REVIEWS. Reviewed by ANDREW NEVINS, Harvard University

Similar documents
Radical CV Phonology: the locational gesture *

Parallel Evaluation in Stratal OT * Adam Baker University of Arizona

Consonants: articulation and transcription

Pobrane z czasopisma New Horizons in English Studies Data: 18/11/ :52:20. New Horizons in English Studies 1/2016

Consonant-Vowel Unity in Element Theory*

To appear in the Proceedings of the 35th Meetings of the Chicago Linguistics Society. Post-vocalic spirantization: Typology and phonetic motivations

Speech Recognition using Acoustic Landmarks and Binary Phonetic Feature Classifiers

Universal contrastive analysis as a learning principle in CAPT

Partial Class Behavior and Nasal Place Assimilation*

SOUND STRUCTURE REPRESENTATION, REPAIR AND WELL-FORMEDNESS: GRAMMAR IN SPOKEN LANGUAGE PRODUCTION. Adam B. Buchwald

Markedness and Complex Stops: Evidence from Simplification Processes 1. Nick Danis Rutgers University

Manner assimilation in Uyghur

An argument from speech pathology

Precedence Constraints and Opacity

Lexical phonology. Marc van Oostendorp. December 6, Until now, we have presented phonological theory as if it is a monolithic

1. REFLEXES: Ask questions about coughing, swallowing, of water as fast as possible (note! Not suitable for all

Phonetics. The Sound of Language

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

Underlying Representations

Christine Mooshammer, IPDS Kiel, Philip Hoole, IPSK München, Anja Geumann, Dublin

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

The analysis starts with the phonetic vowel and consonant charts based on the dataset:

A Fact in Historical Phonology from the Viewpoint of Generative Phonology: The Underlying Schwa in Old English

5. Margi (Chadic, Nigeria): H, L, R (Williams 1973, Hoffmann 1963)

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Sounds of Infant-Directed Vocabulary: Learned from Infants Speech or Part of Linguistic Knowledge?

**Note: this is slightly different from the original (mainly in format). I would be happy to send you a hard copy.**

Similarity Avoidance in the Proto-Indo-European Root

Phonological Processing for Urdu Text to Speech System

Language Acquisition by Identical vs. Fraternal SLI Twins * Karin Stromswold & Jay I. Rifkin

The Journey to Vowelerria VOWEL ERRORS: THE LOST WORLD OF SPEECH INTERVENTION. Preparation: Education. Preparation: Education. Preparation: Education

DOWNSTEP IN SUPYIRE* Robert Carlson Societe Internationale de Linguistique, Mali

The Indian English of Tibeto-Burman language speakers*

Learning Methods in Multilingual Speech Recognition

Unvoiced Landmark Detection for Segment-based Mandarin Continuous Speech Recognition

SEGMENTAL FEATURES IN SPONTANEOUS AND READ-ALOUD FINNISH

The presence of interpretable but ungrammatical sentences corresponds to mismatches between interpretive and productive parsing.

RP ENGLISH AND CASTILIAN SPANISH DIPHTHONGS REVISITED FROM THE BEATS-AND-BINDING PERSPECTIVE

Portuguese Vowel Harmony: A Comparative Analysis and the Superiority of Autosegmental Representations

LING 329 : MORPHOLOGY

Canadian raising with language-specific weighted constraints Joe Pater, University of Massachusetts Amherst

On the Formation of Phoneme Categories in DNN Acoustic Models

Speech Segmentation Using Probabilistic Phonetic Feature Hierarchy and Support Vector Machines

The Perception of Nasalized Vowels in American English: An Investigation of On-line Use of Vowel Nasalization in Lexical Access

Derivations (MP) and Evaluations (OT) *

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Lang Speech. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 January 1.

DEVELOPMENT OF LINGUAL MOTOR CONTROL IN CHILDREN AND ADOLESCENTS

The Strong Minimalist Thesis and Bounded Optimality

Different Task Type and the Perception of the English Interdental Fricatives

ABSTRACT. Some children with speech sound disorders (SSD) have difficulty with literacyrelated

Phonology Revisited: Sor3ng Out the PH Factors in Reading and Spelling Development. Indiana, November, 2015

Multiple case assignment and the English pseudo-passive *

Phonological encoding in speech production

Affricates. Affricates, nasals, laterals and continuants. Affricates. Affricates. Study questions

Linguistics. Undergraduate. Departmental Honors. Graduate. Faculty. Linguistics 1

2,1 .,,, , %, ,,,,,,. . %., Butterworth,)?.(1989; Levelt, 1989; Levelt et al., 1991; Levelt, Roelofs & Meyer, 1999

Module 12. Machine Learning. Version 2 CSE IIT, Kharagpur

An Introduction to the Minimalist Program

CEFR Overall Illustrative English Proficiency Scales

Abstractions and the Brain

On Developing Acoustic Models Using HTK. M.A. Spaans BSc.

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Sound symbolism in deictic words

Problems of the Arabic OCR: New Attitudes

TEKS Comments Louisiana GLE

Perceived speech rate: the effects of. articulation rate and speaking style in spontaneous speech. Jacques Koreman. Saarland University

age, Speech and Hearii

Bare Root Nodes in Basaa

SARDNET: A Self-Organizing Feature Map for Sequences

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. VCV-sequencies in a preliminary text-to-speech system for female speech

Effectiveness of Electronic Dictionary in College Students English Learning

Contrastiveness and diachronic variation in Chinese nasal codas. Tsz-Him Tsui The Ohio State University

On the nature of voicing assimilation(s)

Correspondence between the DRDP (2015) and the California Preschool Learning Foundations. Foundations (PLF) in Language and Literacy

Quantitative Reasoning in Linguistics

Process-specific constraints in Optimality Theory

CLASSIFICATION OF PROGRAM Critical Elements Analysis 1. High Priority Items Phonemic Awareness Instruction

MASTER S THESIS GUIDE MASTER S PROGRAMME IN COMMUNICATION SCIENCE

10.2. Behavior models

Acoustic correlates of stress and their use in diagnosing syllable fusion in Tongan. James White & Marc Garellek UCLA

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

The Odd-Parity Parsing Problem 1 Brett Hyde Washington University May 2008

Full text of O L O W Science As Inquiry conference. Science as Inquiry

Linguistics 220 Phonology: distributions and the concept of the phoneme. John Alderete, Simon Fraser University

Florida Reading Endorsement Alignment Matrix Competency 1

Rule-based Expert Systems

I propose an analysis of thorny patterns of reduplication in the unrelated languages Saisiyat

Contrasting English Phonology and Nigerian English Phonology

Clinical Application of the Mean Babbling Level and Syllable Structure Level

A Neural Network GUI Tested on Text-To-Phoneme Mapping

THE RECOGNITION OF SPEECH BY MACHINE

WHY SOLVE PROBLEMS? INTERVIEWING COLLEGE FACULTY ABOUT THE LEARNING AND TEACHING OF PROBLEM SOLVING

Developing an Assessment Plan to Learn About Student Learning

CONSTRUCTION OF AN ACHIEVEMENT TEST Introduction One of the important duties of a teacher is to observe the student in the classroom, laboratory and

ADDIS ABABA UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF GRADUATE STUDIES MODELING IMPROVED AMHARIC SYLLBIFICATION ALGORITHM

Salience in Sociolinguistics

The College Board Redesigned SAT Grade 12

Multilingual Speech Data Collection for the Assessment of Pronunciation and Prosody in a Language Learning System

ELA/ELD Standards Correlation Matrix for ELD Materials Grade 1 Reading

Listener-oriented phonology

Transcription:

J. Linguistics 43 (2007), 223 267. f 2007 Cambridge University Press Printed in the United Kingdom REVIEWS J. Linguistics 43 (2007). doi:10.1017/s0022226706214580 f 2007 Cambridge University Press Andrea Calabrese, Markedness and economy in a derivational model of phonology (Studies in Generative Grammar 80). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, 2005. Pp. viii+532. Reviewed by ANDREW NEVINS, Harvard University Researchers across various disciplines from propositional logic to theoretical linguistics agree that rules and constraints are always logically intertranslatable, with Mohanan (2000: 145ff.) being the most recent to emphasize the relevance of this insight for phonological theory: (1) ppq is equivalent to :(p & :q) [+nasal] p[+voice] *[+nasal, xvoice] Unfortunately, most linguists take this equivalence to mean that one should adopt a model which is either rules-only or constraints-only, for the sake of theoretical sanitation. Thus, proponents of Optimality Theory (OT) employ only constraints, while proponents of classical generative phonology (see, for example, Kenstowicz & Kisseberth 1979) employ only rules. At last, someone recognizes that intertranslatability does not mean that a given phonological model must choose between rules and constraints and should have only one or the other. As Andrea Calabrese argues in this book, logical equivalence does not imply equivalent explanatory adequacy for a given phonological phenomenon (34). According to Calabrese, an ideal phonological theory should contain BOTH constraints and rules (119), each serving a different function: (2) (a) Constraints are instructions to avoid a given configuration. (b) Rules are instructions to create a given configuration. Employing both rules and constraints allows for a principled separation between what is a conspiracy, e.g. the ban on three consecutive consonants (*CCC) (Kisseberth 1970), and what is not, e.g. the ban on open syllables ending with [a] (*a] Syll ) (McCarthy 2000). Calabrese proposes that only conspiracies should be handled with constraints, and illustrates at length with a discussion of vowel hiatus. Constraints are characterized by the fact that they have multiple repairs across languages. Thus, in the survey conducted by Casali (1998), vowel 223

JOURNAL OF LINGUISTICS hiatus is sometimes repaired by deleting the first vowel, sometimes by deleting the second vowel, sometimes by deleting the least sonorous vowel, sometimes by glide insertion, and sometimes by vowel coalescence. Rules, on the other hand, characterize processes that never have multiple repairs across languages. For example, o-raising before word-final voiced obstruents in Polish is better represented by the rule in (3a) than by the constraint in (3b). (3) (a) [+back, +round] p[+high]/_ [xsonorant, +voice] # (b) *[+back, +round, xhigh] [xsonorant, +voice] # The constraint in (3b) is not a good model of Polish o-raising because it would lead one to expect that a possible repair strategy lies in changing any of the features of [back], [round] or [high] in order to satisfy the constraint. However, Polish o-raising involves the instruction to CREATE the vowel [u], not merely to get rid of the offending combination in (3b). Rules are often the best model for phonological processes like Polish o-raising, which are idiosyncratic, language-particular effects of rule-telescoping. Bach & Harms s (1972) crazy rules represent cases that are not conspiracies, but processes that learners, lacking historical knowledge, can only suppose are due to idiosyncratic, conventionalized instructions. On the other hand, in Italian dialects, the effect of metaphony on mid [xa(dvanced)t(ongue)r(oot)] vowels is very well handled by the inviolable constraint in (4), first proposed in Calabrese s 1988 dissertation. (4) *[+high, xatr] Since in Italian, metaphony involves the transfer of the feature [+high] to a stressed vowel when the target is [xatr], it will by nature contravene (4). As Calabrese (1998) shows, various Italian dialects resolve the offending configuration created by metaphony in different ways. Foggiano deletes [xatr], resulting in hypermetaphony, where a mid lax vowel alternates with a high tense vowel. Arpino enacts breaking ; that is, it creates a diphthong whose first half is [+high] and whose second half is [xatr], thus separating the offending combination under separate timing slots. (In the book under review, Calabrese accounts for breaking or diphthongization via fission: when a co-occurrence constraint against [*+af,+bg] is resolved by insertion of the value [xbg], the result is a feature bundle containing both [+bg, xbg], which is most economically repaired by separating the features into their own bundle.) A nice theme throughout Calabrese s book is the idea that dialect variation results from different choices of repair strategy for the same constraint. There are many potential advantages of adopting the model proposed here instead of OT. Formulating the ban on open syllables ending with [a], which is not invoked in any conspiracy, in terms of constraints does not 224

REVIEWS represent any insightful advancement over a rule. It is better to make predictions about what kinds of processes will lead to various repairs and what kind of processes will not, rather than predicting that any or every phonological process represents a conspiracy. Calabrese s constraints are resolved by a ranking of certain dedicated repairs, all of which are phonological operations, that is, deletion or insertion operations. Calabrese s particular formulation of how repairs work avoids the need to postulate dozens of highly specific IDENT constraints, which are otiose in his model. Finally, employing rules allows stating a general process (e.g. assimilation) as factored into a single rule with varying conditions. A constraint formalism does not really accomplish this. Calabrese develops a very sensible theory of markedness. In order to express coda s-palatalization in Brazilian Portuguese, OT would need to posit a constraint like *s] Syll. In Calabrese s model, this allophony is not derived by constraints but expressed as part of a conventionalized instruction to insert the feature [xanterior] into coda stridents, which has the advantage of not having to corrupt the theory of markedness. Here, Calabrese builds on his previous work on grounded constraints, which predates Archangeli & Pulleyblank s (1994) important book. Importantly, Calabrese s constraints are always inviolable in a given language. Thus, *CCC is inviolable in Yawelmani, *[+high, xatr] is inviolable in Italian, and so forth. In his model of constraint-checking and repair, Calabrese invokes serial computation. The crucial difference between his constraints and OT is thus that the constraints can be checked and repaired in a serial derivation, which means that Calabrese can handle opacity without any new innovations, in classical ordering fashion. Constraint-checking may be ordered with respect to rules (183). Calabrese s book is to be commended for its explicitness about representational assumptions. Since Calabrese is not constrained by using OT-style tableaux, his diagrams and derivations include much fuller representational detail (e.g. explicit featural representations, and full skeletal and prosodic structure). In what follows, I would like to present an analysis of glide fortition in various languages that demonstrates how the insightful analysis of Palatalization and affrication (chapter 4) can be extended to new processes with positive results. Note that not everything I say below would be explicitly adopted or necessarily endorsed by Calabrese (my analysis combines featural proposals and analyses in Halle 2005, Calabrese s book, and Chitoran & Nevins 2006), but the point here is to illustrate the architecture that is proposed in the book under review. Let us begin by taking a closer look at phonological processes that change the Designated Articulator (DA) or primary place of articulation of a consonant and how they are modeled in Calabrese s book. Consider the 225

JOURNAL OF LINGUISTICS well-known process of velar softening, whereby /ki/ p[si] or [tsi] (e.g. English electric, electricity and Italian [amiko] friend, [amitsi] friends ). Calabrese proposes an implicational relationship between tongue body and tongue blade features, whereby the raising of the tongue body to produce a constriction requires an automatic raising of the posterior part of the tongue blade, following articulatory research in Perkell (1980). (5) tongue body [+high, xback] $ tongue blade [ anterior, +distributed] As a consequence of (5), the future representation of the front high vowel /i/ contains [xanterior, +distributed], on the assumption that vowels may have two designated articulators. Velar softening results from the assimilation rule in (6), which spreads the tongue blade node and its dependents: (6) Spread [tongue blade: xanterior, +distributed] leftward to a velar stop The result of (6) for a [DA: dorsal] stop such as /k/ leads to the feature set in (7). (Note that & is a typographical convenience used here to separate place & manner/voice features.) (7) [DA: tongue body] [tongue blade: xanterior, +distributed] & [xvoice, xcontinuant] However, (7) violates a crucial feature co-occurrence filter (cf. (8)) that Calabrese assumes is inviolable in all languages that lack laminal palatoalveolar stops. As shown by Lahiri & Blumstein (1984), such segments are extremely rare, possibly because the length of a laminal constriction makes it difficult to execute the abrupt release characteristic of a stop (Catford 1977: 152). (8) *[+distributed, xcontinuant] The filter in (8) is presumably what underlies the cross-linguistic markedness of palatal stops. It represents a true constraint, i.e. one which has many cross-linguistic repairs. If it were to be expressed solely in terms of rules, we would fail to capture the conspiratorial generalization that fricativization and affrication in various languages are repairs to (8). In English, the filter in (8) is repaired by deleting [xanterior] and [xcontinuant]. (7) thus becomes (9). (9) [DA: tongue body] [tongue blade: +anterior, +distributed] & [xvoice, +continuant] Note that the repairs to (8) depend on the value of [voice]. A voiceless segment is repaired by the deletion of the features [xcontinuant] and [xanterior], yielding /s/, whereas a voiced segment will be repaired by the deletion of [xanterior] at the left margin and [xcontinuant] at the right margin, resulting in /dz/. In Italian, on the other hand, the repair of voiceless as well as voiced segments will result in an affricate. As Catford s (1977) 226

REVIEWS observation is relevant only to the release portion of a stop, Calabrese (343) proposes that [+continuant] may be inserted at the right margin of the feature set in (7). Fission (described above for diphthongization) subsequently yields [ts]. The feature set in (9) involves activation of two designated articulators, viz. both tongue body and tongue blade. Following secondary-articulator promotion and primary-articulator demotion (cf. Romanian k w pp), (9) becomes (10). (10) [DA: tongue blade: +anterior, +distributed] & [xvoice, +continuant] The features in (10) correspond to [s]. The derivation of English velar softening thus proceeds via two very well-motivated steps: (i) the correlation statement between tongue body and tongue blade features in (5); and (ii) the constraint in (8), which is repaired by deleting the offending features and reconfiguring the Designated Articulator. In considering what drives the operation of secondary-articulator promotion, it is useful to assume the cross-classification of vowels, glides, and consonants in (11), where [consonantal] is a major class feature, and [vocalic] specifies degree of constriction. (11) Cross-classification of vowels, glides and consonants Vowels Glides Consonants + [consonantal] + [vocalic] This classification enables a representational encoding of Maddieson & Emmorey s (1985) finding that vowels and glides differ in their degree of constriction. It is further necessary to assume a co-occurrence filter that bans a consonant with two designated articulators (cf. (12)). As argued by Halle (2005: 37), [i]n most languages, consonants may not have more than one designated articulator. (12) *[+consonantal, 2 DA] Calabrese proposes that markedness is a usage cost ; this suggests that (12) is not active in all languages, but that in those languages in which it is active, complex segments such as labiovelar [gb] are inviolably disallowed (see Sagey 1986). Importantly, (12) has no effect on glides, which are [xconsonantal] and which host two designated articulators: /j/ has both [DA: dorsal & DA: coronal], whereas /w/ has both [DA: dorsal & DA: labial]. The reason for formulating (12) as a constraint is once again the fact that it has a wide variety of different repairs. In the step from (9) to (10), the violation of (12) is repaired by deleting one of the DAs. Deletion of the dorsal DA results in a coronal fricative. 227

JOURNAL OF LINGUISTICS One of the predictions that (12) makes is that, if glides have two designated articulators DA: tongue body AND either DA: tongue blade (in the case of /j/) or DA: lips (in the case of /w/) then only one of these DAs will be able to remain when a glide becomes [+consonantal]. Empirical support for this prediction is Kaisse s (1992) important study of the behavior of glides under fortition. She observes that in Bergüner Romantsch, the glide /j/ alternates with [k]. The derivation is shown in (13). (13) Underlying representation: [DA: dorsal, +high, xback] [DA: coronal, xanterior, +distributed] & [ consonantal, xvocalic] Spreading/change to: [DA: dorsal] [DA: coronal] & [+consonantal, xvocalic] Checking of inviolable (12): violated Repair to (12): delete [DA: coronal], resulting in [DA: dorsal] & [+consonantal, xvocalic], mapped to [k] By contrast, Harris & Kaisse (1999: 146) note the fortition of /j/ to [z] in Argentinian Spanish, also modeled here as the result of a change to [+consonantal] and subsequent repair to the co-occurrence filter in (12) via promotion of coronal as the DA. (14) Underlying representation: [DA: dorsal, +high, xback] [DA: coronal, anterior, +distributed] & [ consonantal, xvocalic] Syllable-initial change to [+consonantal]: [DA: dorsal] [DA: coronal] & [+consonantal, xvocalic] Checking of inviolable (12): violated Repair to (12): Delete [DA: dorsal], resulting in [DA: coronal, xanterior, +distributed] & [+consonantal, xvocalic], mapped to [z] Thus, (12) receives support as a constraint from the fact that it enjoys different repairs in different languages. Finally, to return to the effect that spreading the features of the high front vowel /i/ has on stops (discussed above for velar palatalization), the resolution of (12) as shown in (10) may be contrasted with what happens in the Brazilian language Maimandé. The prediction of (12) is that when a stop is both [DA: dorsal] & [DA: coronal] as the result of spreading, languages may choose to repair the constraint violation differently. As discussed in Halle (2005), in Maimandé, the underlying sequence /it/ is mapped to [ik]. (15) Rightward spreading of [DA: dorsal, +high, xback] (cf. (5)) to /t/: [DA: coronal] [DA: dorsal, +high, xback] & [ voice, xcontinuant] Constraint checking of (12): violated Repair: delete [DA: coronal], mapped to /k/ 228

REVIEWS Thus, the wide range of different behaviors in English velar palatalization, Romantsch glide fortition, and Maimandé dorsalization all result from the operation of (12) (often in concert with (8)). This is the kind of mileage we should expect out of a constraint. In conclusion, Calabrese s book represents an important move towards a realistic model of markedness within a theory that is constrained in its predictions and explicit in its representational proposals. Calabrese s model, where the grammar contains both rules and constraints, may not be everybody s idea of an Occamian theory, but something like it has got to be the right theory. REFERENCES Archangeli, D. & Pulleyblank, D. (1994). Grounded phonology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Bach, E. & Harms, R. (1972). How do languages get crazy rules? In Stockwell, R. & Macaulay, R. (eds.), Linguistic change and generative theory: essays. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press. 1 21. Calabrese, A. (1988). Towards a theory of phonological alphabets. Ph.D. dissertation, MIT. Calabrese, A. (1998). Metaphony revisited. Rivista di Linguistica 10. 7 68. Casali, R. (1998). Resolving hiatus. New York: Garland. Catford, J. C. (1977). Fundamental problems in phonetics. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press. Chitoran, I. & Nevins, A. (2006). Towards a phonetic and phonological typology of glides. Presented at the 80th Annual Meeting of the Linguistic Society of America, Albuquerque, NM, in January. Halle, M. (2005). Palatalization/velar softening: what it is and what it tells us about the nature of language. Linguistic Inquiry 36. 23 41. Harris, J. W. & Kaisse, E. (1999). Palatal vowels, glides, and obstruents in Argentinian Spanish. Phonology 16. 117 190. Kaisse, E. (1992). Can [consonantal] spread? Language 68. 313 332. Kenstowicz, M. & Kisseberth, C. (1979). Generative phonology. New York: Academic Press. Kisseberth, C. (1970). On the functional unity of phonological rules. Linguistic Inquiry 1. 291 306. Lahiri, A. & Blumstein, S. E. (1984). A re-evaluation of the feature coronal. Journal of Phonetics 12. 133 145. Maddieson, I. & Emmorey, K. (1985). Relationship between semivowels and vowels: cross-linguistic investigations of acoustic difference and coarticulation. Phonetica 42. 163 174. McCarthy, J. (2000). Sympathy and phonological opacity. Phonology 16. 331 401. Mohanan, K. P. (2000). The theoretical substance of the optimality formalism. The Linguistic Review 17. 143 166. Perkell, J. (1980). Phonetic features and the physiology of speech production. In Butterworth, B. (ed.), Language production, vol. 1: Speech and talk. New York: Academic Press. 337 372. Sagey, E. (1986). The representation of features and relations in non-linear phonology. Ph.D. dissertation, MIT. Author s address: Department of Linguistics, 317 Boylston Hall, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, U.S.A. E-mail: nevins@fas.harvard.edu (Received 19 October 2006) 229