Acoustic investigations of the syllabic -es plural in 2-year-olds speech: A preliminary. investigation. KT Mealings

Similar documents
Language Acquisition by Identical vs. Fraternal SLI Twins * Karin Stromswold & Jay I. Rifkin

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

ELA/ELD Standards Correlation Matrix for ELD Materials Grade 1 Reading

The Perception of Nasalized Vowels in American English: An Investigation of On-line Use of Vowel Nasalization in Lexical Access

Rachel E. Baker, Ann R. Bradlow. Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

Unvoiced Landmark Detection for Segment-based Mandarin Continuous Speech Recognition

1 st Quarter (September, October, November) August/September Strand Topic Standard Notes Reading for Literature

1. REFLEXES: Ask questions about coughing, swallowing, of water as fast as possible (note! Not suitable for all

An Acoustic Phonetic Account of the Production of Word-Final /z/s in Central Minnesota English

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Voiced-voiceless distinction in alaryngeal speech - acoustic and articula

The analysis starts with the phonetic vowel and consonant charts based on the dataset:

First Grade Curriculum Highlights: In alignment with the Common Core Standards

Pobrane z czasopisma New Horizons in English Studies Data: 18/11/ :52:20. New Horizons in English Studies 1/2016

Sample Goals and Benchmarks

Houghton Mifflin Reading Correlation to the Common Core Standards for English Language Arts (Grade1)

Taught Throughout the Year Foundational Skills Reading Writing Language RF.1.2 Demonstrate understanding of spoken words,

Florida Reading Endorsement Alignment Matrix Competency 1

Phenomena of gender attraction in Polish *

SOUND STRUCTURE REPRESENTATION, REPAIR AND WELL-FORMEDNESS: GRAMMAR IN SPOKEN LANGUAGE PRODUCTION. Adam B. Buchwald

Word Stress and Intonation: Introduction

Correspondence between the DRDP (2015) and the California Preschool Learning Foundations. Foundations (PLF) in Language and Literacy

Summary / Response. Karl Smith, Accelerations Educational Software. Page 1 of 8

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

Books Effective Literacy Y5-8 Learning Through Talk Y4-8 Switch onto Spelling Spelling Under Scrutiny

Demonstration of problems of lexical stress on the pronunciation Turkish English teachers and teacher trainees by computer

Running head: DELAY AND PROSPECTIVE MEMORY 1

On the nature of voicing assimilation(s)

SEGMENTAL FEATURES IN SPONTANEOUS AND READ-ALOUD FINNISH

The Prosodic (Re)organization of Determiners

English for Life. B e g i n n e r. Lessons 1 4 Checklist Getting Started. Student s Book 3 Date. Workbook. MultiROM. Test 1 4

Different Task Type and the Perception of the English Interdental Fricatives

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

The influence of metrical constraints on direct imitation across French varieties

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Revisiting the role of prosody in early language acquisition. Megha Sundara UCLA Phonetics Lab

Writing a composition

A Cross-language Corpus for Studying the Phonetics and Phonology of Prominence

THE PERCEPTION AND PRODUCTION OF STRESS AND INTONATION BY CHILDREN WITH COCHLEAR IMPLANTS

Coast Academies Writing Framework Step 4. 1 of 7

Lexical phonology. Marc van Oostendorp. December 6, Until now, we have presented phonological theory as if it is a monolithic

Phonological encoding in speech production

Speech Recognition at ICSI: Broadcast News and beyond

LING 329 : MORPHOLOGY

DEVELOPMENT OF LINGUAL MOTOR CONTROL IN CHILDREN AND ADOLESCENTS

Universal contrastive analysis as a learning principle in CAPT

FOREWORD.. 5 THE PROPER RUSSIAN PRONUNCIATION. 8. УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) 4 80.

Stages of Literacy Ros Lugg

Language Acquisition Fall 2010/Winter Lexical Categories. Afra Alishahi, Heiner Drenhaus

Rhythm-typology revisited.

Journal of Phonetics

Speech Recognition using Acoustic Landmarks and Binary Phonetic Feature Classifiers

DIBELS Next BENCHMARK ASSESSMENTS

AUTOMATIC DETECTION OF PROLONGED FRICATIVE PHONEMES WITH THE HIDDEN MARKOV MODELS APPROACH 1. INTRODUCTION

Acoustic correlates of stress and their use in diagnosing syllable fusion in Tongan. James White & Marc Garellek UCLA

Introduction to HPSG. Introduction. Historical Overview. The HPSG architecture. Signature. Linguistic Objects. Descriptions.

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. VCV-sequencies in a preliminary text-to-speech system for female speech

Perceived speech rate: the effects of. articulation rate and speaking style in spontaneous speech. Jacques Koreman. Saarland University

NAME: East Carolina University PSYC Developmental Psychology Dr. Eppler & Dr. Ironsmith

raıs Factors affecting word learning in adults: A comparison of L2 versus L1 acquisition /r/ /aı/ /s/ /r/ /aı/ /s/ = individual sound

Learners Use Word-Level Statistics in Phonetic Category Acquisition

Dyslexia/dyslexic, 3, 9, 24, 97, 187, 189, 206, 217, , , 367, , , 397,

Phonological Encoding in Sentence Production

The Acquisition of English Grammatical Morphemes: A Case of Iranian EFL Learners

AN ANALYSIS OF GRAMMTICAL ERRORS MADE BY THE SECOND YEAR STUDENTS OF SMAN 5 PADANG IN WRITING PAST EXPERIENCES

Learning Methods in Multilingual Speech Recognition

**Note: this is slightly different from the original (mainly in format). I would be happy to send you a hard copy.**

Segregation of Unvoiced Speech from Nonspeech Interference

have to be modeled) or isolated words. Output of the system is a grapheme-tophoneme conversion system which takes as its input the spelling of words,

An Evaluation of the Interactive-Activation Model Using Masked Partial-Word Priming. Jason R. Perry. University of Western Ontario. Stephen J.

ABSTRACT. Some children with speech sound disorders (SSD) have difficulty with literacyrelated

Atypical Prosodic Structure as an Indicator of Reading Level and Text Difficulty

SOFTWARE EVALUATION TOOL

Handout #8. Neutralization

Age Effects on Syntactic Control in. Second Language Learning

GRAMMATICAL MORPHEME ACQUISITION: AN ANALYSIS OF AN EFL LEARNER S LANGUAGE SAMPLES *

Abstract. Janaka Jayalath Director / Information Systems, Tertiary and Vocational Education Commission, Sri Lanka.

AGENDA LEARNING THEORIES LEARNING THEORIES. Advanced Learning Theories 2/22/2016

Approaches to control phenomena handout Obligatory control and morphological case: Icelandic and Basque

Linking object names and object categories: Words (but not tones) facilitate object categorization in 6- and 12-month-olds

Linguistics 220 Phonology: distributions and the concept of the phoneme. John Alderete, Simon Fraser University

The Acquisition of Person and Number Morphology Within the Verbal Domain in Early Greek

CHILDREN S POSSESSIVE STRUCTURES: A CASE STUDY 1. Andrew Radford and Joseph Galasso, University of Essex

Program in Linguistics. Academic Year Assessment Report

Speech Segmentation Using Probabilistic Phonetic Feature Hierarchy and Support Vector Machines

Visual processing speed: effects of auditory input on

DOES RETELLING TECHNIQUE IMPROVE SPEAKING FLUENCY?

Laporan Penelitian Unggulan Prodi

To appear in the Proceedings of the 35th Meetings of the Chicago Linguistics Society. Post-vocalic spirantization: Typology and phonetic motivations

Understanding and Supporting Dyslexia Godstone Village School. January 2017

Lower and Upper Secondary

A Note on Structuring Employability Skills for Accounting Students

On the Formation of Phoneme Categories in DNN Acoustic Models

Developing Grammar in Context

CLASSIFICATION OF PROGRAM Critical Elements Analysis 1. High Priority Items Phonemic Awareness Instruction

Copyright by Niamh Eileen Kelly 2015

ADDIS ABABA UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF GRADUATE STUDIES MODELING IMPROVED AMHARIC SYLLBIFICATION ALGORITHM

Transcription:

Macquarie Matrix: Vol.1, December 2011 Acoustic investigations of the syllabic -es plural in 2-year-olds speech: A preliminary investigation Department of Linguistics, Faculty of Human Sciences, Macquarie University Abstract Children have been found to acquire /-əz/ plurals (e.g. noses) later than /s,z/ plurals (e.g. cats, dogs) (Brown, 1973). This study explores why there is delayed acquisition of the syllabic -es plural and whether word length or segmental factors are involved. We conducted an elicited imitation task involving eight target items, half with a disyllabic root (e.g. letter letters), and half with a monosyllabic root (e.g. bus buses). Children saw pictures of the items on a computer and repeated pre-recorded three-word-utterances with the target both utterance-medial and utterance-final. Acoustic analysis was used to determine the presence or absence of the plural morpheme. Preliminary results from two children show that they have more trouble producing the syllabic -es morpheme, especially when the target word is in utterance-medial position. This suggests articulatory difficulties with the two consecutive fricatives especially when there is less time for the child to perceive/produce the word without phrase-final lengthening. Keywords: speech acoustics, child language acquisition, syllabic plurals. 69

Acoustic investigations of the syllabic -es plural in 2-year-olds speech Introduction Children s early productions of grammatical morphemes have been found to be highly variable (Brown, 1973; Berko, 1958). In particular, children have been found to acquire /-əz/ syllabic plurals which are required after word-final sibilants (e.g. noses) later than /s,z/ plurals required after non-sibilants (e.g. cats, dogs). Several studies have investigated why this might be. The classic Berko (1958) study involved a morpheme elicitation task to see if children (aged five to seven) possessed morphological rules. She found that children were able to form plurals requiring /-s/ or /-z/ but failed to produce the morpheme when the root ended in /s/ or /z/ as the plural required the more complex /-əz/. Berko suggested that children at this age may think that a final sibilant makes the word plural. Goad (1989) furthered this research to include another stage in plural acquisition which comes between the child adding nothing to the root and adding the syllabic -es plural. In this stage, children add the underlying plural morpheme -s to the root word which results in two consecutive fricatives produced without the schwa (e.g. bus-s). Goad refers to this as a gemination strategy. This is used prior to children learning to add the schwa epenthesis in line with the Obligatory Contour Principle (OCP) (McCarthy, 1986), which states that adjacent identical phonemes are not allowed (hence bus-s must then become buses). However, these two studies only investigated assumed deep structure motivations for the acquisition pattern of the syllabic -es plural. Kirk and Demuth (2006) and Song, Sundara and Demuth (2009) propose that some of the variability in children s early productions of grammatical morphemes may be due to phonological context effects such as phonological complexity and ease of articulation. For example, Kirk and Demuth found that the variability could be accounted for by word duration and word position. Regarding word 70

Macquarie Matrix: Vol.1, December 2011 duration, Kirk and Demuth found that codas of monosyllabic words are produced more accurately compared to disyllabic words. Monosyllabic words have longer syllable durations providing the articulators more time to approximate their targets as there is an inverse relationship between the number of syllables in a word and their duration. From this we would also expect difficulties in the production of plurals of disyllabic words. Studies on the past tense morpheme production (for example Berko, 1958; Marchman, Wulfeck, & Weismer, 1999) have shown that adding an extra syllable to the inflected word makes the word more difficult for the child to produce compared to when the past tense morpheme results in a coda cluster (e.g. melted /mel-təd/ vs. baked /bæɪ kt/) This poses the question of whether similar effects occur when the plural adds an extra syllable to the word. Hence the aim of the current study was to examine children s acquisition of the -es plural morpheme in monosyllabic nouns (e.g. bus buses) compared to disyllabic plural nouns (e.g. letter letters). Song et al. (2009) showed that production of third-person singular -s was better in contexts that were phonologically less difficult, such as when the word had a less complex coda, or was in the lengthened utterance-final position. Theodore, Demuth and Shattuck- Hufnagel (2010) also found this utterance position effect in children s production of the plural -s in monosyllabic words, but did not find any coda complexity effects. In regard to utterance position, Theodore et al. found that children produced the plural morpheme -s more often in utterance-final position compared to utterance-medial position. This is probably due to phrase-final lengthening which gives the child more time to perceive/plan/produce and articulate the morpheme. This supports Kirk and Demuth (2006) who found that coda consonants are more likely to be omitted in non-final, unstressed contexts, probably due to the shortened duration compared to words in phrase-final 71

Acoustic investigations of the syllabic -es plural in 2-year-olds speech position. Theodore et al. also suggested that the difficulty in producing utterance-medial morphemes could be because the child still has to plan and articulate the following word, whereas this is not required when the word is utterance-final. The current study will therefore incorporate utterance position lengthening effects by comparing the production of the syllabic -es plural in utterance-medial and utterance-final positions. Note that utteranceinitial position does not need to be considered as well as utterance-medial position as Swanson and Leonard (1994) found no durational differences between the two. Another possible explanation for the delay of the /-əz/ plural after /s/ or /z/ may be that it is articulatory difficult for the child. Smit (1993) found that fricatives are often acquired later than stops, which occur at a higher frequency. This may suggest an articulatory difficulty in the child producing the /-əz/ morpheme after a fricative as they have to produce a fricative twice, separated only by the reduced vowel schwa. Berko found that children were worst at adding the /-əz/ morpheme when the singular also ends in /z/ compared to other phonemes including /s/. Smit found that codas ending in /z/ were only produced accurately 43% of the time at age three compared to codas ending in /s/ (accuracy 60%). Berko however found that children correctly produced the high frequency familiar word glasses, showing capability of producing the /-əz/ morpheme in words that are plural dominant and a part of their lexicon, but may not yet be analysed as a plural. This past research leads to several questions. Is the delayed acquisition of the -es morpheme following the fricative /s/ or /z/ due to children assuming the word is already plural because it ends with this fricative? Is it all disyllabic plural words that children have trouble producing from an increased phonological processing load, or is this more so when the plural adds a syllable? Is the delayed acquisition of the syllabic plural because of difficulties in the articulation and production of two consecutive challenging phonemes? To 72

Macquarie Matrix: Vol.1, December 2011 answer these questions it will be assumed that if there is morpheme absence, then it is likely that children have the assumption that a final fricative makes a word plural. If there is syllable or morpheme reduction, it is likely that it is the longer word duration that is the problem. However, if there are variable realisations of the syllabic plural, then it is more likely that the plural absences may be due to segmental problems. Given the previous findings it is more likely that if there are problems in the production of this morpheme, they will present themselves in utterance-medial position. The current experiment 1 aimed to answer these questions by examining the production of the -es plural in two-year-old children by conducting acoustic analysis of speech from an elicited imitation task. It was hypothesised that the variability would be explained more by segmental factors rather than word duration or incorrect morpheme application. Firstly it was hypothesised that children would have more trouble producing the target word when the plural adds a syllable (i.e. in the CVs/z+əz (buses/noses) contexts) than the already disyllabic words (i.e. the CVCə+z (letters) contexts) in both utterance-medial and utterance-final positions. Secondly it was hypothesised that more plurals would be realised when the target word is in utterance-final position compared to utterance-medial position due to the increased amount of time the child has for both perceiving and producing the word in utterance-final position due to phrase-final lengthening. Method Participants Preliminary results are reported here from two typically developing subjects, each from monolingual Australian-English-speaking homes. One was a 26-month-old male and the 1 Ethics approval number: 5201001398R 73

Acoustic investigations of the syllabic -es plural in 2-year-olds speech other was a 28-month-old female. Both children were healthy on the day of testing and passed the hearing screening otoacoustic emission (OAE) test. Their MacArthur CDI vocabulary test percentile scores were both strong at 100 and 99 respectively. Stimuli Eight target plural nouns were selected for the experiment, four of which had a disyllabic root and were in CVCə+z context (e.g. letter letters), and four that had a monosyllabic root ending in either /s/ or /z/ so that it becomes disyllabic once pluralised (e.g. bus buses). The second group of words were split into two contexts (CVs+əz and CVz+əz) to control for voicing. Each target word appeared in two sentences, one utterance-medially and the other utterance-finally as shown in Table 1. A female native speaker of Australian-English was recorded producing the sixteen sentences using child-directed speech. The recording took place in a sound-attenuated room using a Behringer C-2 microphone and Pro Tools LE software at a sampling rate of 44.1 K. Afterwards, each sentence was segmented using the Praat software. Table 1. Target nouns and their corresponding stimulus sentences Target Transcription Utterance-medial Utterance-final CVCə+z Letters /letəz/ My letters come Send my letters Tigers /tɑ egəz/ His tigers bite Pat his tigers Bakers /bæɪ kəz/ The bakers cook See the bakers Ladders /lædəz/ The ladders break Climb the ladders CVs+əz Horses /hoːsəz/ Her horses kick See her horses Buses /bɐ səz/ The buses come Hear the buses Noses /nəʉ zəz/ Their noses blow Touch their noses 74

Macquarie Matrix: Vol.1, December 2011 CVz+əz Hoses /həʉ zəz/ The hoses bend Pull the hoses The target words were high frequency, familiar, picturable nouns with similar lexical frequencies across the two conditions (the sum of plural frequencies for the CVCə+z words was 126 and the sum of plural frequencies for CVs/z+əz was 117). These frequencies were extracted via ChildFreq from the CHILDES database which calculates the child s frequency of saying the target word per 1 000,000 words between two to three-years-old. The stimulus sentences were all in the present tense. To control for utterance length, each sentence consisted of two monosyllabic words plus the disyllabic target noun. To control for any articulatory influences, the target noun in utterance-medial position was always followed by a word that began with a stop at a different place of articulation to the alveolar plural -s. Following Theodore et al. (2010), this makes the context more challenging and reduces the possibility of resyllabification of the plural with the following word. When the target word was in utterance-final position, the preceding words were either the or a pronoun, thereby controlling for rhythmic effects across conditions. Each target noun was represented by a picture to serve as a visual prompt during the experiment. All pictures were real photos with minimal background distractions. Each picture appeared twice on the PowerPoint slide. Brown (1973) noted that for children two means more than one hence the plural is required. Zapf and Smith (2008) also found that children were much more likely to produce the plural of well known nouns presented as identical objects rather than just similar objects (e.g. having two of the same type of dog rather than having two dogs of different breeds). The PowerPoint presentation then consisted of each of the sixteen stimulus prompts which were then pseudo-randomised. The presentation began with the auditory direction Say what I say. 75

Acoustic investigations of the syllabic -es plural in 2-year-olds speech Procedure The child and their parent were invited into a sound-attenuated test room containing a child-sized table and chairs, with a computer monitor and speakers on top of the table. After becoming familiarised with the experimenters by engaging the child with a picture book or toys, the child was invited to play a game. The room was equipped with two Mac mini computers (one used for the PowerPoint and the other for the sound recording), Sony SRS-55 speakers, and a Behringer C-2 microphone. The microphone was placed on the table near the child in order to best capture his or her speech. The child was directed to face the computer and was asked to repeat what the computer said. The experiment began with a brief warm-up period in order to familiarise the child with the task, and to check the sound levels. Once the child was ready the test items began. For each item, two pictures of the target noun appeared on the monitor along with the auditory prompt, and the child was directed to repeat the prompt. If needed, three attempts were allowed for each utterance in order to obtain one acceptable recording to be analysed. The child was encouraged with praise for each trial. Following the completion of the task (which took about 10 minutes), the child s parents were asked to fill out a brief demographic survey and the MacArthur CDI short form in order to estimate the child s vocabulary size. The child was also screened by otoacoustics emissions for normal hearing. The entire procedure took approximately 30 minutes. The child was given a T-shirt and the parents a gift card for their time. Acoustic Analysis Coding Each utterance was recorded using Pro Tools LE, then excised and coded using Praat. Tokens were excluded if they had poor acoustic quality (e.g. were whispered or had interfering background noise) or if the child failed to produce the correct target word in the sentence. 76

Macquarie Matrix: Vol.1, December 2011 The relevant coding labels and criteria as outlined in the Manual for coding acoustic events: Child Language Lab (2011) were used for coding the final syllable of the target words (letters, tigers, bakers, ladders, horses, buses, noses, hoses). This criterion outlines the acoustic events and their relevant codes for identifying stops, schwas and fricatives. The cues were identified by visual inspection of the waveform, spectrogram and listening to the utterance. Figure 1 shows a representative waveform and spectrogram illustrating the acoustic cues of interest. Figure 1. Representative waveform and spectrogram showing acoustic landmarks for the target word buses After the preceding stop or fricative, the first cue of the -es production was the presence of high-amplitude regularity in the waveform and strong second formant (F2) representing the schwa. The second cue was for the devoiced /z/ which was high frequency, aperiodic noise following periodicity for the vowel. We used the presence or absence of the schwa and devoiced /z/ acoustic cues to determine whether or not the plural was realised. The /-əz/ duration of the target words were also measured. This was so durational utterance position effects could be compared. 77

Percentage of Plurals Realised Acoustic investigations of the syllabic -es plural in 2-year-olds speech Results For the first subject, 12 out of 16 tokens were included in the analysis (four tokens were excluded due to poor acoustic quality or failure to produce the required utterance). For the second subject, all 16 tokens were included. Figure 2 shows the percentage of fully realised plural productions out of those attempted. Note that in order for the syllabic plural production to be considered fully realised, the child had to produce both the /ə/ and the /z/ phonemes. Figure 2. Percentage of realised plural productions in utterance-medial and final position across subjects 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 CVCə+z CVs/z+əz CVCə+z CVs/z+əz Medial Position Final Position Notice that when the target word is in utterance-final position for both conditions the results are at ceiling. The fact that the children were able to accurately produce these plurals provides evidence against Goad s suggestion that incorrect productions are due to the child applying the wrong morpheme. Also, notice the difficulties in producing the CVs/z+əz words (requiring the syllabic plural) utterance-medially compared to the CVCə+z context. The fact that the former are produced less often than the CVCə+z words rules out the possibility of the disyllabic word durational effects explaining the difficulties. 78

Percentage of Plurals Realised Macquarie Matrix: Vol.1, December 2011 Figure 3 gives a closer look at the syllabic plural realisations for each the CVs+əz and CVz+əz contexts for each child. Notice that the errors are only evident when the target words are in are utterance-medial position, suggesting segmental or articulatory effects may be the main contributor to these problems. Figure 3.Percentage of realised plural productions for each context in utterance-medial and final position 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 CVCə+z CVs+əz CVz+əz CVCə+z CVs+əz CVz+əz Subject1 Subject 2 Utterance-Medial Utterance-Final Figure 4 shows the difference in the /-əz/ phoneme durations in utterance-medial and utterance-final positions. 79

Average Duration of Syllabic Plural (s) Acoustic investigations of the syllabic -es plural in 2-year-olds speech Figure 4. Comparison of the average duration of the final syllable of the target word in utterancemedial and final positions for each subject 0.5 0.45 0.4 0.35 0.3 0.25 0.2 0.15 0.1 0.05 0 Subject 1 Subject 2 Medial Position Final Position Figure 4 shows that the phonemes are much shorter in utterance-medial position. This less amount of time to articulate the syllabic plural is likely to account for the difficulties in its production. Table 2 shows a phonetic realisation of the various attempts of these plurals. Table 2. Phonetic realisation of CVs/z+əz target words in medial position Target Word Subject 1 Production Subject 2 Production /bɐ səz/ /bɐ s-s/ /bɐ sə/ /hoːsəz/ /hoːsi:/ /hoːsəz/ /həʉ zəz/ /həʉ zə/ /həʉ zə/ /nəʉ zəz/ - /nəʉ zəz/ Table 2 shows the /-əz/ plural only being realised on two of the seven occasions, but on four occasions the schwa was produced without the final fricative. In another case, two final fricatives were produced leaving out the schwa (bus-s) which could be construed as an example of Goad s (1989) gemination strategy. The child however only did this utterancemedially and on one occassion, so segmental factors are still more likely to explain the 80

Macquarie Matrix: Vol.1, December 2011 delayed acquisition of the syllabic plural morpheme rather than incorrect morpheme application. Discussion As previously reviewed, children have been found to acquire /-əz/ syllabic plurals later than /s,z/ plurals. The aim of this study was to explore why this is the case and whether word length or segmental factors such as phonological complexity, articulatory difficulty, or utterance position, contribute most. As in previous studies (e.g. Berko, 1959), children had more trouble producing the plural of the target word when it necessitates the addition of a syllable. This difficulty was particularly evident for both subjects when the syllabic plural was produced in utterance-medial position. A comparison of the durations of the final syllable in utterance-medial and utterance-final positions showed that the syllable is much longer when the target word is in utterance-final position. One reason why more plurals are realised in utterance-final position could therefore be because of the increased amount of time the child has for both perceiving and producing the word due to phrase-final lengthening. This is consistent with the findings of Theodore et al. (2010). From this it would seem that it is not necessarily all disyllabic plural words that children have trouble producing from an increased processing load, but the difficulty in production arises more so when the plural adds a syllable. This is likely to be explained by either articulatory difficulties, or that the child is applying the wrong grammatical morpheme, assuming that the root word is already plural because it ends with the sibilant fricative. In our study, both children were at ceiling in producing the syllabic plural when it appeared in utterance-final position where they had longer time to perceive and articulate it. This shows that they have actually acquired this rule, but it is still vulnerable in particular 81

Acoustic investigations of the syllabic -es plural in 2-year-olds speech segmental contexts. In utterance-medial position, extra planning is needed to produce the following word after the target word. This is likely to account for the partial application of the syllabic plural rule by adding only the schwa or fricative rather than applying the wrong grammatical morpheme. Following on from this study, future research areas could include longitudinal studies into the development of producing the syllabic -es plural, as well as comparisons between child and adult productions of this plural. The effects of of word frequency in child language acquisition could also be explored as this study focussed on the production of the syllabic -es plural in high frequency words, but it would be interesting to investigate if there are any differences in its production when applied to lower frequency words. Conclusion In this preliminary investigation, the production of the syllabic -es plural morpheme in twoyear-old children was found to be more problematic than the production of the plural in disyllabic words, particularly when the target word was in utterance-medial position. As this study is only in its early stages, strong conclusions cannot yet be drawn. These preliminary results suggest, however, that that the delayed acquisition of this morpheme is due more to segmental factors than word duration or incorrect morpheme application. This is shown by the difficulty in articulating the fricatives in a short space of time when there is additional planning required for the following word. Hopefully, with further investigation of more subjects, clearer results will be evident. These can then provide a baseline measure to compare with the suspected difficulties and delays in grammatical morpheme acquisition for children with SLI and the hearing impaired. 82

Macquarie Matrix: Vol.1, December 2011 Acknowledgements I would like to thank my supervisor Katherine Demuth as well as Felicity Cox, Ivan Yuen, Nan Xu, Kelly Miles, and all of those that are a part of the Child Language Lab at Macquarie for their guidance and feedback regarding this study. References Berko, J. (1958). The Child's learning of English morphology. Word, 14, 150-177. Boersma, P. & Weenink, D. (2011). Praat: doing phonetics by computer (Version 5.2.23) [Computer program]. Retrieved May 4, 2011, from http://www.praat.org Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Goad, H. (1989). The function of the obligatory contour principle in English: Evidence from child language. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 28, 115-122. Kirk, C., & Demuth, K. (2006). Accounting for variability in 2-year-olds production of coda consonants. Language Learning and Development, 2(2), 97-118. Manual for coding acoustic events: Child Language Lab. (2011), Retrieved February 4, 2011, from http://www.ling.mq.edu.au/cll/index.html McCarthy, J. J. (1986). OCP effects: Gemination and antigemination. Linguistic Inquiry 17, 207-263. Marchman, V. A., Wulfeck, B., & Weismer, S. E. (1999). Morphological productivity in children with normal language and SLI: A study of the English past tense. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 206-219. Smit, A. B. (1993). Phonologic error distributions in the Iowa-Nebraska articulation 83

Acoustic investigations of the syllabic -es plural in 2-year-olds speech norms project: consonant singletons. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 533-547. Song, J., Sundara, M., & Demuth, K. (2009). Phonological constraints on children s production of English third person singular s. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52, 623-642. Swanson, L. A., & Leonard, L. B. (1994). Duration of function-word vowels in mothers speech to young children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 1394-1405. Theodore, R., Demuth, K., & Shattuck-Hufnagel, S. (2010). An acoustic examination of the effects of word frequency and utterance position on 2-year-olds production of 3rd person singular s. Proceedings from Speech Science and Technology (SST) 2010, pp. 158-161, Melbourne. Zapf, J. A., & Smith, L. B. (2008). Meaning matters in children s plural productions. Cognition, 108, 466-476. 84