STYLISTIC FOREGROUNDING

Similar documents
Reading Grammar Section and Lesson Writing Chapter and Lesson Identify a purpose for reading W1-LO; W2- LO; W3- LO; W4- LO; W5-

Oakland Unified School District English/ Language Arts Course Syllabus

The College Board Redesigned SAT Grade 12

CEFR Overall Illustrative English Proficiency Scales

Highlighting and Annotation Tips Foundation Lesson

Common Core State Standards for English Language Arts

Arizona s English Language Arts Standards th Grade ARIZONA DEPARTMENT OF EDUCATION HIGH ACADEMIC STANDARDS FOR STUDENTS

Text Type Purpose Structure Language Features Article

2006 Mississippi Language Arts Framework-Revised Grade 12

Achievement Level Descriptors for American Literature and Composition

Think A F R I C A when assessing speaking. C.E.F.R. Oral Assessment Criteria. Think A F R I C A - 1 -

Opportunities for Writing Title Key Stage 1 Key Stage 2 Narrative

The Common European Framework of Reference for Languages p. 58 to p. 82

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

Ohio s New Learning Standards: K-12 World Languages

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

LANGUAGE IN INDIA Strength for Today and Bright Hope for Tomorrow Volume 12: 9 September 2012 ISSN

5 th Grade Language Arts Curriculum Map

Life Imitates Lit: A Road Trip to Cultural Understanding. Dr. Patricia Hamilton, Department of English

PAGE(S) WHERE TAUGHT If sub mission ins not a book, cite appropriate location(s))

Oakland Unified School District English/ Language Arts Course Syllabus

Author: Justyna Kowalczys Stowarzyszenie Angielski w Medycynie (PL) Feb 2015

Grade 11 Language Arts (2 Semester Course) CURRICULUM. Course Description ENGLISH 11 (2 Semester Course) Duration: 2 Semesters Prerequisite: None

Florida Reading Endorsement Alignment Matrix Competency 1

Primary English Curriculum Framework

Language Arts: ( ) Instructional Syllabus. Teachers: T. Beard address

Student Name: OSIS#: DOB: / / School: Grade:

Lower and Upper Secondary

Candidates must achieve a grade of at least C2 level in each examination in order to achieve the overall qualification at C2 Level.

Night by Elie Wiesel. Standards Link:

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

5. UPPER INTERMEDIATE

Intensive Writing Class

Pennsylvania Common Core Standards English Language Arts Grade 11

California Department of Education English Language Development Standards for Grade 8

English Language and Applied Linguistics. Module Descriptions 2017/18

AGENDA LEARNING THEORIES LEARNING THEORIES. Advanced Learning Theories 2/22/2016

CS 598 Natural Language Processing

Formative Assessment in Mathematics. Part 3: The Learner s Role

PREP S SPEAKER LISTENER TECHNIQUE COACHING MANUAL

This Performance Standards include four major components. They are

Prentice Hall Literature Common Core Edition Grade 10, 2012

Grade 4. Common Core Adoption Process. (Unpacked Standards)

Derivational: Inflectional: In a fit of rage the soldiers attacked them both that week, but lost the fight.

Constraining X-Bar: Theta Theory

LEXICAL COHESION ANALYSIS OF THE ARTICLE WHAT IS A GOOD RESEARCH PROJECT? BY BRIAN PALTRIDGE A JOURNAL ARTICLE

TRAITS OF GOOD WRITING

ENGLISH. Progression Chart YEAR 8

Pearson Longman Keystone Book D 2013

Mock Trial Preparation In-Class Assignment to Prepare Direct and Cross Examination Roles 25 September 2015 DIRECT EXAMINATION

"Be who you are and say what you feel, because those who mind don't matter and

AN INTRODUCTION (2 ND ED.) (LONDON, BLOOMSBURY ACADEMIC PP. VI, 282)

PUBLIC SCHOOLS OF EDISON TOWNSHIP DIVISION OF CURRICULUM AND INSTRUCTION LLD LANGUAGE ARTS

Common Core Curriculum- Draft

Parallel Evaluation in Stratal OT * Adam Baker University of Arizona

GOLD Objectives for Development & Learning: Birth Through Third Grade

Critical Thinking in Everyday Life: 9 Strategies

The Effect of Extensive Reading on Developing the Grammatical. Accuracy of the EFL Freshmen at Al Al-Bayt University

Rubric for Scoring English 1 Unit 1, Rhetorical Analysis

Advanced Grammar in Use

Loughton School s curriculum evening. 28 th February 2017

Syntax Parsing 1. Grammars and parsing 2. Top-down and bottom-up parsing 3. Chart parsers 4. Bottom-up chart parsing 5. The Earley Algorithm

Prentice Hall Literature: Timeless Voices, Timeless Themes Gold 2000 Correlated to Nebraska Reading/Writing Standards, (Grade 9)

Exemplar Grade 9 Reading Test Questions

Characteristics of the Text Genre Realistic fi ction Text Structure

Common Core Exemplar for English Language Arts and Social Studies: GRADE 1

Big Fish. Big Fish The Book. Big Fish. The Shooting Script. The Movie

Case study Norway case 1

Teaching Vocabulary Summary. Erin Cathey. Middle Tennessee State University

Prentice Hall Literature: Timeless Voices, Timeless Themes, Platinum 2000 Correlated to Nebraska Reading/Writing Standards (Grade 10)

Kindergarten Lessons for Unit 7: On The Move Me on the Map By Joan Sweeney

L1 and L2 acquisition. Holger Diessel

Create Quiz Questions

TASK 1: PLANNING FOR INSTRUCTION AND ASSESSMENT

Enduring Understandings: Students will understand that

Holt McDougal Literature, Grade 11. Write Source, Grade 11

Loveland Schools Literacy Framework K-6

ELA/ELD Standards Correlation Matrix for ELD Materials Grade 1 Reading

Slam Poetry-Theater Lesson. 4/19/2012 dfghjklzxcvbnmqwertyuiopasdfghjklzx. Lindsay Jag Jagodowski

Som and Optimality Theory

MYP Language A Course Outline Year 3

Characteristics of the Text Genre Informational Text Text Structure

STAFF DEVELOPMENT in SPECIAL EDUCATION

Language Acquisition Chart

evans_pt01.qxd 7/30/2003 3:57 PM Page 1 Putting the Domain Model to Work

Georgia Department of Education Dr. John D. Barge, State School Superintendent May 3, 2012 * Page 1 All Rights Reserved

Pearson Longman Keystone Book F 2013

Listening and Speaking Skills of English Language of Adolescents of Government and Private Schools

Observing Teachers: The Mathematics Pedagogy of Quebec Francophone and Anglophone Teachers

Literature and the Language Arts Experiencing Literature

Lecturing Module

English 2, Grade 10 Regular, Honors Curriculum Map

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

Pre-AP English 1-2. Mrs. Kimberly Cloud Career Tech & Global Studies Room N-201

Iraqi EFL Students' Achievement In The Present Tense And Present Passive Constructions

English Language Arts Missouri Learning Standards Grade-Level Expectations

1.2 Interpretive Communication: Students will demonstrate comprehension of content from authentic audio and visual resources.

C a l i f o r n i a N o n c r e d i t a n d A d u l t E d u c a t i o n. E n g l i s h a s a S e c o n d L a n g u a g e M o d e l

Modern Fantasy CTY Course Syllabus

Not the Quit ting Kind

Transcription:

STYLISTIC FOREGROUNDING Definitions and concepts Foreground is a term usually used in art, having opposite meaning to background. It s a very general principle of artistic communication that a work of art in some way deviates from norms which we, as members of society, have learnt to expect in the medium used and that anyone who wishes to investigate the significance and value of a work of art must concentrate on the element of interest and surprise, rather than on the automatic pattern. Such deviations from linguistic or other socially accepted norms are labeled foregrounding, which invokes the analogy of a figure seen against a background (Leech,1968: 57). In stylistics, the notion of foregrounding, a term borrowed from the Prague School of Linguistics, is used by Leech and Short (1981: 48) to refer to artistically motivated deviation. The term foregrounding has its origin with the Czech theorist Jan Mukarovský: it is how Mukarovský's original term, aktualisace, was rendered in English by his first translator (Mukarovský, 1932/1964). It refers to the range of stylistic effects that occur in literature, whether at the phonetic level (e.g., alliteration, rhyme), the grammatical level (e.g., inversion, ellipsis), or the semantic level (e.g., metaphor, irony).

As Mukarovský pointed out, foregrounding may occur in normal, everyday language, such as spoken discourse or journalistic prose, but it occurs at random with no systematic design. In literary texts, on the other hand, foregrounding is structured: it tends to be both systematic and hierarchical. That is, similar features may recur, such as a pattern of assonance or a related group of metaphors, and one set of features will dominate the others (Mukarovský, 1964, p. 20), a phenomenon that Jakobson termed "the dominant" (1987, pp. 41-46). Foregrounding is the opposite of automatization, that is, the deautomatization of an act; the more an act is automatized, the less it is consciously executed; the more it is foregrounded, the more completely conscious does it become. Objectively speaking: automatization schematizes an event; foregrounding means the violation of the scheme. (1964, p. 19) The technique of art, including literature, is to make objects "unfamiliar," to make forms difficult, to increase the difficulty and length of perception because the process of perception is an aesthetic end in itself and must be prolonged. (1917/1965, p. 12) Miall and Kuiken refer foregrounding to stylistic variation that evokes feelings and prolong reading time

The immediate effect of foregrounding is to make strange (ostranenie), to achieve defamiliarization. Shklovsky saw defamiliarization as accompanied by feeling: he noted, more precisely, that stylistic devices in literary texts "emphasize the emotional effect of an expression" (Shklovsky, 1917/1965, p. 9). And, Mukarovský concurs, "When used poetically, words and groups of words evoke a greater richness of images and feelings than if they were to occur in a communicative utterance" (1977, p. 73). Verdonk states that foregrounding is the psychological effect a literary reader has as s/he is reading a work of literature. There is some evidence that foregrounding in literary texts strikes readers as interesting and captures their attention. Hunt and Vipond (1985) investigated the effects of textual features that they, following Labov (1972), refer to as "discourse evaluations." These are described as "words, phrases, or events" that are "unpredictable against the norm of the text" and that convey the narrator's evaluations of story characters or events. Since discourse evaluations resemble foregrounding as discussed in the present report, Hunt and Vipond's findings are noteworthy. In a study with readers of a short story, they found that readers were more likely to report that story phrases "struck them" or "caught their eye" when presented with the original discourse evaluations than when those phrases had been adapted so that the same story events were described in relatively "neutral" terms.

Examples: 1. Sillitoe s text Now you d think, and I d think, and everybody with a bit of imagination would think, that we d done as clean a job as could ever be done, that with the baker s shop being at least a mile from where we lived, and with not a soul having seen us, and what with the fog and the fact that we weren t more than five minutes in the place, that the coppers should never have been able to trace us. But then, you d be wrong, I d be wrong and everybody else would be wrong, no matter how much imagination was diced out between us.

1. Foregrounding in Sillitoe s text (α1) Now you d think, and I d think, and everybody with a bit of imagination would think, that we d done as clean a job as could ever be done, that (β1) with the baker s shop being at least a mile from where we lived, (β2) and with not a soul having seen us, (β3) and what with the fog and the fact that we weren t more than five minutes in the place, that the coppers should never have been able to trace us. (α2) But then, you d be wrong, (α3) I d be wrong (α4) and everybody else would be wrong, (β4) no matter how much imagination was diced out between us. 1.1. Foregrounded features and stylistic effect The impression the reader gets when reading this passage is that of a cleverly structured text which effectively recounts an experience. Its effectiveness and vividness are due in great measure to the following factors: a) The recursion of clauses, mainly through co-ordination, with similar or identical structure; b) The distribution of clauses in groups of threes, producing a striking pattern of parallelisms: Now you d think α1 and I d think and everybody would think that we d done as ever could ever be done that β1 with the baker s lived β2 and with not seen us

β3 and what with the fog and the fact that place that the coppers should never trace us But then α2 You d be wrong α3 I d be wrong α4 and everybody wrong β4 No matter between us Even the final arrangement is that of three blocks of clauses, realizing a symmetrical pattern where a contradiction is expressed between the two symmetrical elements (the clauses you d think, I d think, etc., and the clauses you d be wrong, I d be wrong, etc.). The contradiction is in a way made more prominent by the insertion of the group of subordinated clauses between them, having a stronger dramatic effect, which makes the reader try to anticipate what is going to be said. c) The third factor is lexical repetition in the co-ordinated units; the effect produced by the recursion on the syntactic level can be said to be reinforced by the repetition of the lexical items. This is particularly noticeable in the independent clauses ( You d think, I d think, etc., and You d be wrong, I d be wrong, etc.), though it also appears in the subordinated clauses. The text seems to present a contrast between features that characterize spoken, colloquial language on the one hand, and a complex structural pattern that is more typical of written, more formal varieties of language, on the other.

There are several features that characterize spoken versus written language: first, the use of co-ordination. Extensive use of co-ordination is usually said to be preferred in spoken language to subordination, since it simplifies the planning of sentence structure (Leech and Short, 1981: 163). In the second place, we can notice other features, such as the first person narrative reinforced by direct address to the reader, with expressions such as you d be wrong, you know, to tell you the truth, which are recurrent in the novel and which contribute to the conversational tone of the narration. Thirdly, there is the use of nonstandard forms of the language, though no instances in the particular extract I am analyzing. Throughout the novel we can find examples such as: the use of them for the demonstrative those ; use of was for were ; use of ungrammatical past participles, as in could have took, etc. These features seem to go well with the colloquial character of the passage, but are even more striking if we consider the parallel structures and the lexical repetitions that seem to be so carefully arranged in the text. Also, and in spite of the predominance of paratactic relations, the length of sentence 1 and the inclusion of several subordinate and embedded clauses make it difficult to imagine that the passage could have been realized in spoken form. Its rhetorical elaboration reminds us of a prepared speech; it is not for the vocabulary used and the content of the text. 1.2. Mind-style and the social context The apparently paradoxical character of the text can be better understood if the reader has some previous knowledge of the novel and its main character, who is also the narrator: a young boy who is sent to Borstal after the theft he has been narrating just before the passage I am analyzing. There is in the first page of the novel a sentence which I think is very significant for the understanding of the novel and this passage in particular:

cunning is what counts in this life, and even that you ve got to use in the slyest way you can (17). One thing he proves is that he actually is cunning, since, although hardly able to write, as he himself puts it (p. 13) he can handle language in a very effective way and successfully attract the reader s attention: it is the blending of the colloquial or conversational elements into a complex structure that makes the text so fascinating. In general terms, and applying Fowler s notion to the mind-style of the narrator, we can describe it as being the instrument through which a world view is given through the eyes of the narrator and main character from a social and personal perspective at the same time: it is the language and the world view of an uneducated young man, a potential criminal or outcast, who resorts to cunning as a means of protecting his integrity and individuality without giving in to external social pressures. 1.3. Analysis of Sillitoe s text This passage consists of two complex sentences: 1. Now you d think trace us. 2. But then between us. Sentence 1 is realized by an independent clause followed by three dependent clauses: the structure can be represented as follows: α1 β1 + β2 + β3 α1 is discontinuous, being interrupted by the three dependent clauses. Also, it presents a recursive SP pattern:

A: Now + SP: You d think + SP: (and) I d think + SP: (and) everybody would think Od: that we d done ever be done + Od: that the coppers trace us The dependent clauses can be said to have the following structure: β1- SPACs: with the baker s lived β2- SPOd: with not a soul us β3- S: what with the fog β3- S: and the fact that place β1 and β2 are non-finite ING clauses with expressed subject, while β3 is realized by verbless clause introduced by what with, co-ordinated with a finite that clause, also introduced by an ellipted what with. Sentence 2 is realized by three independent clauses followed by a dependent one. The structure can be represented as follows: where: α2 + α3 + α4 β4, α2 - SPC: you d be wrong α3 - SPC: I d be wrong α4 - SPC: and everybody wrong β4 - SPA: no matter us We can conclude saying that the text presents a predominance of coordinated units, four of them being of the asyndetic type and five of

them syndetic. The distribution of the clauses can be expressed in the following way: 1) Sentence 1: realised by one independent α clause dominating three co-ordinated subordinate β clauses. 2) Sentence 2: realised by three independent α clauses dominating a subordinate β clause.