal comparisons have the potential to be maladaptive, mastery-focused praise may facilitate intrinsic motivation to a greater degree than social-comparison praise. Indeed, social-comparison praise may have the potential much like performance-avoidance goals to undermine intrinsic motivation. The extant praise literature is not nearly as clear as the goals literature might suggest. Not surprisingly, motivationally enhancing effects of mastery-focused praise have been found in

studies that have used a no-praise control group as the comparison condition (e.g., Anderson et al., 1976; Danner & Lonky, 1981). Social-comparison praise, however, also has been shown to have motivationally enhancing effects relative to no praise (e.g., Blanck et al., 1984; Deci, 1971; Koestner et al., 1990). A key methodological problem in these studies is that the dependent measures were all collected immediately following the praise induction when participants only had reason to feel good about their performance. The present research is the first to provide a direct comparison of the relative benefits of social-comparison versus mastery praise, while also examining motivational outcomes after children were led to feel uncertain about their performance on a subsequent task. Our expectation was that, if children learned to gauge personal success by comparing themselves to others rather than by focusing on the acquisition of skills, they would not be well-equipped to deal with a situation in which their normative standing was ambiguous. Specifically, we hypothesized that, under these circumstances, children given social-comparison praise would show less intrinsic motivation than children given mastery praise or no praise. Study 1 86 4 th - and 5 th -grade children (51 girls) worked on a set of novel puzzles followed by a high score (> 80%) and either (1) no additional feedback, (2) social-comparison praise (e.g., most kids don t do as well as that! ), or (3) mastery praise (e.g., you ve really become an expert at these! ). Next, children worked on a drawing task but did not receive any feedback, in order to create ambiguity about their performance. Children were then asked to complete a 9-item selfreport scale of intrinsic motivation, assessing their curiosity, challenge-seeking, and mastery goals (Lepper, Corpus, & Iyengar, in press). Finally, as a behavioral measure of intrinsic motivation, children selected either a long or short version of a task similar to the initial one for which they had been praised. Results As predicted, children reported the highest levels of intrinsic motivation in the mastery praise condition, followed by the neutral feedback and social-comparison praise conditions (see Table 1). Moreover, children were more likely to select the long versus short task in the mastery praise condition compared to the social-comparison praise or neutral feedback conditions (see Table 2). Study 2 Mastery praise enhanced intrinsic motivation relative to social-comparison praise when subsequent ambiguity about performance was created, but what if children were given unambiguous feedback that their performance continued to be outstanding? 78 4 th - and 5 th grade children (35 girls) participated in a parallel version of Study 1 with two exceptions: children were (1) told that they earned a really good score on the drawing task and (2) given the opportunity to look through a folder of other scored drawings that was designed to reveal their strong performance.

Results As predicted, social-comparison praise no longer had a detrimental effect on intrinsic motivation, relative to mastery praise and neutral feedback, on either self-report or behavioral measures (see Tables 1 and 2). Analyses combining data from Studies 1 and 2 confirmed that children doubted their performance on the drawing task more in Study 1 than in Study 2, t(162) = 5.97, p <.001. Moreover, there were significant interactions of Feedback Condition and Study for both the selfreported and behavioral measures of intrinsic motivation (see Tables 1 and 2). Conclusion Intrinsic motivation was enhanced by mastery praise and dampened by social-comparison praise only when uncertainty about performance was subsequently introduced. Social-comparison praise may teach children that personal competence is measured by outperforming peers, rather than developing skills. This message can be harmful when children are in situations that lead them to doubt their ability. Motivation and resilience may be maximized, therefore, by avoiding social-comparison praise in favor of praise that emphasizes skill acquisition and development. Table 1 Self-Reported Intrinsic Motivation by Feedback Condition for Studies 1 and 2 Study 1 Study 2 Feedback Condition M (SE) M (SE) Social-Comparison Praise 3.48 a (.10) 3.84 c (.09) Mastery Praise 3.89 b (.10) 3.85 c (.09) Neutral Feedback 3.70 ab (.11) 3.84 c (.09) Within-Study Main Effect of Feedback Condition, [Based on 3 (Feedback Condition) x 2 (Sex) ANOVA] F(2, 80) = 4.03, p <.05, η 2 =.09 F(2, 71) =.20, ns Note. Ratings were made on a 5-point scale with higher numbers indicating greater intrinsic motivation. Means with different subscripts indicate significant differences based on a tukey pairwise comparison done within each study. Across the two studies, there was a significant 3 (Feedback Condition) x 2 (Study) interaction: F(2, 151) = 3.03, p =.05, η 2 =.04.

Table 2 Behavioral Intrinsic Motivation by Feedback Condition for Studies 1 and 2 Study 1 Study 2 Feedback Condition % Choosing Long Task % Choosing Long Task Social-Comparison Praise 30 62 Mastery Praise 61 60 Neutral Feedback 29 69 Within-Study Test χ 2 (2, N = 86) = 8.92, p <.05 χ 2 (2, N = 77) =.54, ns Note. Choosing a long versus short task to work on in the future was assumed to indicate greater intrinsic motivation. To compare data across the two studies, a logistic regression was conducted. The Feedback Condition by Study interaction was marginally significant, Wald χ 2 (df = 2, N = 162) = 5.29, p =.07. References Ames, C. (1992). Classrooms: Goals, structures, and student motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 261-271. Anderson, R., Manoogian, S. T., & Reznick, J. S. (1976). The undermining and enhancing of intrinsic motivation in preschool children. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 915-922. Blanck, P. D., Reis, H. T., & Jackson, L. (1984). The effects of verbal reinforcement of intrinsic motivation for sex-linked tasks. Sex Roles, 10, 369-386 Danner, F. W., & Lonky, E. (1981). A cognitive-developmental approach to the effects of rewards on intrinsic motivation. Child Development, 52, 1043-1052. Deci, E. L. (1971). Effects of externally mediated rewards on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 18, 105-115. Dweck, C. S. (1986). Motivational processes affecting learning. American Psychologist, 41, 1040-1048. Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. Philadelphia: Psychology Press. Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 95, 256-273. Elliot, A. J., & McGregor, H. A. (2001). A 2 x 2 achievement goal framework. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 501-519.

Harackiewicz, J. M., & Sansone, C. (1991). Goals and intrinsic motivation: You can get there from here. In M. Maehr & P. Pintrich (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement (vol. 7, pp. 21-49). Greenwich, CT: JAI Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., & Elliot, A. J. (1998). Rethinking achievement goals: When are they adaptive for college students and why? Educational Psychologist, 33, 1-21. Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., Pintrich, P. R., Elliot, A. J., & Thrash, T. M. (2002). Revision of achievement goal theory: Necessary and illuminating. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 638-645. Henderlong, J., & Lepper, M. R. (2002). The effects of praise on children s intrinsic motivation: A review and synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 774-795. Koestner, R., Zuckerman, M., & Olsson, J. (1990). Attributional style, comparison focus of praise, and intrinsic motivation. Journal of Research in Personality, 24, 87-100. Lepper, M. R., Corpus, J. H., & Iyengar, S. S. (2005). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivational orientations in the classroom: Age differences and academic correlates. To appear in the Journal of Educational Psychology. Midgley, C. M., & Urdan, T. (2001). Academic self-handicapping and achievement goals: A further examination. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 26, 61-75. Nicholls, J. G. (1984). Achievement motivation: Conceptions of ability, subjective experience, task choice, and performance. Psychological Review, 91, 328-346. Pintrich, P. R. (2000). Multiple goals, multiple pathways: The role of goal orientation in learning and achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 544-555. Acknowledgements and Correspondence We thank Tracy Tomlinson, Penelope Stanton, Annie Block, and the Portland Public Schools Office of Research and Evaluation for their support. We are also grateful to the children and families who participated in this research. Please direct correspondence to Jennifer Henderlong Corpus, Department of Psychology, Reed College, 3203 SE Woodstock Blvd., Portland OR 97202. Email: jennifer.henderlong.corpus@reed.edu.