Updating the analysis of Japanese compound accent

Similar documents
LING 329 : MORPHOLOGY

The Odd-Parity Parsing Problem 1 Brett Hyde Washington University May 2008

Parallel Evaluation in Stratal OT * Adam Baker University of Arizona

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Linguistics 220 Phonology: distributions and the concept of the phoneme. John Alderete, Simon Fraser University

Revisiting the role of prosody in early language acquisition. Megha Sundara UCLA Phonetics Lab

Acoustic correlates of stress and their use in diagnosing syllable fusion in Tongan. James White & Marc Garellek UCLA

Phonological encoding in speech production

Som and Optimality Theory

Phonological Processing for Urdu Text to Speech System

Pobrane z czasopisma New Horizons in English Studies Data: 18/11/ :52:20. New Horizons in English Studies 1/2016

Rhythmic Licensing Theory: An extended typology

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

Parsing of part-of-speech tagged Assamese Texts

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

Precedence Constraints and Opacity

The optimal placement of up and ab A comparison 1

The Prosodic (Re)organization of Determiners

**Note: this is slightly different from the original (mainly in format). I would be happy to send you a hard copy.**

English Language and Applied Linguistics. Module Descriptions 2017/18

Towards a Robuster Interpretive Parsing

Multiple case assignment and the English pseudo-passive *

The Strong Minimalist Thesis and Bounded Optimality

Ternary rhythm in alignment theory René Kager Utrecht University

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

An Introduction to the Minimalist Program

Constraining X-Bar: Theta Theory

The presence of interpretable but ungrammatical sentences corresponds to mismatches between interpretive and productive parsing.

Listener-oriented phonology

Interfacing Phonology with LFG

Abstractions and the Brain

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

Optimality Theory and the Minimalist Program

Derivational and Inflectional Morphemes in Pak-Pak Language

18 The syntax phonology interface

Ph.D. Linguistics, University of Arizona. Dissertation: Confluence in phonology: evidence from Micronesian reduplication Director: Diana Archangeli

The influence of metrical constraints on direct imitation across French varieties

Enclitic-triggered stress shift in Catalan

Syntax Parsing 1. Grammars and parsing 2. Top-down and bottom-up parsing 3. Chart parsers 4. Bottom-up chart parsing 5. The Earley Algorithm

Lexical phonology. Marc van Oostendorp. December 6, Until now, we have presented phonological theory as if it is a monolithic

Underlying and Surface Grammatical Relations in Greek consider

Florida Reading Endorsement Alignment Matrix Competency 1

Concept Acquisition Without Representation William Dylan Sabo

An argument from speech pathology

Rhythm-typology revisited.

Approaches to control phenomena handout Obligatory control and morphological case: Icelandic and Basque

Joan Bybee, Phonology and Language Use. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2001,

Introduction to HPSG. Introduction. Historical Overview. The HPSG architecture. Signature. Linguistic Objects. Descriptions.

Word Stress and Intonation: Introduction

Lexical specification of tone in North Germanic

Progressive Aspect in Nigerian English

Case government vs Case agreement: modelling Modern Greek case attraction phenomena in LFG

AN EXPERIMENTAL APPROACH TO NEW AND OLD INFORMATION IN TURKISH LOCATIVES AND EXISTENTIALS

have to be modeled) or isolated words. Output of the system is a grapheme-tophoneme conversion system which takes as its input the spelling of words,

CS 598 Natural Language Processing

Frequency and pragmatically unmarked word order *

Norwegian stress and quantity: The implications of loanwords

1/20 idea. We ll spend an extra hour on 1/21. based on assigned readings. so you ll be ready to discuss them in class

How to analyze visual narratives: A tutorial in Visual Narrative Grammar

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

To appear in The TESOL encyclopedia of ELT (Wiley-Blackwell) 1 RECASTING. Kazuya Saito. Birkbeck, University of London

Writing the Personal Statement

The Effect of Discourse Markers on the Speaking Production of EFL Students. Iman Moradimanesh

SOME MINIMAL NOTES ON MINIMALISM *

Manner assimilation in Uyghur

An Evaluation of the Interactive-Activation Model Using Masked Partial-Word Priming. Jason R. Perry. University of Western Ontario. Stephen J.

Project in the framework of the AIM-WEST project Annotation of MWEs for translation

arxiv: v1 [cs.cl] 2 Apr 2017

UC Santa Cruz Graduate Research Symposium 2016

On the final vowel in Kikae

Rachel E. Baker, Ann R. Bradlow. Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA

Markedness and Complex Stops: Evidence from Simplification Processes 1. Nick Danis Rutgers University

Basic concepts: words and morphemes. LING 481 Winter 2011

Japanese mimetic palatalization revisited: implications for conflicting directionality *

An Interactive Intelligent Language Tutor Over The Internet

Dissertation Summaries. Headedness in Word Formation and Lexical Semantics: Evidence from Italiot and Cypriot (University of Patras, 2014)*

Underlying Representations

Corpus Linguistics (L615)

Classification. Universals

The phonological grammar is probabilistic: New evidence pitting abstract representation against analogy

Demonstration of problems of lexical stress on the pronunciation Turkish English teachers and teacher trainees by computer

Routledge Library Editions: The English Language: Pronouns And Word Order In Old English: With Particular Reference To The Indefinite Pronoun Man

Sounds of Infant-Directed Vocabulary: Learned from Infants Speech or Part of Linguistic Knowledge?

A simpler view of Danish stød

Noun incorporation in Sora: A case for incorporation as morphological merger TLS: 19 February Introduction.

Focusing bound pronouns

A Cross-language Corpus for Studying the Phonetics and Phonology of Prominence

Applications of memory-based natural language processing

Morphotactics as Tier-Based Strictly Local Dependencies

Control and Boundedness

Learning Structural Correspondences Across Different Linguistic Domains with Synchronous Neural Language Models

Executive Guide to Simulation for Health

The College Board Redesigned SAT Grade 12

I propose an analysis of thorny patterns of reduplication in the unrelated languages Saisiyat

ELA/ELD Standards Correlation Matrix for ELD Materials Grade 1 Reading

A cautionary note is research still caught up in an implementer approach to the teacher?

HIGH SCHOOL COURSE DESCRIPTION HANDBOOK

Phenomena of gender attraction in Polish *

Transcription:

In Short 'schrift for Alan Prince, compiled by Eric Baković. http://princeshortschrist.wordpress.com/squibs/alderete/ Updating the analysis of Japanese compound accent John Alderete, Simon Fraser University; alderete@sfu.ca Investigating the predictions of a theory requires both a strong purchase of its consequences and the rigorous pursuit of these consequences in the domains pertaining to them (Prince 2007). In this squib, we attempt to emulate the rigorous pursuit of theory so characteristic of Alan Prince s work by investigating some of the consequences of a recent approach to Japanese pitch accent proposed in Itô & Mester 2015/to appear. In this approach, pitch accent is a property of the head of a moraic trochee that follows a Latinlike pattern, where, roughly speaking, accent falls on the syllable containing the antepenultimate mora. For example, pitch accent falls on the antepenultimate light syllable in loanwords like barusérona Barcelona (2b). Words like amerika are unaccented in this analysis (1b) because it is impossible to produce the antepenultimate pattern without violating other high-ranking constraints in the language. The patterns predicted by this approach overlap with many of the patterns found in noun-noun compounds, supporting the conjecture that the analysis of the two patterns are unified in some sense (see Kubozono 1988/1993: 17 for a particularly clear statement of this). However, certain recalcitrant facts prevent a direct carry-over of Itô & Mester s analysis to compounds. First, when the second member is of a compound is long (=three or more moras), accent regularly falls on the first syllable of the second member (1a), but this regular accent is not found in non-compound words like amerika (1b). Second, when the second member of a compound is short (1-2 moras), accent falls on the last syllable of the first member (2a). While on the surface (2a) resembles barusérona in having antepenultimate accent, for a variety of reasons, prior research assumes that accent falls on the head of a penultimate foot in this case, and that this foot is iambic. These assumptions require foot flipping and do not accord with Itô & Mester s close to final moraic trochee analysis. Compound noun Non-compound words (1) (L L)(LL) (L L)(L L) Accentedness problem a. minami-(áme)(rika) b. (ame)(rika) (2) L(LL )-(LL) (L L)(L L) L Foot-flipping problem a. ni(sikí)-(hebi) b. (baru)(séro)na The two problems above seem to suggest that compound and non-compound nouns have certain irreconcilable differences in terms of the existence and placement of feet, and that the possibility of a single internally consistent analysis is limited. I will argue instead that a unified analysis is possible that both maintains a formal commitment to Itô & Mester s approach to antepenultimacy, and realizes Kubozono s contention that the two systems are in fact related by the antepenultimacy generalization. Let us review the core facts of noun-noun compounds in more detail before delving into analysis. These facts, and many analytical assumptions for organizing them, are guided by prior research, including Kubozono 1988/1993, Poser 1990, and the recent OT analyses of Kubozono 1995, 1997, Alderete 2001, Tanaka 2001, and Itô & Mester 2007. The data below illustrate the three basic patterns in compounds with short second members (N2 s henceforth). While lexically specified accent (shown with underlining) can bring about surface accent on a heavy or a non-final light (3b), the default pattern in these forms is for accent on the last syllable of the first conjunct (3a), a pattern that can even trump lexical accent, e.g., huransú-pan. Each of these syllabic profiles can also surface unaccented (3c), and 1

this is indeed very common in some forms. For example, approximately 70% of words with LL-LL profiles are unaccented. Tanaka (2001) states that Japanese appears to be moving away from [ σ-l L] patterns, towards [ σ -LL], supporting the default pattern as a core pattern in the system. Also, (3b) may be more characteristic of foreign words, and some nativized loans have shifted to the (3a) pattern, which again supports regularization to the default. (3) Compound nouns with short N2 (1 or 2 µ) a. Default final in N1 b. Lexical µ in N2 c. Unaccented compound σ -L nagoyá-si Nagoya City σ -L L* nisikí-hebi reticulating python σ -L L kabutó-musi beetle σ -H huransú-pan french bread ( σ-l rare) σ-l L niwaka-áme shower σ-h sunakku-báa snack bar σ-l nihon-go Japanese language σ-ll kinu-ito silk thread µ µ -LL mizu-bana running nose σ-h efferu-tou The Eiffel Tower In compound nouns with long N2 s, we again have the emergence of non-final lexical accent (4b) and a default pattern (4a). However, in this context, default accent falls on the first syllable of the second conjunct. Support for the default comes in two forms: (i) it surfaces when the input nouns have no accent, e.g., minami-ámerika, and (ii) the fact that some forms alternate between lexical antepenultimate accent and the default, e.g., neri-hámigaki ~ neri-hamígaki. (4) Compound nouns with long N2 (3+µ) a. Default initial in N2 b. Non-initial lexical accent σ-l LL onna-gókoro women s heart σ-l LLL neri-hámigaki toothpaste In general, non-final lexical accent can cause non-initial accent in N2 σ-ll LL neri-hamígaki σ-l LLL minami-ámerika South America Any analysis of accent in compound nouns will have to reckon with (i) the emergence of lexical prosody, (ii) unaccented compounds with short N2s, and (iii) the two default patterns. I follow prior research (Kubozono 1995, 1997, Alderete 2001, Tanaka 2001) in assuming that the constraints that predict the defaults interact with faithfulness constraints, allowing lexical prosody to surface in non-final syllables. It seems that some measure of lexical specification is necessary to account for both realization of lexical prosody and its suppression by the default, and presumably faithfulness constraints can be coindexed with distinct correspondence relations in a way that accounts for both classes (Itô & Mester 1999). I will also not analyze unaccented words here, but just note that many of the ideas put forth in Itô 2

& Mester (2015/to appear) seem to straightforwardly extend to some of these cases. Several of these words are tri- and quadramoraic, which mirrors these patterns in non-compound words. I focus instead on the default patterns and the problems they raise mentioned above. The accentedness problem is that compounds have regular accent, but some words that are accented in compounds are unaccented in non-compound words, e.g., minami-ámerika, cf. amerika. A key insight into this problem comes from the idea that compounds are headed structures, and that headed constituents have special properties (Alderete 2001, Itô & Mester 2007). In the typology of compounds developed in Itô & Mester (2007), based on many original insights in Kubozono (1988/1993), the nounnoun compounds under discussion here are word compounds that involve a recursive prosodic word. In Alderete (2001), these recursive prosodic words are right-headed, as shown below. Given these assumptions, the difference between unaccented amerika and minami-ámerika can be explained by assuming the head PrWd of the larger compound must have a peak prominence. Amerika does not have a head PrWd, so this constraints is vacuously satisfied in non-compounds. (5) Accentedness to the head PrWd PrWd / \ Ft Ft (ame)(rika) PrWd / \ PrWd PrWdHead / / \ Ft Ft Ft (mina)mi (áme)(rika) We may formalize this intuition with the constraint PRWDHEADACCENT, cf. Itô & Mester s MINWORDACCENT, which is similar in spirit. Inserting this constraint at the top of the hierarchy resolves the inconsistency between these two classes of examples (7). (6) PRWDHEADACCENT (PRWDHEADACC) The head PrWd of a prosodic word compound must have a peak prominence. (7) Inconsistency resolved by identifying PrWd Head Winner ~ Loser PRWDHDACCT RIGHTMOST WORDACCENT a. minami-(áme)(rika) ~ minami-(ame)(rika) W L W b. (ame)(rika) ~ (áme)(rika) W L I assume that the relevant constraint requiring accentedness in long N2 s is prosodic in nature because, while we focus on right-headed compound nouns morpho-syntactically, Sino-Japanese compounds behave in similar respects to these compounds, but they are left-headed (Kageyama 1982). It seems self-evident that compounds should have heads and that they should be demarcated somehow through prominence. In a sense, therefore, a constraint like PRWDHEADACCENT is the null hypothesis for why compounds have regular accent on long second conjuncts. Interestingly, this same constraint paves the way to solving the foot-flipping problem, as shown below. The normal way of satisfying PRWDHEADACCENT would be to accent the final LL, as in candidate (9a) (illustrated in (8)), but this puts accent on a final foot, which is outlawed by Itô & Mester s NONFINAL(FT ). Another sensible thing to do would be to accent the penultimate foot, as with candidate (9c), but this does not satisfy PRWDHDACCENT. Candidate (9b) can thus be viewed as the optimal way of satisfying this constraint. It is not perfect, because the head foot straddles the morpheme boundary (violating MATCH(STEM, PRWD) of Alderete and MacMillan (2015)), but it is otherwise perfect prosodically. 3

Finally, since mismatches are allowed, I note that stretching the PrWd Head two syllables inward, as in (9d), is not okay for the same reason it is not okay elsewhere: this leads to a violation of RIGHTMOST. (8) Attempts at satisfying PRWDHDACC in short N2 s Candidate (9a) Candidate (9c) Candidate (b) (9) Antepenultimacy in compounds with short N2 (PrWd boundaries shown with [ ]) /kabuto + musi/ PRWDHDACCT RIGHTMOST NONFINAL(FT ) MATCH(STEM, PRWD) a. [ka(buto)]-[(músi)] *! b. à [(kabu)][(tó-mu)si] * c. [ka(búto)]-[(musi)] *! d. [ka][(búto)-(musi)] *! * In this analysis, PRWDHEADACCENT works in concert with all of the constraints involved in predicting the antepenultimate pattern in Itô & Mester s system, even their INITIALFOOT (as demonstrated in the associated OTWorkPlace files available from the author s webpage). There is thus strong theoretical motivation for a footing like that given in (9b). But what other evidence might support (9b), which is, after all, a little odd given its misalignment of prosodic and morpho-syntactic structure? Further evidence may come from another well-known morpho-phonological process in compounds, rendaku (Itô & Mester 1986; McCawley 1968; Vance 1987). Recent work in Rosen (2001, 2003) has examined how rendaku, the voicing of initial obstruents in compounds, can be blocked in semi-regular ways. In particular, Rosen documents a loose correlation between irregular accent in compounds with short N2s and a higher incidence of rendaku blocking. While there are no absolute regularities between accent pattern and rendaku blocking, Rosen s data support an interesting conjecture about voicing and footing. Suppose the foot straddling the two conjuncts in (9b) creates a kind of phonological cohesion that sets this structure apart from other compounds. In such a context, we might expect words with greater cohesion to be more word-like, and thus subject more often to idiosyncractic exceptions, like the resistance to rendaku. To check this, I have examined the raw data from Rosen s study (thanks to Eric Rosen for sharing this on short notice) and found an interesting trend in LL-LL and LL-L compounds (10). Compounds with straddled feet, including all LL-L cases and LL -LL compounds with antepenultimate accent, seem to have a greater propensity for rendaku blocking than unaccented LL-LL words, which must be parsed as (LL)-(LL) under Itô & Mester s analysis. These facts also fit with a pattern of allomorphy in island name constructions in which pre-accented stems (which have straddling feet) correlate with rendaku blocking, e.g., (itu)(kú-si)ma, but unaccented words do not, as in (saku)ra-(zima) (Tanaka 2005). 4

(10) Rendaku blocking in LL-LL and LL-L compounds Rendaku Rendaku blocked Totals L(L-L), L(L -L)L 146 98 (40%) 244 (LL)-(LL) 297 148 (33%) 445 While the trend in (10) is not statistically significant, together with the allomorphy pattern, it lends support to the foot straddling analysis, and in turn the larger goal here of unifying the analysis of compound and non-compound words. Furthermore, the analysis proposed here really involves a small modification of Kubozono and Tanaka s original OT analyses. In essence, it motivates a role for PrWd heads in requiring compounds to have a peak prominence, even if this leads to a slight mismatch between the prosody and morpho-syntax (9b). This single assumption has a big impact, which can be appreciated by comparing the analyses in (7) and (9) with the foot-flipping approach in (1-2). The latter approach essentially involves a hybrid system of accenting the penultimate foot: the penultimate foot is a moraic trochee in words like minami-(áme)(rika), but an iamb in ka(butó)-(musi). This system is in two ways distinct from non-compound accent: it allows iambs in a system that is otherwise trochaic, and it assigns accent to the penultimate foot, not the final foot. Both approaches have odd foot parses, but the approach advocated here unifies both compound and non-compound systems with foot straddling. The iambic analysis in words like ka(butó)-(musi) accounts for the antepenultimate pattern, but in a way rather distinct from non-compound accent. References Alderete, John. 2001. Morphologically governed accent in Optimality Theory New York: Routledge. Alderete, John & Kayleigh MacMillan. 2015. Reduplication in Hawaiian: Variations on a theme of minimal word. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 33.1-45. Itô, Junko & Armin Mester. 1986. The phonology of voicing in Japanese: Theoretical consequences for morphological accessibility. Linguistic Inquiry 17.49-73.. 1999. The structure of the phonological lexicon. The handbook of Japanese linguistics, ed. by N. Tsujimura, 62-100. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishers.. 2007. Prosodic adjunction and Japanese compounds. MIT Working Papers in Linguistics. Formal Approaches to Japanese Linguistics 4, 97-111.. 2015/to appear. Unaccentedness in Japanese. Linguistic Inquiry. Kageyama, Taro. 1982. Word formation in Japanese. Lingua 57.215-58. Kubozono, Haruo. 1988/1993. The Organization of Japanese Prosody: Kurosio Publishers [revised 1988 doctoral dissertation from the University of Edinburgh].. 1995. Constraint interaction in Japanese phonology: Evidence from compound accent. Phonology at Santa Cruz [PASC], ed. by R. Walker, O. Lorentz & H. Kubozono, 21-38. Santa Cruz: Linguistics Research Center, UC Santa Cruz.. 1997. Lexical markedness and variation: A nonderivational account of Japanese compound accent. Proceedings from the West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics 15, ed. by B. Agbayani & S.- W. Tang, 273-87. McCawley, James D. 1968. The phonological component of a grammar of Japanese The Hague, Paris: Mouton. Poser, William J. 1990. Evidence for foot structure in Japanese. Language 66.78-105. Prince, Alan. 2007. The pursuit of theory. The Cambridge handbook of phonology, ed. by P. de Lacy, 33-60: Cambridge University Press. Rosen, Eric. 2001 Phonological processes interacting with the lexicon: Variable and non- regular effects in Japanese phonology: University of British Columbia.. 2003. Systematic irregularity in Japanese Rendaku: How the grammar mediates patterned lexical exceptions. Canadian Journal of Linguistics 48.1-37. Tanaka, Shin- ichi. 2001. The emergence of the 'unaccented': Possible patterns and variation in Japanese compound accentuation. Issues in Japanese phonology and morphology, ed. by J. van ed Weijer, 159-92. Dordrecht: Mouton de Gruyter.. 2005. Where voicing and accent meet: (In) completeness in the harmonic scale of phonological prominence. Voicing in Japanese, ed. by J. van de Weijer, K. Nanjo & Nishihara, 261-78. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Vance, Timothy. 1987. An Introduction to Japanese Phonology New York: SUNY Press. 5