*Physics Education Research Group, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN

Similar documents
Reflective problem solving skills are essential for learning, but it is not my job to teach them

WHY SOLVE PROBLEMS? INTERVIEWING COLLEGE FACULTY ABOUT THE LEARNING AND TEACHING OF PROBLEM SOLVING

Pedagogical Content Knowledge for Teaching Primary Mathematics: A Case Study of Two Teachers

THE PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIVERSITY SCHREYER HONORS COLLEGE DEPARTMENT OF MATHEMATICS ASSESSING THE EFFECTIVENESS OF MULTIPLE CHOICE MATH TESTS

What is PDE? Research Report. Paul Nichols

ReFresh: Retaining First Year Engineering Students and Retraining for Success

Full text of O L O W Science As Inquiry conference. Science as Inquiry

Observing Teachers: The Mathematics Pedagogy of Quebec Francophone and Anglophone Teachers

Extending Place Value with Whole Numbers to 1,000,000

Inquiry Learning Methodologies and the Disposition to Energy Systems Problem Solving

Achievement Level Descriptors for American Literature and Composition

Creating Meaningful Assessments for Professional Development Education in Software Architecture

The Indices Investigations Teacher s Notes

Arizona s English Language Arts Standards th Grade ARIZONA DEPARTMENT OF EDUCATION HIGH ACADEMIC STANDARDS FOR STUDENTS

Stacks Teacher notes. Activity description. Suitability. Time. AMP resources. Equipment. Key mathematical language. Key processes

Mapping the Assets of Your Community:

TabletClass Math Geometry Course Guidebook

Guidelines for Writing an Internship Report

Monitoring Metacognitive abilities in children: A comparison of children between the ages of 5 to 7 years and 8 to 11 years

Case study Norway case 1

Copyright Corwin 2015

5. UPPER INTERMEDIATE

PAGE(S) WHERE TAUGHT If sub mission ins not a book, cite appropriate location(s))

The lab is designed to remind you how to work with scientific data (including dealing with uncertainty) and to review experimental design.

Metadiscourse in Knowledge Building: A question about written or verbal metadiscourse

The College Board Redesigned SAT Grade 12

Kindergarten Lessons for Unit 7: On The Move Me on the Map By Joan Sweeney

Number of students enrolled in the program in Fall, 2011: 20. Faculty member completing template: Molly Dugan (Date: 1/26/2012)

Rubric for Scoring English 1 Unit 1, Rhetorical Analysis

MASTER OF ARTS IN APPLIED SOCIOLOGY. Thesis Option

MASTER S THESIS GUIDE MASTER S PROGRAMME IN COMMUNICATION SCIENCE

DIDACTIC MODEL BRIDGING A CONCEPT WITH PHENOMENA

NCEO Technical Report 27

Improving Conceptual Understanding of Physics with Technology

How to analyze visual narratives: A tutorial in Visual Narrative Grammar

Digital Fabrication and Aunt Sarah: Enabling Quadratic Explorations via Technology. Michael L. Connell University of Houston - Downtown

PHILOSOPHY & CULTURE Syllabus

Improving Advanced Learners' Communication Skills Through Paragraph Reading and Writing. Mika MIYASONE

A Study of the Effectiveness of Using PER-Based Reforms in a Summer Setting

This Performance Standards include four major components. They are

Classifying combinations: Do students distinguish between different types of combination problems?

STUDENTS' RATINGS ON TEACHER

LEAD 612 Advanced Qualitative Research Fall 2015 Dr. Lea Hubbard Camino Hall 101A

FIGURE IT OUT! MIDDLE SCHOOL TASKS. Texas Performance Standards Project

Characteristics of Functions

Getting Started with Deliberate Practice

Teachers Guide Chair Study

Lecturing Module

How People Learn Physics

Primary Teachers Perceptions of Their Knowledge and Understanding of Measurement

Designing a Rubric to Assess the Modelling Phase of Student Design Projects in Upper Year Engineering Courses

Developing an Assessment Plan to Learn About Student Learning

Statistical Analysis of Climate Change, Renewable Energies, and Sustainability An Independent Investigation for Introduction to Statistics

TABE 9&10. Revised 8/2013- with reference to College and Career Readiness Standards

The Heart of Philosophy, Jacob Needleman, ISBN#: LTCC Bookstore:

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

Major Milestones, Team Activities, and Individual Deliverables

Focus of the Unit: Much of this unit focuses on extending previous skills of multiplication and division to multi-digit whole numbers.

Advancing the Discipline of Leadership Studies. What is an Academic Discipline?

Developing Effective Teachers of Mathematics: Factors Contributing to Development in Mathematics Education for Primary School Teachers

Innovative Methods for Teaching Engineering Courses

A BLENDED MODEL FOR NON-TRADITIONAL TEACHING AND LEARNING OF MATHEMATICS

A Study of Successful Practices in the IB Program Continuum

Welcome to the Purdue OWL. Where do I begin? General Strategies. Personalizing Proofreading

Exemplar Grade 9 Reading Test Questions

South Carolina English Language Arts

Positive turning points for girls in mathematics classrooms: Do they stand the test of time?

CHAPTER V: CONCLUSIONS, CONTRIBUTIONS, AND FUTURE RESEARCH

Concept mapping instrumental support for problem solving

Critical Thinking in Everyday Life: 9 Strategies

Houghton Mifflin Online Assessment System Walkthrough Guide

The Writing Process. The Academic Support Centre // September 2015

A cautionary note is research still caught up in an implementer approach to the teacher?

Evaluation of a College Freshman Diversity Research Program

Content analysis (qualitative, thematic) (Last updated: 9/4/06, Yan Zhang)

Systematic reviews in theory and practice for library and information studies

Marking the Text. AVID Critical Reading

Strategies for Solving Fraction Tasks and Their Link to Algebraic Thinking

Synthesis Essay: The 7 Habits of a Highly Effective Teacher: What Graduate School Has Taught Me By: Kamille Samborski

Reinventing College Physics for Biologists: Explicating an Epistemological Curriculum

Maximizing Learning Through Course Alignment and Experience with Different Types of Knowledge

November 2012 MUET (800)

Grade 2: Using a Number Line to Order and Compare Numbers Place Value Horizontal Content Strand

Sample from: 'State Studies' Product code: STP550 The entire product is available for purchase at STORYPATH.

Mathematics Program Assessment Plan

Scoring Guide for Candidates For retake candidates who began the Certification process in and earlier.

Cognitive Thinking Style Sample Report

Delaware Performance Appraisal System Building greater skills and knowledge for educators

Graduate Program in Education

Miami-Dade County Public Schools

Calculators in a Middle School Mathematics Classroom: Helpful or Harmful?

Virtually Anywhere Episodes 1 and 2. Teacher s Notes

A Correlation of. Grade 6, Arizona s College and Career Ready Standards English Language Arts and Literacy

White Paper. The Art of Learning

WORK OF LEADERS GROUP REPORT

Language Acquisition Chart

The Talent Development High School Model Context, Components, and Initial Impacts on Ninth-Grade Students Engagement and Performance

SCHEMA ACTIVATION IN MEMORY FOR PROSE 1. Michael A. R. Townsend State University of New York at Albany

Using e-portfolios to Measure Student Learning in a Graduate Preparation Program in Higher Education 1. Steven M. Janosik 2 and Tara E.

learning collegiate assessment]

Transcription:

Multi-Layered Concept Maps for the Analysis of Complex Interview Data Roundtable Discussion presented at the Physics Education Research Conference August 7, 2003, Madison, WI Charles Henderson +, Edit Yerushalmi x, Kenneth Heller*, Patricia Heller*, Vince H. Kuo* + Western Michigan University, Kalamazoo, MI ξ Weizmann Institute, Rehovot, Israel *Physics Education Research Group, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN Concept maps were first used by Novak and Gowin [1] to understand and describe student conceptions about physical phenomena based on interview transcripts. We have used features of this technique in the analysis of interviews with physics faculty aimed at understanding their conceptions of several interrelated aspects of the teaching and learning of physics. Concept maps have several advantages in the analysis of complex interview data. These advantages include: (1) reflecting, as much as possible, the interviewee worldview; (2) providing a powerful representation tool to show complex interrelations between conceptions; (3) forcing the researchers to be explicit about claimed interrelations in the data; and (4) establishing a transparent link between the results and the raw data. In this paper, we use concrete examples from our study to explain the procedures that we used to go from the interview transcripts to the final concept maps. I. Introduction Multi-layered concept maps can be used in the analysis of many types of interview data. The goal of this type of analysis is to use interview transcripts to understand what conceptions * a group of people have about some phenomena. These goals are consistent with phenomenological [3, 4] or phenomenographic [5, 6] research studies. Throughout this paper clarifying examples will be given from our study, which was designed to understand how six research university faculty conceive of the teaching and learning of problem solving in the context of an introductory calculus-based physics course. [7-13] We hope that other researchers can benefit from our experiences. II. What is a Multi-Layered Concept Map? A multi-layered concept map is a hierarchically arranged set of ideas and can be developed based on research participants discourse during an interview. There are four basic layers: a main map, composite child maps, participant child maps, and participant statements. Main Map The main map is the highest layer and consists of the important features of the way that the interview participants conceive of the phenomena and how these features are related. For example, in our study, there were 14 important features represented on the main map. Figure 1 shows two of these important features and how they are related. Important Feature 1 indicates that the instructors have the conception that only some college students are capable of achieving the desired course outcomes. Important Feature 2 indicates that the instructors have conceptions about the problem-solving process. The Main Map also shows the relationship between these two important features. Taken together, it represents the instructors conception that some college students learn how to solve physics problems. * In our study and in this paper, the term conception is used as a broad term to describe beliefs, knowledge, mental images, preferences, and similar aspects of cognition. [2] - 1 -

The main map also indicates which of the interview participants conceptions of the phenomena could be described by each of the important features. In our study, almost all of the important features and their relationships described all instructors. Thus, notation for Some College individual instructors was only used when a feature or relationship was not representative of all instructors. The complete main map from our study is shown in Figure 2. This map was partly affected by the way the interview was structured. Several important features are closely linked to the interview in the sense that the interview was structured around these features. For example, in the interview we asked instructors about what they expected students to know or be able to do at the end of the course. Their responses, along with data from other parts of the interview, formed the important feature of Appropriate Knowledge. Other important features (, Some College ) and the relationships between important features originated from the instructors discourse. Notice that even from this highest layer map we have useful information about the ways these instructors conceive of the teaching and learning of problem solving. For example, they all believe that only some of the students in their classes will learn. The details about which students they believe can learn are found in the child maps. learn how to Solve Physics Important Feature 1 Important Feature 2 Relationship Figure 1: Example of two main features and their relationship from the Main Map. Teacher can help 1. Some College learn how to 2. Solve Physics provides Management 11 - Path A 12 - Path B 13 - Path C based on 3. ' Current State of students when they affects how they while they Engage in Learning Activities influences by of of using of Providing Resources Setting Constraints Making Suggestions 4. Working while/ after 5. Feedback 6. Looking/ Listening (RU1, RU2, RU3, RU5, RU6) 14. Reflection on Teaching on the can be can be can be can be 7. appropriate problems 8. individualized responses 9. appropriate example solutions Lectures to (Path A) to (Path B) get the to (Path C) 10. appropriate knowledge Figure 2: Main map from our study. The numbered boxes indicate important features that are elaborated in a child map. For some of the important features (, lectures) there was not enough information available from the interviews to construct a child map. - 2 -

Composite Child Maps A composite child map contains the details about how all participants conceive of a particular important feature. Figure 3 shows a simplified composite child map that shows the details for the important feature of Some College. This map represents the instructors conceptions of the differences between college students who can learn how to solve physics problems and those who can not. It shows a fairly strong agreement among the instructors that students can learn if they have enough natural ability as well as beneficial to learning. This map also identifies two types of students that these instructors believe will not learn. There are some students who do not have the ability to learn and then there are other students enough ability, but have other that prevent learning. These, such as not working hard, are included in the complete composite child map (Figure 8). Notation in each of the boxes (, RU1, RU2, etc.) indicates which of the participants expressed a particular conception. The notation unclear is used when the researcher infers that a participant has a particular conception, but the conception is not explicitly expressed by the participant. Similar notation can be used on the links between boxes. We used the convention that notation on links would only be added if it would be unclear otherwise. Participant Child Maps Participant child maps contain the details about how an individual participant conceives of a particular important feature. In addition to providing an in-depth view of an individual they also provide a direct link to the interview data. Figure 4 shows a simplified participant child map for the important feature of Some College. The numbers on each box and link refer to statements made during the interview. The statements are numbered sequentially, making it possible to easily determine which conceptions are expressed in two or more distinct parts of the interview. For example, the conception that not caring is a characteristic that is detrimental to learning is expressed once early in the interview Lower (31, 308, 312, 313) who one hopes can learn A little bit (31) of how to detrimental to learning (87, 259, 295, 300, 304, 305, 312, 313) don't care (87, 295, 304) not enough natural ability (RU2, RU3-unclear, RU4, RU6) cannot be helped much by instructor and don't have the ability (308) will not learn how to and have and have detrimental to learning (RU2, RU3, RU4, RU5, RU6) do not learn 1. Some College enough natural ability (RU2-unclear, RU4, RU5, RU6) how to 2. Solve Physics 1. Some College Middle (30, 313) need help to learn how to(30) 2. Solve Physics beneficial to learning (RU1, RU2, RU4, RU5, RU6) do learn can learn how to more than enough natural ability (RU1-unclear, RU3, RU4, RU5, RU6) do not need much instuctor help and Figure 3: Example of a composite child map for the important feature of Some College. helpful for learning (86, 87, 296, 297, 299, 305, 331, 349) motivated (87, 296, 349) don't need help to learn how to (27) Higher (27, 29, 296, 313) Figure 4: Example of a participant child map for the important feature of Some College. This one is from Instructor 6 (RU6). - 3 -

(statement #87) and again later (statements #295 and #304). Similar to the composite child maps, we used the convention that notation on links would only be used if it would be unclear otherwise. Participant Statements Participant statements are the basic units of analysis. Each statement was constructed from the interview transcripts to capture a single idea expressed by the interviewee. [14] The words used by the participant were retained whenever possible. Each statement was designed to stand alone and was assigned a code to allow easy reference back to the original transcript. Parentheses were used when it was necessary to add context to a statement. Text in parentheses was not considered to be part of the statement. Figure 5 shows an example of a statement. Each of the 1½ to 2 hour long interviews used in our study yielded approximately 400 statements. III. Advantages of Using Multi-Layered Concept Maps A. Multi-layered concept maps reflect, as much as possible, the interviewee worldview Concept maps allow the researchers to identify new conceptions that are implicitly or explicitly expressed by the interviewees. Since the concept maps are built directly from interviewee statements, the new conceptions emerge from the interviewees point of view. Often these conceptions could not have been explicitly stated by the interviewees or the researchers prior to the construction of the maps. B. Multi-layered concept maps provide a powerful representation tool Three advantages of this representation tool are described below. (Time is one factor that accounts for some students being more successful than others.) And of course there are some that just don t have the ability too. Figure 5: An example of a participant statement. This one was statement #308 from Instructor 6. Complex interrelations between conceptions can be represented. A concept map can show complex relationships that would be difficult to describe in prose. In addition, concept maps clearly show where a relationship does not occur. For example, in our study, instructors believed that most students had poor problem solving abilities at the beginning of the course. One aspect of this was the instructors conception that students used poor approaches to solving problems, such as tending to rely on looking up formulas rather than understanding physics. This poor problem solving ability, though, is not shown on the Some College composite child map. Thus, it is not seen by these instructors as a factor that limits students from learning how to solve physics problems. Concept maps clearly show the similarities and differences between individual participants. The composite concept maps show the range of participant conceptions as well as the overlap and distribution of these conceptions. For example, in our study, the composite child map for the important feature of Some College shows that all of the instructors have the same conception that only students with enough natural ability and beneficial to learning (such as motivation) will learn how to solve physics problems. Other composite child maps, however, show that the instructors do not have the same conceptions. For example, the composite child map for Solve Physics shows that each of the instructors has one of three distinct conceptions about the process of solving physics problems. - 4 -

The participant child maps can also provide useful information about similarities and differences between participants. For example, comparing the participant child map of Some College for Instructor 3 (see Figure 7) to that of other instructors (for example, Instructor 6 in Figure 6) shows that instructor 3 talked considerably more about the of very good students in his class than any of the other instructors. Instructor 3 also describes student of average students that are detrimental to learning, but not of average students that are beneficial to learning. This contrasts with other instructors who expressed detailed conceptions about of average students that were beneficial to learning. Multiple layers make it possible to quickly zoom on an area of interest. This representation allows the reader to use higher-order maps to navigate quickly to a particular area of interest at the desired level of detail. C. Researchers are forced to be explicit about interrelations in the data We found this to be one of the most difficult and most useful aspects of this analysis method. When constructing a concept map, the researcher is required to make an explicit link between two boxes and to name that link. One of the primary goals of constructing the concept maps was to combine similar conceptions in order to clarify the underlying thinking. This was done both within individual participants when constructing the participant maps and across participants when constructing the composite maps. This combination process forced us to clarify our interpretation of the conceptions and be explicit about why and how they were similar or different. This could be done by a single researcher, but we found the different perspectives afforded by multiple researchers to be particularly important at this stage. As with any type of research, one of the problems that a lone researcher has is becoming aware of his/her implicit conceptions. These implicit conceptions can have a significant influence on the research results. For example, in our study, four researchers were involved in the creation of the concept maps. When developing the combined concept maps we would all, at times, use the term feedback. Because of the connections made between feedback and other concepts on the map, it became apparent that we were all using this term to mean different things. Through discussion we were able to come to an agreement about what constituted feedback. This forced us to develop more accurate categorizations of instructor conceptions related to feedback. Instead of feedback, the final composite child maps contained more meaningful terms such as real-time feedback, delayed feedback, and individualized response. D. There is a transparent link between the results and the raw data. This enhances the credibility of the analysis by showing the degree of support for each box and link. [4, 15] On the participant child maps it is possible to see how many interview statements support a particular box or link as well as where in the interview these statements came from. Similarly, on the composite child maps it is possible to see how many participants have conceptions consistent with a particular box or link. In addition, this provides a data trail that can be used by an external or internal researcher to audit the study and evaluate the results. [4] IV. Procedures for Constructing Multi-Layered Concept Maps The five basic stages in constructing multi-layered concept maps are shown in Appendix A (pp. 11-12). The table is meant to summarize the basic procedures used in our study. It is - 5 -

important to note, however, that the actual procedures we used were much more iterative than the relatively distinct stages presented in the table. V. An Example from Our Study This example will follow a piece of interview transcript from one participant, Instructor 6, as it gets broken into statements and then put onto a participant child map. Finally, it will show how this concept map for Instructor 6 and one from another instructor were combined to form a composite concept map. Clarify Focus of Analysis Hycner suggests that a researcher make all possible statements from an interview transcript and then decide which ones can inform the research interests and which can be discarded. [14] We found this impossible, however, since there is considerable interpretation in deciding what constitutes a single statement. In order to be transparent about this interpretation we created 7 broad questions to guide the creation of statements. These questions were based on a system view of teaching/learning, where the system of interest is the people, things, and activities related to an introductory physics course. From this perspective the instructor and students enter the system with some set of. The instructor and students interact within the system. There are also outside influences on the system. The instructor and students leave the system with some new set of. The 7 broad questions included What are the instructor (, beliefs about how physics should be taught) when he/she enters the teaching/learning system, What are the possible interactions between participants in the teaching/learning system?, What student outcomes does the instructor desire from the course? How do they compare to actual outcomes?. Making Statements After the interview, the audio portion of the interview was transcribed. Table 1 (next page) shows a portion of the Instructor 6 s interview transcript and the resulting statements. This portion of the interview primarily informed the important feature of Some College (Map 1), which contains qualities of students that the instructor explicitly relates to success or failure in learning how to solve physics problems. The column labeled Used? indicates whether the statement was used in one of the participant child maps for Instructor 6 or whether it was excluded for being vague ( V ), not relevant ( NR ), or not understandable ( NU ). When the interview text was not understandable, the text was left as is. Creating a Participant Child Map Figure 6 shows the complete participant concept map for the important feature of Some College for Instructor 6, which contains information from the statements from the example portion of the interview and other statements from other places in the interview. In each box on the concept map and on each link is the statement number that provides support for that particular idea. - 6 -

Table 1: An example of statements made from a portion of the interview transcript. Interviewer: Ok. I want to talk about two different kinds of students. And looking at your chart [created earlier in the interview] there s students that come in knowing stuff, which is great. But there are also a lot of students who don t come in being able to handle these areas. And of those students that come in without being able to handle them, some of the students get better and some of the students don t. So I m wondering what the difference between those two types of students is -- the students who improve during the class and the students that don t. Transcript Stmt. # Statement Used? Instructor 6: Well, I mean, there s certainly a lot of categories. First of all, there s the ones that just don t care, that aren t gonna get any better. And of course, there s the other extreme, the people that really have the intelligence and motivation to look into these things. I think problem solving in general is something that some people find fun, and some others don t. I mean, some people like going through, and I think probably most physicists are in it because they like doing it. And so I think the people that enjoy a challenge, that enjoy the idea of working these things out, and coming up with knowledge that they didn t have before. I mean, I think that s the sort of sense of wonder sort of thing. I think on the negative end of things there s a lot of people that just think all this stuff is just totally beyond them, they ll never be able to do it. And therefore they re not going to try. I think some people have a sort of feeling that if they re not going to be good at it, why worry about it. It s not going to be important for them. Here are these things about there was a newspaper article that [name] used to have on his office a long, long time ago, which was some columnist saying, why do we have to know algebra anyway? I never see any want ads for an algebra-doer! or things like that. So some people, they have a tendency to disparage what they can t do. And so they won t care about it. I think that s the biggest problem with teaching these big general courses, is you get that bigger fraction that just don t care. 294 Well, I mean, there s certainly a lot of categories. V 295 First of all, there s the students that just don t care, that Map 1 aren t gonna get any better. 296 And of course, there s the other extreme (as opposed to Map 1 students who just don't care and aren t gonna get any better), the people that really have the intelligence and motivation to look into these things. 297 I think problem solving in general is something that Map 1 some people find fun, and some others don t. (This is a difference between students who improve and students who don t.). 298 (I think problem solving in general is something that some people find fun, and some others don t.) I mean, some people like going through, and I think probably most physicists are in it because they like doing it. 299 (I think problem solving in general is something that some people find fun, and some others don t.) And so I think the people that enjoy a challenge, that enjoy the idea of working these things out, and coming up with knowledge that they didn t have before (will improve in the class). I mean, I think that s the sort of sense of wonder sort of thing. 300 I think on the negative end of things there s a lot of students that just think all this stuff is just totally beyond them, they ll never be able to do it. 301 (I think on the negative end of things there s a lot of people that just think all this stuff is just totally beyond them, they ll never be able to do it.) And therefore they re not going to try. I think some people have a sort of feeling that if they re not going to be good at it, why worry about it. It s not going to be important for them. NR (this study is not concerned with why people go into physics) Map 1 Map 1 Map 1 Map 3 302 Here are these things about NU 303 There was a newspaper article that [name] used to NR have on his office a long, long time ago, which was (it s not some columnist saying, why do we have to know clear what algebra anyway? I never see any want ads for an this example algebra-doer! or things like that. relates to) 304 So some people, they have a tendency to disparage what they can t do. And so they won t care about it. I think that s the biggest problem with teaching these big general courses, is you get that bigger fraction that just don t care. Map 1 Map 3-7 -

1. Some College Lower (31, 308, 312, 313) don't have the ability (308) poor math background (309, 312, 313) ~ 1/3 of the class (32) Middle (30, 313) Whom I target in my class (32) Higher (27, 29, 296, 313) who one hopes can learn ~ 1/3 of the class (32) don't care (87, 295, 304) perhaps because (301, 304) think they can't do it (300, 301, 309) detrimental to learning (87, 259, 295, 300, 304, 305, 312, 313) need help to learn how to(30) helpful for learning (86, 87, 296, 297, 299, 305, 331, 349) enjoy a challenge (299) find problem solving fun (297, 331) motivated (87, 296, 349) spend enough time (305) ~ 1/3 of the class (32) A little bit (31) looking up formula and relying on it as an alternative to understanding (259) don't spend enough time (307) of how to will not learn how to 2. Solve Physics can learn how to having a sense of wonder (299) interest (349) don't need help to learn how to (27) Figure 6: Instructor 6 s participant child map for the relevant feature of Some College. Combining Concept Maps In a similar way, participant child maps were constructed for all of the instructors. The participant child map for Instructor 3 is shown in Figure 7. These participant maps were combined to get the composite child map shown in Figure 8. Note that in combining the concept maps the goal was to combine individual instructor ideas when they seemed to have the same conception and to leave the ideas separate when they seemed to have different conceptions. That is, the composite maps should show the common core of conceptions as well as the range of the conceptions. The wording used on the composite concept maps is the wording that the research team believes can convey the instructor conceptions most accurately and most compactly. are hopeless (390) need to learn the material (225) most students (225) problem solving is difficult (225) cannot be (390) and has 1. Some College Average (390) can be (390) influenced by the instructor (390) ~2% of the class (263, 413) cannot be (390) very good (37, 51, 390, 405, 407) should ace each test (263, 407) often do not take a disciplined path to problem solving (51) can go through the reasoning process (like IS2, IS3) to determine what steps to take (147) do not do not look at the IS that I post (33, 35) these students just don't care about the class (37) detrimental to learning frequently do not learn how to can learn how to 2. Solve Physics do not need help to (36) have real faith in themselves (421) some students may be geniuses (205, 414) most likely to produce a sparse solution (like IS1) (146) Figure 7: Instructor 3 s participant child map for the relevant feature of Some College. - 8 -

As an example of this process, consider the path to the left of the Some College box on each of the participant concept maps. Instructor 6 (see Figure 6) describes a group of students that he calls lower who don t have the ability, have poor math background and who he hopes can learn a little bit about how to solve physics problems. Instructor 3 (see Figure 7) describes a group of students that he calls hopeless, whom the instructor cannot influence. These two instructors seemed to be describing the same thing that there is a group of students in their class who lack some sort of natural ability and who won t learn how to solve physics problems. This, along with similar evidence from the other 4 instructors, led to the creation of the path to the left of the Some College box on the composite map (see Figure 8). Notice that in the not enough natural ability box on the composite map that Instructor 3 is shown as unclear. This is because on the individual map for Instructor 3 (see Figure 7), it is implied, but not explicitly stated that these students hopeless are hopeless because of a lack of natural ability rather than some other cause. Instructors 2, 4, and 6 explicitly identify the lack of natural ability as the reason that these students will not learn how to solve physics problems. 1. Some College 10-15% of students (RU2) ~25% of students (RU4) ~33% of students (RU6) not enough natural ability (RU2, RU3-unclear, RU4, RU6) do not look at posted solutions (RU3) relying on formulas rather than a real understandi ng (RU6) think they can't do it (RU6) Don't Care/ Not Hard Working (RU2, RU3, RU4, RU5, RU6) Poor Study Habits (Reflection) (RU2, RU3, RU4, RU6) Personal Characteristics (RU1, RU6) most students (RU3, RU5) ~33% of students (RU6) No Interest in Physics (RU2, RU5) and have and have detrimental to learning (RU2, RU3, RU4, RU5, RU6) enough natural ability (RU2-unclear, RU4, RU5, RU6) RU6 beneficial to learning (RU1, RU2, RU4, RU5, RU6) RU2 I Target Motivation/ Hard Working (RU1, RU2, RU4, RU5, RU6) Interest in Physics (RU2, RU6) RU6 Good Study Habits (Reflection) (RU2, RU4, RU5) Personal Characteristics (RU1,RU5,RU6) more than enough natural ability (RU1-unclear, RU3, RU4, RU5, RU6) analyze quiz mistakes to learn from them (RU2) ability to admit they don't know something (RU5) maturity (RU5) enjoy a challenge (RU6) a few students (RU3, RU5) ~33% of students (RU6) cannot be helped much by instructor and do not learn do learn do not need much instuctor help and being outgoing (RU1) how to 2. Solve Physics Figure 8: Composite child map for the relevant feature of Some College. - 9 -

VI. References 1. J. D. Novak and D. B. Gowin, Learning how to learn (Cambridge University Press, New York, 1984). 2. A. G. Thompson, "Teachers' beliefs and conceptions: A synthesis of the research," in Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning, edited by D. A. Grouws (MacMillan, New York, 1992). 3. C. Moustakas, Phenomenological research methods (Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA, 1994). 4. J. W. Creswell, "Qualitative inquiry and research design: Choosing among five traditions," in edited by (Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA, 1998). 5. F. Marton and S. Booth, "The idea of phenomenography," in Learning and Awareness, edited by Ference Marton and Shirley Booth (Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, 1997). 6. J. T. E. Richardson, "The concepts and methods of phenomenographic research," Review of Educational Research. 69 (1), 53-82 (1999). 7. P. Heller, K. Heller, C. Henderson, V. Kuo and E. Yerushalmi, "Instructors' beliefs and values about learning problem solving," Proceedings of the Physics Education Research Conference, Rochester, NY., (2001). 8. C. Henderson, K. Heller, P. Heller, V. Kuo and E. Yerushalmi, "Instructors' ideas about problem solving: Setting goals," Proceedings of the Physics Education Research Conference, Rochester, NY., (2001). 9. C. Henderson, Doctoral Dissertation, University of Minnesota, 2002. 10. C. Henderson, K. Heller, P. Heller, V. Kuo and E. Yerushalmi, " learning problem solving in introductory physics: Forming an initial hypothesis of instructors' beliefs," Proceedings of the Physics Education Research Conference, Boise, ID., (2002). 11. V. Kuo, K. Heller, P. Heller, C. Henderson and E. Yerushalmi, "Instructors' ideas about problem solving: Grading," Proceedings of the Physics Education Research Conference, Rochester, NY., (2001). 12. V. Kuo, K. Heller, P. Heller, C. Henderson and E. Yerushalmi, "Teaching students problem solving in introductory physics: Forming an initial hypothesis of instructors' beliefs," Proceedings of the Physics Education Research Conference, Boise, ID., (2002). 13. E. Yerushalmi, K. Heller, P. Heller, C. Henderson and V. Kuo, "Why solve problems? - Interviewing instructors about the learning and teaching of problem solving," Proceedings of the International Research Group on Physics Teaching Conference, Barcelona, Spain. (2000). 14. R. H. Hycner, "Some guidelines for the phenomenological analysis of interview data," Human Studies. 8, 279-303 (1985). 15. M. B. Miles and A. M. Huberman, Qualitative data analysis: An expanded sourcebook (Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA, 1994). - 10 -

Appendix A: Summary of procedures used to construct multi-layered concept maps Analysis Stage Goal Basic Procedure Details/Suggestions 1. Clarify Focus of Analysis 2. Construct Statements 3. Construct Main Map Sharpen focus of analysis and identify researcher bias. This is important since constructing statements is already an interpretive act. Break the interview transcript into smaller units that can be used in constructing concept maps. Develop tentative main map that will help to organize the data and focus further analysis. The main map should present a meaningful story about the way that the interview participants view the phenomenon. Identify general questions that it is hoped can be answered using the interview data. (The analysis is open to other questions that may arise, but guided by these questions.) Rules for creating statements (also see Hycner [14]): Statements should use participant s words and phrasing whenever possible this minimizes the effect of researcher interpretation. Each statement should represent a coherent idea. Statements must be meaningful on their own. Statements should be as brief as possible. Keep statements that are not understandable or don t seem relevant they may be important later. Begin by identifying natural clusters of statements [14, 15] and possible relationships between clusters. These will form the basis for an initial best guess main concept map to use as a starting point. Each of these will eventually become an important feature and have its own child map. (, the cluster of student that prevent them from learning became important feature of Some College ) In our study we had 7 broad openended questions based on our understanding of the context in which the interviewees worked ( What does the instructor think students are like? ). Number statements sequentially from the beginning of the transcript. When writing statements use parentheses to identify information that adds necessary context, but that his not part of the statement. We wrote and organized statements in one column of an excel spreadsheet. This allowed us to use other columns for different categorization schemes and facilitated sorting. To develop skill in constructing statements and identify researcher biases we initially had two researchers make all statements. The resulting statements were compared and differences discussed. After two complete transcripts, the researchers were in agreement 93% of the time. This is more likely to be robust if it is based on the statements from more than one participant. - 11 -

Analysis Stage Goal Basic Procedure Details/Suggestions 4. Construct Participant Child Maps Refine and validate main concept map. 5. Construct Composite Child Maps Create link between raw data and emerging concept map structure. Understand and represent similarities and differences among interview participants. Identify the common core of conceptions that all participants have. Identify the range of conceptions (i.e., the outcome space) for conceptions that are not part of the common core. Take all of the statements from one participant and categorize each as belonging to one or more of the clusters. Start with one of the clusters. Try to arrange the statements into a concept map that shows how that participant spoke about that important feature and how that important feature is related to other important features. Keep the statement number with all statements. Do this for the other clusters. Select the participant who is most unlike the first participant and repeat this process. Revise main map and repeat. Repeat the process above for remaining participants, revising the main map and already completed participant child maps as necessary. Work with one important feature at a time. Construct a composite map that represents the similarities and differences on each of the participant maps. Language on the concept maps should express the underlying ideas as clearly and concisely as possible. It is not necessary to use participant s language, but should be done when possible. Use notation on each box or link to identify which interview participants expressed that idea. Two statements can be combined if the researcher believes that they mean the same thing. If there is any ambiguity it may be best to keep both wordings at this point. Statements can be shortened if there is no loss of information (they do not need to stand on their own since they are now linked to other statements in the context of an important feature). We used three types of researcher checking to diminish the effect of individual researcher interpretations on the final concept maps. 1) Two researchers independently create a map for a particular relevant feature for a particular participant. 2) After completing a concept map another researcher carefully scrutinizes it for aspects that are unclear or lack face validity. 3) After completing a concept map another researcher spot checks the connections between the map and the statements going back to the transcript if necessary. We made the distinction between connections explicitly stated by the interviewee and those inferred by the researcher. The later were identified by dashed lines or dashed line boxes. Arrange all participant concept maps for a particular important feature so that similar ideas are in similar places. This prepares the maps for step 5 and can lead to the identification of discrepancies in the ways that different maps were created. We found that there were two basic types of composite maps. One type was where all of the participants basically thought about that relevant feature in the same way (, the relevant feature of some college students can learn ). The other type was where there were two or more qualitatively different ways that the participants viewed the relevant feature (, the relevant feature of solve physics problems ). We found that it was often necessary to go back to the original transcript to clarify the meaning. - 12 -