Introduction. Introduction to Syntax, EGG 2011, Lecture 1. July 25th, Break up into groups of 3 or 4 just the people sitting near you.

Similar documents
A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Part I. Figuring out how English works

Constraining X-Bar: Theta Theory

CS 598 Natural Language Processing

Syntax Parsing 1. Grammars and parsing 2. Top-down and bottom-up parsing 3. Chart parsers 4. Bottom-up chart parsing 5. The Earley Algorithm

Proof Theory for Syntacticians

Informatics 2A: Language Complexity and the. Inf2A: Chomsky Hierarchy

An Introduction to the Minimalist Program

1/20 idea. We ll spend an extra hour on 1/21. based on assigned readings. so you ll be ready to discuss them in class

The Strong Minimalist Thesis and Bounded Optimality

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

PREP S SPEAKER LISTENER TECHNIQUE COACHING MANUAL

Construction Grammar. University of Jena.

Natural Language Processing. George Konidaris

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

Developing Grammar in Context

Compositional Semantics

Evidence-based Practice: A Workshop for Training Adult Basic Education, TANF and One Stop Practitioners and Program Administrators

a) analyse sentences, so you know what s going on and how to use that information to help you find the answer.

Derivational and Inflectional Morphemes in Pak-Pak Language

Introduction to HPSG. Introduction. Historical Overview. The HPSG architecture. Signature. Linguistic Objects. Descriptions.

CEFR Overall Illustrative English Proficiency Scales

Guidelines for Writing an Internship Report

Writing the Personal Statement

Some Principles of Automated Natural Language Information Extraction

Theoretical Syntax Winter Answers to practice problems

Parsing of part-of-speech tagged Assamese Texts

Context Free Grammars. Many slides from Michael Collins

A process by any other name

The College Board Redesigned SAT Grade 12

How to analyze visual narratives: A tutorial in Visual Narrative Grammar

Derivational: Inflectional: In a fit of rage the soldiers attacked them both that week, but lost the fight.

Language Acquisition Fall 2010/Winter Lexical Categories. Afra Alishahi, Heiner Drenhaus

CAS LX 522 Syntax I. Long-distance wh-movement. Long distance wh-movement. Islands. Islands. Locality. NP Sea. NP Sea

The presence of interpretable but ungrammatical sentences corresponds to mismatches between interpretive and productive parsing.

Linguistics. Undergraduate. Departmental Honors. Graduate. Faculty. Linguistics 1

Full text of O L O W Science As Inquiry conference. Science as Inquiry

How we look into complaints What happens when we investigate

IN THIS UNIT YOU LEARN HOW TO: SPEAKING 1 Work in pairs. Discuss the questions. 2 Work with a new partner. Discuss the questions.

Kelli Allen. Vicki Nieter. Jeanna Scheve. Foreword by Gregory J. Kaiser

Grammar Lesson Plan: Yes/No Questions with No Overt Auxiliary Verbs

NAME: East Carolina University PSYC Developmental Psychology Dr. Eppler & Dr. Ironsmith

AGENDA LEARNING THEORIES LEARNING THEORIES. Advanced Learning Theories 2/22/2016

The lab is designed to remind you how to work with scientific data (including dealing with uncertainty) and to review experimental design.

Why Pay Attention to Race?

Welcome to the Purdue OWL. Where do I begin? General Strategies. Personalizing Proofreading

A. What is research? B. Types of research

Basic Syntax. Doug Arnold We review some basic grammatical ideas and terminology, and look at some common constructions in English.

MASTER S THESIS GUIDE MASTER S PROGRAMME IN COMMUNICATION SCIENCE

Virtually Anywhere Episodes 1 and 2. Teacher s Notes

Loughton School s curriculum evening. 28 th February 2017

Grammars & Parsing, Part 1:

Inleiding Taalkunde. Docent: Paola Monachesi. Blok 4, 2001/ Syntax 2. 2 Phrases and constituent structure 2. 3 A minigrammar of Italian 3

Language acquisition: acquiring some aspects of syntax.

Author: Justyna Kowalczys Stowarzyszenie Angielski w Medycynie (PL) Feb 2015

ELA/ELD Standards Correlation Matrix for ELD Materials Grade 1 Reading

Ch VI- SENTENCE PATTERNS.

1 st Quarter (September, October, November) August/September Strand Topic Standard Notes Reading for Literature

CHAPTER IV RESEARCH FINDING AND DISCUSSION

Tutoring First-Year Writing Students at UNM

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

Aspectual Classes of Verb Phrases

Argument structure and theta roles

Dear Teacher: Welcome to Reading Rods! Reading Rods offer many outstanding features! Read on to discover how to put Reading Rods to work today!

Copyright Corwin 2015

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 154 ( 2014 )

Using computational modeling in language acquisition research

Multi-genre Writing Assignment

Chapter 4 - Fractions

Phenomena of gender attraction in Polish *

What s in Your Communication Toolbox? COMMUNICATION TOOLBOX. verse clinical scenarios to bolster clinical outcomes: 1

Control and Boundedness

HISTORY COURSE WORK GUIDE 1. LECTURES, TUTORIALS AND ASSESSMENT 2. GRADES/MARKS SCHEDULE

First Grade Curriculum Highlights: In alignment with the Common Core Standards

English Language and Applied Linguistics. Module Descriptions 2017/18

Build on students informal understanding of sharing and proportionality to develop initial fraction concepts.

The Foundations of Interpersonal Communication

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

Chapter 4: Valence & Agreement CSLI Publications

AQUA: An Ontology-Driven Question Answering System

Reading Grammar Section and Lesson Writing Chapter and Lesson Identify a purpose for reading W1-LO; W2- LO; W3- LO; W4- LO; W5-

Candidates must achieve a grade of at least C2 level in each examination in order to achieve the overall qualification at C2 Level.

Case study Norway case 1

What is PDE? Research Report. Paul Nichols

Case government vs Case agreement: modelling Modern Greek case attraction phenomena in LFG

A Pumpkin Grows. Written by Linda D. Bullock and illustrated by Debby Fisher

Critical Thinking in the Workplace. for City of Tallahassee Gabrielle K. Gabrielli, Ph.D.

Psychology and Language

LNGT0101 Introduction to Linguistics

BANGLA TO ENGLISH TEXT CONVERSION USING OPENNLP TOOLS

What is Thinking (Cognition)?

Age Effects on Syntactic Control in. Second Language Learning

Possessive have and (have) got in New Zealand English Heidi Quinn, University of Canterbury, New Zealand

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

Advanced Grammar in Use

Using a Native Language Reference Grammar as a Language Learning Tool

2014 Free Spirit Publishing. All rights reserved.

Introduction to CRC Cards

Number of students enrolled in the program in Fall, 2011: 20. Faculty member completing template: Molly Dugan (Date: 1/26/2012)

THE REFLECTIVE SUPERVISION TOOLKIT

Transcription:

Introduction Introduction to Syntax, EGG 2011, Lecture 1 July 25th, 2011 1 An exercise to start Let s remind ourselves of what syntax is all about. Break up into groups of 3 or 4 just the people sitting near you. Give a description of the structure of the sentences that follow in 1 below: How do the pieces fit together? Do some pieces belong more closely together than others? Which pieces are more central or important to the sentence and how it is put together? How do the various pieces function? Do not use any technical terms. Try to explain the structures in a way that a non-linguist would understand. Exercise: (1) a. Ellie ate all the bacon. b. Anna thinks that Teddy is a friendly dog. c. Cole seems to like everything. 2 What is Syntax? Subfields of linguistics are normally divided up according to the levels of structure they deal with: 1 Phonetics: the physical properties of the sounds used in language: how they are produced, perceived and transmitted 1 Of course there are also subfields of linguistics which deal with the relationship between language and external factors. Historical linguistics deals with how languages change over time, sociolinguistics with how languages vary in relation to social factors, computational linguistics with how human language can be dealt with using computers and computer-related techniques and ideas, neurolinguistics with how language is manifested in the brain, etc. 1

Phonology: how these sounds are organized and utilized in language systems Morphology: how words are put together out of smaller elements Syntax: how sentences are put together out of their constituent elements Semantics: how the sounds and structures of words and sentences are associated with their meanings So syntax is about how sentences are put together. Before we get into details, let s clarify some terminology: Utterance: a concrete instance of linguistic output, whether spoken, written, signed... Sentence: an abstraction over a series of (potential) utterances whose forms are identical as far as is linguistically significant Proposition: a message or chunk of meaning which is complete in the sense that we can in principle determine whether it is true or false in a particular context (2) John saw Stephan. (3) Jean a vu Stephan. (4) Stephan was seen by John. 2, 3 and 4 are three distinct utterances on the page. They are also three distinct sentences, but if spoken at the same time could express the same proposition. 2 (or 3 or 4) individually spoken at different times would represent distinct utterances, and could represent distinct propositions, but would always be the same sentence. The basic data syntacticians work with are the sentences and the non-sentences of a given language. (5) John saw Stephan. (6) * John Stephan saw. (7) # The car saw Stephan. If an utterance does not constitute a sentence of the language in question, we say it is ungrammatical and mark it with a *. If it does constitute a sentence syntactically, but corresponds to an odd or nonsensical proposition, we say it is deviant and mark it with a #. 2

Modern theoretical syntax has as its goal a complete and explicit account of sentence structures in all languages. So we have to deal with big issues like word order: (8) Her finner du publikasjoner. (Norwegian) (9) Here you find publications. (English) But we also need to worry about things that may seem too obvious to mention, e.g. the fact that you can t say 10 if John is male: (10) * John likes herself. And also stuff that is so weird and obscure that you may have never noticed it: (11) Who do you think Ciaran will ask first? (12) Who do you think that Ciaran will ask first? (13) Who do you think will ask Seamus first? (14) * Who do you think that will ask Seamus first? 3 The scientific method Modern theoretical syntax approaches its goals from a scientific perspective. Being scientific just means following the scientific method: 1. Gather some empirical observations X about a phenomenon. 2. Propose hypotheses about why we observed X (and not some other imaginable things Y). 3. Test these hypotheses on the basis of new observations Z. 4. Revise the hypotheses to account for Z. 5. Repeat steps 3 and 4 until all available data can be accounted for. This process generally does not terminate. Our understanding of the phenomenon gets better and better, but we can never be sure that our knowledge is complete. To see the scientific method at work in syntax, let s consider yes/no question formation in English. We ll start with the following data: 3

(15) Galahad can speak French. (16) Can Galahad speak French? These two sentences are clearly related: 16 questions the statement made in 15. In terms of form, the two sentences have exactly the same words, but they are in a slightly different order. This is probably not an accident, so let s formulate a hypothesis to account for how the sentences are related to each other: Hypothesis 1: In English, yes/no questions are like statements, but have the verb at the beginning of the sentence. That handles 15 and 16, so let s test it on some new data. (17) * Speak Galahad can English. Our hypothesis predicts that this should be a valid way to form a question from 15 we ve moved a verb to the beginning of the sentence. The problem here is clearly that we ve moved the wrong verb. So we need to modify our hypothesis to capture this restriction: 2 Hypothesis 2: In English, yes/no questions are like statements, but have the first verb of the statement at beginning of the sentence. Again, we re ready for some new data: (18) Annabel knows Galahad. (19) * Knows Annabel Galahad? (20) Does Annabel know Galahad? Hypothesis 2 predicts that the yes/no question formed from 18 should be 19, but 19 is not a sentence of English. The real yes/no question related to 18 is in 20, where the form does has appeared out of nowhere and gotten itself to the front of the sentence. The issue here is the identity of the verb, knows as opposed to can. questions with some other verbs: Consider 2 As we refine our hypotheses, I ll use boldface for things that change. 4

(21) a. Ferris has eaten Pizza. b. Has Ferris eaten Pizza? c. * Does Ferris have eaten Pizza? (22) a. Ferris will eat Pizza. b. Will Ferris eat Pizza? c. * Does Ferris will eat Pizza? (23) a. Ferris ate Pizza. b. * Ate Ferris Pizza? c. Did Ferris eat Pizza? (24) a. Ferris likes Pizza. b. * Likes Ferris Pizza? c. Does Ferris like Pizza? So-called auxiliary verbs like can, have and will can come at the front of questions, but normal verbs like know and eat can t. So again, we have to adjust our hypothesis: Hypothesis 3: In English, yes/no questions are like statements, but have the first auxiliary verb of the statement at the beginning of the sentence. If there is no auxiliary verb in the statement, a form of do appears as auxiliary at the beginning of the question. We re far from finished here, but what we ve got already works pretty well and gets the basic idea of English yes/no question formation down. 4 The problem of linguistic knowledge Something that sets linguistics apart from other sciences like physics and biology is the nature of the object of study Unlike stars and algae, languages aren t independent physical entities out in the world. A language is really a body of knowledge in the minds of its speakers. Specific utterances can have a concrete existence in the world in the form of sound waves or marks on paper. But we re really more interested in the sentences that underly utterances, and these are abstract mental objects. So what we really want to understand is not just the languages themselves, but the knowledge that people have of their languages. 5

But what kind of knowledge do speakers have?? When I say that I can speak English, what does that imply about what is in my mind?? And how did that knowledge get there in the first place? 5 Towards explanation Linguistic knowledge is crucial to the explanations we re after in theoretical syntax. An influential proposal from Noam Chomsky evaluates theories based on this insight: Observational adequacy: being able to simply identify which utterances correspond to grammatical sentences of a language I.e. accurately cataloguing what s out there in a concise and systematic fashion This takes a lot of work and forms the basis for everything else, but it doesn t necessarily reflect much deep insight. Descriptive adequacy: additionally determining the structures for those sentences and showing how they are systematically related to each other I.e. a solid analysis of the systems at work in a given language, understood completely in its own terms. This still doesn t get at language in general or the role of knowledge. Explanatory adequacy: additionally providing a framework which distinguishes possible languages from impossible ones and provides an explanation for how languages can be acquired I.e. setting each language in a universal, comparative context, showing what all languages have in common and how they can differ. An understanding of the knowledge underlying language and how it is acquired underpins the theory. All and only those languages which a child can acquire are the possible human languages. Now, the toy investigation of yes/no questions we started above is going to have difficulty achieving explanatory adequacy. 6

All of the hypotheses specifically mention English, because all of the data so far have come from English. So it is appropriate to reconsider our hypotheses in terms of data from other languages. How about some German sentences... (25) Reinhold kann Französisch. Reinhold can French Reinhold can speak French. (26) Kann Reinhold Französisch? can Reinhold French Can Reinhold speak French? So (at least some) other languages form yes/no questions in a similar way, by moving a verb to the beginning of the sentence (with important differences of detail). In fact, similar patterns of question formation are found in lots of languages. There are languages that do things differently, but the variation seems to be limited. The following are commonly attested ways of forming yes/no questions: Move a verb to the beginning of the sentence (English, German... ) Leave the word order the same as in statements, but add a special question-marker (Latin, Tamil). Change the morphology on the verb (Greendlandic). Use the same syntax as in a statement, but with a special question intonation (Modern Greek, colloquial French). But lots of other patterns we can imagine do not seem to be used in any language to form a yes/no question: Move one or more words to the end of the sentence Reverse the order of all the words in the sentence Change the morphology on the subject So our theory should explain why so many languages show the same 3 or 4 patterns, and why other patterns are impossible. 7

6 Generative Grammar and Universal Grammar The most influential approach to these issues is due to Noam Chomsky and his colleagues. Chomsky s idea is that the basic capacity for language is a human instinct, encoded in our genes. That is, we are born knowing something like a template for human languages, what Chomsky calls Universal Grammar (UG). When we learn a language as children, we only have to learn the details of how it fills in the UG template. So for Chomsky, the goal of linguistic theory is to identify the properties of UG, and to determine the specific settings that yield languages like English, German and Chinese. The theory of a language is a model of the knowledge of its speakers, a set of rules that can account for or generate all the sentences of that language and no others. Hence the Chomskyan enterprise as a whole is often called Generative Grammar. Current versions of Chomskyan Generative Grammar instantiate the approach known as the Minimalist Program or simply Minimalism, which we will also pursue in this course. 7 Formalism and formal properties of language Generative Grammar has an additional very important property: it is a formal theory. This means that rules and principles of the theory will be formulated in concise and precise terms, using a fair amount of mathematical notation. 3 E.g. rather than a prose description like 27, we might have a formal phrasestructure rule like 28: 3 The word formal has (at least) two distinct senses when used to describe theories, in particular linguistic theories, which often leads to confusion. In addition to the sense described in the main text, formal can refer to a theory which manipulates symbols purely in terms of their form as defined within the theory, with no sensitivity to the meanings those symbols might have (and which by extension pursues the thesis that this is a productive way to approach its subject matter). Generative Grammar is also formal in this sense, but it is not always the case that a theory which is formal one sense is also formal in the other. 8

(27) A sentence consists of a noun phrase followed by a verb phrase. (28) S NP VP There are a number of good reasons for this: If written properly, formal rules are clear, concise and unambiguous. This makes them easier to test and potentially disconfirm, and it makes it easier to see what sort of unexpected consequences they might have. Consider again English yes/no questions and our attempts thus far to explain them: Our hypotheses so far have been anything but formal, involving simple prose descriptions without any explicit definitions of the terms used. E.g., we haven t been clear about what an auxiliary verb is. Given this, it s not actually clear what our hypothesis has to say about sentences like 30: (29) Galahad wants to speak French. (30) * Wants Galahad to speak French? We also haven t clarified what the first auxiliary verb in a sentence means. A proper formal understanding of the issue is necessary to account for data like these: (31) The pie which is made with molasses is the most delicious. (32) * Is the pie which made with molasses is the most delicious? (33) Is the pie which is made with molasses the most delicious? But it s not just clarity that makes a formal theory of language advantageous. Language has certain properties which are very naturally treated in formal terms, and would be rather cumbersome to deal with otherwise. E.g., language exhibits clear evidence for hierarchical structure: (34) The pirate smacked the sailor with the treasure map. Note that this sentence can mean two different things: 1. The pirate took the treasure map, rolled it up and used it to smack the sailor. 2. There was a sailor holding the treasure map, and the pirate smacked him (using the back of his hand or a bottle of rum or a dead parrot). 9

The best explanation for this ambiguity is that the two different meanings have structures which differ in terms of how with the treasure map fits in with everything else: 1. The PP is part of the VP. The treasure map was the instrument of the smacking. (35) TP NP VP the pirate V NP PP smacked the sailor P NP with the treasure-map 2. The PP is part of the NP. The sailor who got smacked had the treasure map. (36) TP NP VP the pirate V NP smacked the sailor PP P with NP the treasure-map Language also exhibits rampant recursion, which is when something contains a copy of itself. Here s an example of visual recursion: 10

We have here a box of cocoa powder, ca. 1903, with a picture of a woman holding a tray with a mug of cocoa. On the tray, next to the mug, is a box of cocoa, which contains a smaller version of the same picture: a woman carrying a tray of cocoa. On that tray is an even smaller box of cocoa, and so forth. Each picture contains a smaller copy of itself. So how does recursion work in language? Consider the following examples: (37) The woman met the man. (38) The woman met the man [ from the store ]. (39) The woman met the man [ from the store [ on the shortest street [ in the center [ of the city [ with the spectacular cathedral [ next to the lagoon ] ] ] ] ] ]. 11

What s going on here? English sentences can be formed from a noun phrase followed by a verb followed by another noun phrase, e.g. 37 or, with different words, The cat saw the dog or The Flyers win the Stanley Cup. But noun phrases can be more complex, e.g. including a prepositional phrase like from the store. Note, then, that a prepositional phrase actually consists of a preposition plus a noun phrase. As in 39, the noun phrase could itself contain another prepositional phrase... Crucially, we can capture this complicated pattern with two extremely simple formal rules: (40) NP D N PP (41) PP P NP Describing this in terms of prose is actually much more difficult, takes up more space on the page, and is more likely to lead to misunderstanding. 8 A caveat on developing theories In this course we ll be primarily dealing with theories, and syntactic theory is very much a work in progress. Even the most advanced theories out there in linguistics and in other sciences as well are incomplete, and on some points certainly wrong. It is always possible to find data from some language or another that don t fit with the a given theory and force us to revise it. But this isn t bad. It s totally normal, and in fact it s the way that we make progress. The approach that we take here will basically follow what we just did with questions. We ll start with a very simple, basic set of hypotheses about how sentences are put together. Every day we ll consider new kinds of data that don t fit, and revise our hypotheses to account for them. 12

In this way, we ll work our way up towards something like the theories that specialists in the field are working on. So be prepared: At every step, you ll probably be able to come up with problems for the theory we ve developed to that point. When you do, think about how we could modify the theory to handle it. And bring it up in class. Of course, our analysis of particular data will evolve over the next two weeks. This will take some time to get used to, but it s the only way to really understand how syntax and science in general are done. 13