Transfer of Juggling Skills Acquired in a Virtual Environment Hauge, Alex Patrick; Kragegaard, Christian Skriver; Kjæhr, Emil Bering; Kraus, Martin

Similar documents
A 3D SIMULATION GAME TO PRESENT CURTAIN WALL SYSTEMS IN ARCHITECTURAL EDUCATION

AGENDA LEARNING THEORIES LEARNING THEORIES. Advanced Learning Theories 2/22/2016

Lectora a Complete elearning Solution

LEGO MINDSTORMS Education EV3 Coding Activities

Diploma of Building and Construction (Building)

Specification of the Verity Learning Companion and Self-Assessment Tool

SOFTWARE EVALUATION TOOL

RETURNING TEACHER REQUIRED TRAINING MODULE YE TRANSCRIPT

DISTANCE LEARNING OF ENGINEERING BASED SUBJECTS: A CASE STUDY. Felicia L.C. Ong (author and presenter) University of Bradford, United Kingdom

THE VIRTUAL WELDING REVOLUTION HAS ARRIVED... AND IT S ON THE MOVE!

K 1 2 K 1 2. Iron Mountain Public Schools Standards (modified METS) Checklist by Grade Level Page 1 of 11

Application of Virtual Instruments (VIs) for an enhanced learning environment

Executive Summary: Tutor-facilitated Digital Literacy Acquisition

Introduction to Moodle

Speak Up 2012 Grades 9 12

Simulation in Maritime Education and Training

Practice Examination IREB

UDL AND LANGUAGE ARTS LESSON OVERVIEW

OilSim. Talent Management and Retention in the Oil and Gas Industry. Global network of training centers and technical facilities

THE WEB 2.0 AS A PLATFORM FOR THE ACQUISITION OF SKILLS, IMPROVE ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE AND DESIGNER CAREER PROMOTION IN THE UNIVERSITY

ADVANCED MACHINE LEARNING WITH PYTHON BY JOHN HEARTY DOWNLOAD EBOOK : ADVANCED MACHINE LEARNING WITH PYTHON BY JOHN HEARTY PDF

UNDERSTANDING DECISION-MAKING IN RUGBY By. Dave Hadfield Sport Psychologist & Coaching Consultant Wellington and Hurricanes Rugby.

Appendix L: Online Testing Highlights and Script

Learning Microsoft Office Excel

We re Listening Results Dashboard How To Guide

From Virtual University to Mobile Learning on the Digital Campus: Experiences from Implementing a Notebook-University

DIGITAL GAMING & INTERACTIVE MEDIA BACHELOR S DEGREE. Junior Year. Summer (Bridge Quarter) Fall Winter Spring GAME Credits.

BUILD-IT: Intuitive plant layout mediated by natural interaction

Carolina Course Evaluation Item Bank Last Revised Fall 2009

Using Virtual Manipulatives to Support Teaching and Learning Mathematics

COMM370, Social Media Advertising Fall 2017

Stages of Literacy Ros Lugg

M-Learning. Hauptseminar E-Learning Sommersemester Michael Kellerer LFE Medieninformatik

Is M-learning versus E-learning or are they supporting each other?

Circuit Simulators: A Revolutionary E-Learning Platform

Lesson plan for Maze Game 1: Using vector representations to move through a maze Time for activity: homework for 20 minutes

On May 3, 2013 at 9:30 a.m., Miss Dixon and I co-taught a ballet lesson to twenty

Session Six: Software Evaluation Rubric Collaborators: Susan Ferdon and Steve Poast

Getting the Story Right: Making Computer-Generated Stories More Entertaining

A Comparison of the Effects of Two Practice Session Distribution Types on Acquisition and Retention of Discrete and Continuous Skills

Principal Survey FAQs

THE HEAD START CHILD OUTCOMES FRAMEWORK

What s in a Step? Toward General, Abstract Representations of Tutoring System Log Data

Prepared by: Tim Boileau

EdX Learner s Guide. Release

White Paper. The Art of Learning

Different Requirements Gathering Techniques and Issues. Javaria Mushtaq

Top Ten Persuasive Strategies Used on the Web - Cathy SooHoo, 5/17/01

Chamilo 2.0: A Second Generation Open Source E-learning and Collaboration Platform

University of Groningen. Systemen, planning, netwerken Bosman, Aart

LEt s GO! Workshop Creativity with Mockups of Locations

Requirements-Gathering Collaborative Networks in Distributed Software Projects

1 Use complex features of a word processing application to a given brief. 2 Create a complex document. 3 Collaborate on a complex document.

Understanding and Supporting Dyslexia Godstone Village School. January 2017

USING INTERACTIVE VIDEO TO IMPROVE STUDENTS MOTIVATION IN LEARNING ENGLISH

Innovative Teaching in Science, Technology, Engineering, and Math

Robot manipulations and development of spatial imagery

Economics 201 Principles of Microeconomics Fall 2010 MWF 10:00 10:50am 160 Bryan Building

SIE: Speech Enabled Interface for E-Learning

University of Groningen. Peer influence in clinical workplace learning Raat, Adriana

Steps Before Step Scanning By Linda J. Burkhart Scripting by Fio Quinn Powered by Mind Express by Jabbla

The feasibility, delivery and cost effectiveness of drink driving interventions: A qualitative analysis of professional stakeholders

Save Children. Can Math Recovery. before They Fail?

Fountas-Pinnell Level P Informational Text

Multimedia Courseware of Road Safety Education for Secondary School Students

YMCA SCHOOL AGE CHILD CARE PROGRAM PLAN

Distributed Weather Net: Wireless Sensor Network Supported Inquiry-Based Learning

BLENDED LEARNING IN ACADEMIA: SUGGESTIONS FOR KEY STAKEHOLDERS. Jeff Rooks, University of West Georgia. Thomas W. Gainey, University of West Georgia

Using SAM Central With iread

A Coding System for Dynamic Topic Analysis: A Computer-Mediated Discourse Analysis Technique

An ICT environment to assess and support students mathematical problem-solving performance in non-routine puzzle-like word problems

Welcome to The National Training Institute for Child Care Health Consultants

ADHD Classroom Accommodations for Specific Behaviour

An Evaluation of E-Resources in Academic Libraries in Tamil Nadu

Miami-Dade County Public Schools

An Introduction to Simio for Beginners

Published in: The Proceedings of the 12th International Congress on Mathematical Education

SURVIVING ON MARS WITH GEOGEBRA

Strategy Study on Primary School English Game Teaching

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 209 ( 2015 )

CSC200: Lecture 4. Allan Borodin

Milady Standard Cosmetology

A virtual surveying fieldcourse for traversing

Usability Design Strategies for Children: Developing Children Learning and Knowledge in Decreasing Children Dental Anxiety

PART 1. A. Safer Keyboarding Introduction. B. Fifteen Principles of Safer Keyboarding Instruction

CIS Introduction to Digital Forensics 12:30pm--1:50pm, Tuesday/Thursday, SERC 206, Fall 2015

How to make successful presentations in English Part 2

Quantitative Evaluation of an Intuitive Teaching Method for Industrial Robot Using a Force / Moment Direction Sensor

Institutional repository policies: best practices for encouraging self-archiving

Recursive Loops of Game-Based Learning: a Conceptual model.

MULTIDISCIPLINARY TEAM COMMUNICATION THROUGH VISUAL REPRESENTATIONS

P-4: Differentiate your plans to fit your students

Connect Communicate Collaborate. Transform your organisation with Promethean s interactive collaboration solutions

Entrepreneurial Discovery and the Demmert/Klein Experiment: Additional Evidence from Germany

MEE 6501, Advanced Air Quality Control Course Syllabus. Course Description. Course Textbook. Course Learning Outcomes. Credits.

What is PDE? Research Report. Paul Nichols

Teaching Literacy Through Videos

Course Law Enforcement II. Unit I Careers in Law Enforcement

Client Psychology and Motivation for Personal Trainers

A Framework for Articulating New Library Roles

Transcription:

Aalborg Universitet Transfer of Juggling Skills Acquired in a Virtual Environment Hauge, Alex Patrick; Kragegaard, Christian Skriver; Kjæhr, Emil Bering; Kraus, Martin Published in: Proceedings of the International Conference on Computer Graphics Theory and Applications and International Conference on Information Visualization Theory and Applications Publication date: 2013 Document Version Early version, also known as pre-print Link to publication from Aalborg University Citation for published version (APA): Hauge, A. P., Kragegaard, C. S., Kjæhr, E. B., & Kraus, M. (2013). Transfer of Juggling Skills Acquired in a Virtual Environment. In S. Coquillart, C. Andujar, R. S. Laramee, A. Kerren, & J. Braz (Eds.), Proceedings of the International Conference on Computer Graphics Theory and Applications and International Conference on Information Visualization Theory and Applications (pp. 385-388). Portugal: Institute for Systems and Technologies of Information, Control and Communication. General rights Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the public portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights.? Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the public portal for the purpose of private study or research.? You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain? You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the public portal? Take down policy If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us at vbn@aub.aau.dk providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. Downloaded from vbn.aau.dk on: april 27, 2018

Transfer of Juggling Skills Acquired in a Virtual Environment A. P. Hauge 1, C. S. Kragegaard 1, E. B. Kjæhr 1, M. Kraus 2 1 School of Information and Communication Technology, Aalborg University, Denmark 2 Department of Architecture, Design and Media Technology, Aalborg University, Denmark Keywords: Abstract: natural interaction; virtual training; transfer of skill; juggling; motor skill; simulation. This paper explores whether motoric skills acquired within a virtual training environment can be successfully transferred to the real world by comparing a virtual environment with a traditional learning environment. Specifically, a system for learning juggling with virtual balls was designed with a focus on approximating natural interaction. We propose a method of evaluating the acquisition and transfer of motoric skills through a virtual environment, which is compared to a traditional learning environment. Each environment was evaluated using various criteria ranging from improvement in skills to observations of performance. The findings suggest that a transfer of motoric skills and knowledge takes place for users of the virtual system with only little difference between the environments. They also suggest that a virtual environment can create a less frustrating learning experience. 1 INTRODUCTION Juggling is a sport that heavily involves motor skills in order to facilitate throws from one hand to the other without dropping a ball. It requires the user to learn the basic techniques and to understand the rhythm. Furthermore, the user has to anticipate coming events to organize current actions (Beek and Lewbel, 1995). The goal of the virtual learning environment proposed in this work is to facilitate the acquisition of transferable juggling skills in as effective a way as possible. To this end, the physical motion of juggling balls is simulated and appropriately modified in order to allow for an improved learning experience. The comparison of a virtual environment (VE) and a traditional environment for learning motor skills such as juggling can reveal whether training in a VE could substitute training with physical balls or whether the loss of depth, spatial and tactile feedback when using the VE also means a loss in the transfer of motor skills. Apart from the transfer of knowledge, we also investigate how a well-designed learning VE compares to a more traditional learning environment; in particular, we compare the efficacy of the two environments. The rest of the paper is structured as follows: After a review of previous work, the design of the VE is discussed. This is followed by a description of the experiment and a presentation of the results. Finally, the VE is discussed based on the obtained results. 2 PREVIOUS WORK Prior research in the field of acquisition of knowledge in VEs has focused on virtual reality (VR) applications. This includes transfer of spatial knowledge (Peruch et al., 2000)(Witmer et al., 1996) and basic task-related knowledge (Kenyon and Afenya, 1995). This research suggests that virtual reality technology can facilitate acquisition of transferable knowledge. (Lagarde et al., 2012) focused on teaching timing through their VE. They found no significant difference between learning juggling in a virtual and a real environment. They also found that starting with balls at slow speed and increasing the speed allowed subjects to learn faster than with balls at normal speed. However, VR equipment is not widely available and as such is primarily used in professional training simulators. The rise of computer-vision-based input devices such as the Microsoft Kinect for consumer use can bring the technology to a larger demographic that could potentially benefit from using a VE. Using computer vision techniques, Charalambous facilitated the learning of juggling with virtual balls (Charalambous, 2005) and Marshall et al. studied the enhancement of the presentation of juggling performances with physical balls as well as advanced training by using juggling with physical balls as part of games (Marshall et al., 2007). Our work is similar to the work by Charalambous in that we also facilitate the learning of juggling in a VE; however, we observe the complete learning process. Moreover, we integrate game elements in the VE to improve the learning

experience similar to the work by Marshall et al. 3 DESIGNING THE VIRTUAL ENVIRONMENT The VE was implemented using the Unity3D game engine (Unity Technologies, 2011) and the Microsoft Kinect controller and SDK (Microsoft Corporation, 2011) to track each hand of the player. In the VE the player s hands, are represented by 3D models such that the left hand appears on the lefthand side of the screen and the right hand on the righthand side; see Figure 1. Figure 1: A screenshot of a player juggling with 3 balls using the virtual training environment A basic button-trigger-based system was created to give users more control over releasing (i.e. throwing) the balls. Alternative solutions were investigated such as releasing the balls based on sudden deceleration of the hands; however, players reported problems with unintentional releases of balls. The motion of balls in the VE is restricted to a plane parallel to the view plane. Issues with using more degrees of freedom for the virtual balls have previously been identified by Charalambous (Charalambous, 2005). An important part in the design of the VE was the dynamic difficulty adjustments during play. The training starts with reduced gravity and therefore slower motion of the balls. Furthermore, released balls are initially assigned the appropriate velocity so they reach the other hand. However, the player s control over the balls velocity is gradually increased as his or her score rises. The difficulty level is computed by constantly checking the player s score. If the player scores higher than his or her previous score, the difficulty will increase and vice versa. The difficulty is filtered; thus, dropping a single ball will not reset the difficulty but only reduce it slightly. If the player continues to drop several balls, the difficulty will decrease to help the player get back in the game. This dependency on the player s score results in a basic dynamic difficulty adjustment intended to challenge players without frustrating them, as suggested by flow theory (Csikszentmihalyi, 1997). A level-based system in which new concepts are gradually introduced (in this case, more balls), provides a less steep learning curve compared to that of a traditional learning environment. To keep the attention of the players, flow theory (Csikszentmihalyi, 1997) was used in the design of the levels. Each level has a requirement containing a certain amount of juggles before the next level is reached. Throughout these levels, auditory cues are triggered when a ball is dropped, thrown or a new ball is on the way. The music also gradually changes based on the current score and increases from a lower pitch up to the normal speed of the music. Using a virtual system might also assist in increasing the automation of catching the balls, as the VE enables easy practicing. By increasing the amount of practice, the automation should also become faster (Logan, 1988). Failures were designed to be entertaining as suggested by Ravaja et al. (Ravaja et al., 2005). Thus, players are rewarded by visual and audio effects when they drop balls in order to encourage them to continue playing. 4 EXPERIMENT The test subjects were mostly male and between 17 and 29 years. All were unable to juggle and had never received prior training in juggling. To compare the VE with the traditional learning environment, test subjects were split into two groups. Group 1 consisted of 14 subjects who used the VE. Group 2 consisted of 15 subjects who used the traditional learning environment, i.e. physical balls. The test setup shown in Figure 2 consisted of recording equipment, the VE and a test facilitator to observe and take notes. Furthermore, an instructional juggling training CD-ROM (Duncan Toys Company, 2011) was available for use. The traditional learning environment was identical but without the screen and the Kinect controller. For the first 5 minutes, the subjects of each group were asked to throw as many or as few physical balls from hand to hand as they were comfortable with. The goal of this was to evaluate the basic level of motor control and spatial awareness for each subject. After this, a period of 35 minutes was spent training juggling 30 minutes for the users of the VE. During this time, subjects were allowed to watch training videos from a DVD. The DVD contained short videos with descriptions for juggling one up to four balls. The subjects using the VE were then given 5 minutes to

5 6 2 DVD 1 3 4 Figure 2: The test setup: laptop with instructional DVD (1), test subject (2), Kinect controller (3), screen (4), facilitator (5), spare balls (6), and recording equipment (7). In the traditional environment (3) and (4) were removed. adjust to throwing real balls. Finally, each group was asked to juggle as many or as few physical balls they could for 10 minutes in the same way they would do if they were supposed to learn to juggle. After the performance test, users were asked to fill out a questionnaire. Questions ranged from performance self-assessment to rating their level of frustration during the test. Figure 3: Model of how the performance is calculated. performance differences of all test subjects can be seen in Figure 4. 5 RESULTS The performance score was created as an indication of how well the player performed during the test. The performance score was computed by first counting the number of throws from hand to hand per minute, which was divided by the number of drops per minute. This number x was then transformed by the function (1 1/(x + 1))/3 resulting in a number between 0 (no successful throws) to 1/3 (infinitely many successful throws). If the player was juggling with 2 balls instead of 1 ball, 1/3 was added to the performance score in order to reflect the higher performance. If 3 balls were used, 2/3 was added. The computation is illustrated in Figure 3. If a test subject juggled with different numbers of balls, only the score for the highest number of balls was considered in order to indicate that the subject is at the next level. This also ensures that a player, who only threw one ball, cannot have a better performance than a player who has trained with two balls. Using a paired t-test it was possible to observe significantly better performances (probability level 0.05) of both groups after the training. Comparing the improvements between the groups with an independent t-test did not show a significant difference: the traditional environment had only a 1.52% higher score compared to the mean in the virtual environment. The Figure 4: Individual difference in user performance for each learning environment (11 test subjects had to be culled from the data due to using an incorrect juggling technique.) When the virtual group answered the questionnaire, 57% of the subjects didn t feel they would be able to learn juggling by using the virtual environment. This shows that subjects were unaware of the improvement they made. As the subjects were using the virtual system they reported less frustration as opposed to the traditional learning environment, this was also reflected in the questionnaire answers as shown in Figure 5. As depicted in Figure 6, subjects using the traditional learning environment felt it was difficult as opposed to the subjects using the VE. 71% reported that the VE was not too hard. 6 DISCUSSION The lack of a significant difference between the acquired skills of the two groups suggests that the virtual learning environment is comparable with the traditional learning environment as seen in Figure 4.

Since these results and observations have all been gathered using juggling training, they can not be generalized to skills beyond juggling. In order to draw broader conclusions on the use and benefits of virtual training environments for transfer of motor skills, further research should be carried out across different fields relying on motor skill development. Figure 5: Number of subjects reporting a level of frustration from 1 (very frustrated) to 5 (very satisfied).1 test subject didn t fill out the questionaire. Figure 6: Number of subjects reporting a level of experienced difficulty ranging from 1 (too difficult) to 5 (too easy). 1 test subject didn t fill out the questionaire. The results suggest that the acquisition of transferable motor skills is possible through training in a VE. User feedback indicates that the dynamic difficulty adjustments of gravity and velocity of thrown balls created an experience where players were less frustrated and provided a better training experience as shown in Figures 5 and 6. This could be explored further by letting the training span several days to reduce mental and physical fatigue endured by subjects. Evaluating training over longer periods of time would have been beneficial as the short time makes the data more prone to be due to chance. The reason that it was not possible to find any difference between the two samples might also have been due to the small sample size. 7 CONCLUSION The experiment showed that the use of game design can improve a training environment for juggling by helping learners to maintain focus and by keeping the learning experience engaging and interesting in spite of a repetitive training process. This confirms earlier research by Marshall et al. (Marshall et al., 2007). REFERENCES Beek, P. J. and Lewbel, A. (1995). The Science of Juggling. Scientific American, pages 92 97. Charalambous, T. (2005). Interacting with a virtual environment. http://www.themistoklis.org/mengthesis.pdf [Last accessed: 12/20/2011]. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1997). Finding Flow. Basic Books, A Member of the Perseus Books Group. Duncan Toys Company (2011). Instructional cd rom. http://www.yoyo.com/index.php/site/products/3840jgin [Last accessed: 12/20/2011]. Kenyon, R. V. and Afenya, M. B. (1995). Training in virtual and real environments. Annals of Biomedical Engineering, 23:445 455. Lagarde, J., Gopher, D., Alberto Avizzano, C., Erev, V., Lippi, V., and Zelic, G. (2012). Training to juggle with a light weight juggler (lwj). In Bergamasco, M., Bardy, B., and Gopher, D., editors, Skill Training in Multimodal Virtual Environments, pages 187 198. CRC Press. Logan, G. (1988). Toward an Instance Theory of Automatization. Psychological Review, 492-527, 1988, 95:492 527. Marshall, J., Benford, S., and Pridmore, T. (2007). Eyeballs: juggling with the virtual. In Proceedings of the 6th ACM SIGCHI conference on Creativity & cognition, C&C 07, pages 265 266, New York, NY, USA. ACM. Microsoft Corporation (2011). Microsoft kinect sdk for developers. http://kinectforwindows.org/ [Last accessed: 12/20/2011]. Peruch, P., Belingard, L., and Thinus-Blanc, C. (2000). Transfer of spatial knowledge from virtual to real environments. In Freksa, C., Habel, C., Brauer, W., and Wender, K., editors, Spatial Cognition II, volume 1849 of Lecture Notes in Computer Science, pages 253 264. Springer Berlin / Heidelberg. Ravaja, N., Saari, T., Laarni, J., Kallinen, K., and Salminen, M. (2005). The psychophysiology of video gaming : Phasic emotional responses to game events. Proceedings of the DiGRA conference Changing views worlds in play, it(4):1 13. Unity Technologies (2011). Unity: Game development tool. http://unity3d.com/ [Last accessed: 12/20/2011]. Witmer, B. G., Bailey, J. H., Knerr, B. W., and Parsons, K. C. (1996). Virtual spaces and real world places: transfer of route knowledge. Int. J. Hum.-Comput. Stud., 45:413 428.