Cooperative learning and decision-making in the classroom

Similar documents
Interactions often promote greater learning, as evidenced by the advantage of working

A Study of Metacognitive Awareness of Non-English Majors in L2 Listening

Online Groups and Social Loafing: Understanding Student-Group Interactions

CSC200: Lecture 4. Allan Borodin

University of Suffolk. Using group work for learning, teaching and assessment: a guide for staff

Entrepreneurial Discovery and the Demmert/Klein Experiment: Additional Evidence from Germany

Grade 6: Correlated to AGS Basic Math Skills

Guru: A Computer Tutor that Models Expert Human Tutors

GROUP COMPOSITION IN THE NAVIGATION SIMULATOR A PILOT STUDY Magnus Boström (Kalmar Maritime Academy, Sweden)

A Case Study: News Classification Based on Term Frequency

Pre-Algebra A. Syllabus. Course Overview. Course Goals. General Skills. Credit Value

Does the Difficulty of an Interruption Affect our Ability to Resume?

International Business BADM 455, Section 2 Spring 2008

Business 712 Managerial Negotiations Fall 2011 Course Outline. Human Resources and Management Area DeGroote School of Business McMaster University

What s in a Step? Toward General, Abstract Representations of Tutoring System Log Data

BENCHMARK TREND COMPARISON REPORT:

A Study of the Effectiveness of Using PER-Based Reforms in a Summer Setting

MGT/MGP/MGB 261: Investment Analysis

Guide to Teaching Computer Science

A Note on Structuring Employability Skills for Accounting Students

Management of time resources for learning through individual study in higher education

CONSISTENCY OF TRAINING AND THE LEARNING EXPERIENCE

ACTL5103 Stochastic Modelling For Actuaries. Course Outline Semester 2, 2014

Lahore University of Management Sciences. FINN 321 Econometrics Fall Semester 2017

Self Study Report Computer Science

When!Identifying!Contributors!is!Costly:!An! Experiment!on!Public!Goods!

Learning By Asking: How Children Ask Questions To Achieve Efficient Search

Effects of Classroom Relationships Between Students and Teachers on Emotional Development of Elementary School Students

PROJECT MANAGEMENT AND COMMUNICATION SKILLS DEVELOPMENT STUDENTS PERCEPTION ON THEIR LEARNING

Programme Specification. MSc in International Real Estate

HOW DO YOU IMPROVE YOUR CORPORATE LEARNING?

STUDENT PERCEPTION SURVEYS ACTIONABLE STUDENT FEEDBACK PROMOTING EXCELLENCE IN TEACHING AND LEARNING

Practice Examination IREB

The New Theory of Disuse Predicts Retrieval Enhanced Suggestibility (RES)

An Empirical Analysis of the Effects of Mexican American Studies Participation on Student Achievement within Tucson Unified School District

A cautionary note is research still caught up in an implementer approach to the teacher?

THE PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIVERSITY SCHREYER HONORS COLLEGE DEPARTMENT OF MATHEMATICS ASSESSING THE EFFECTIVENESS OF MULTIPLE CHOICE MATH TESTS

Rote rehearsal and spacing effects in the free recall of pure and mixed lists. By: Peter P.J.L. Verkoeijen and Peter F. Delaney

CHAPTER 4: REIMBURSEMENT STRATEGIES 24

MARKETING MANAGEMENT II: MARKETING STRATEGY (MKTG 613) Section 007

What is beautiful is useful visual appeal and expected information quality

DIANA: A computer-supported heterogeneous grouping system for teachers to conduct successful small learning groups

PUBLIC CASE REPORT Use of the GeoGebra software at upper secondary school

National Survey of Student Engagement (NSSE) Temple University 2016 Results

On-the-Fly Customization of Automated Essay Scoring

Ph.D. in Behavior Analysis Ph.d. i atferdsanalyse

A cognitive perspective on pair programming

Student Perceptions of Reflective Learning Activities

A Game-based Assessment of Children s Choices to Seek Feedback and to Revise

What is PDE? Research Report. Paul Nichols

School Competition and Efficiency with Publicly Funded Catholic Schools David Card, Martin D. Dooley, and A. Abigail Payne

The Good Judgment Project: A large scale test of different methods of combining expert predictions

State Budget Update February 2016

Purdue Data Summit Communication of Big Data Analytics. New SAT Predictive Validity Case Study

NATIONAL SURVEY OF STUDENT ENGAGEMENT

Age Effects on Syntactic Control in. Second Language Learning

The Evaluation of Students Perceptions of Distance Education

Understanding student engagement and transition

Evolution of Symbolisation in Chimpanzees and Neural Nets

Master Program: Strategic Management. Master s Thesis a roadmap to success. Innsbruck University School of Management

Strategic Planning for Retaining Women in Undergraduate Computing

ABET Criteria for Accrediting Computer Science Programs

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 209 ( 2015 )

Life and career planning

NATIONAL SURVEY OF STUDENT ENGAGEMENT (NSSE)

BSP !!! Trainer s Manual. Sheldon Loman, Ph.D. Portland State University. M. Kathleen Strickland-Cohen, Ph.D. University of Oregon

A GENERIC SPLIT PROCESS MODEL FOR ASSET MANAGEMENT DECISION-MAKING

Evidence-based Practice: A Workshop for Training Adult Basic Education, TANF and One Stop Practitioners and Program Administrators

National Survey of Student Engagement

Thesis-Proposal Outline/Template

IS FINANCIAL LITERACY IMPROVED BY PARTICIPATING IN A STOCK MARKET GAME?

VISUAL AND PERFORMING ARTS, MFA

A 3D SIMULATION GAME TO PRESENT CURTAIN WALL SYSTEMS IN ARCHITECTURAL EDUCATION

Unequal Opportunity in Environmental Education: Environmental Education Programs and Funding at Contra Costa Secondary Schools.

VOL. 3, NO. 5, May 2012 ISSN Journal of Emerging Trends in Computing and Information Sciences CIS Journal. All rights reserved.

Inside the mind of a learner

Multidisciplinary Engineering Systems 2 nd and 3rd Year College-Wide Courses

Meriam Library LibQUAL+ Executive Summary

National Survey of Student Engagement Spring University of Kansas. Executive Summary

HARPER ADAMS UNIVERSITY Programme Specification

Greek Teachers Attitudes toward the Inclusion of Students with Special Educational Needs

Success Factors for Creativity Workshops in RE

5.7 Course Descriptions

Presentation 4 23 May 2017 Erasmus+ LOAF Project, Vilnius, Lithuania Dr Declan Kennedy, Department of Education, University College Cork, Ireland.

SASKATCHEWAN MINISTRY OF ADVANCED EDUCATION

Segmentation Study of Tulsa Area Higher Education Needs Ages 36+ March Prepared for: Conducted by:

Implementing Response to Intervention (RTI) National Center on Response to Intervention

Two heads can be better than one

Match or Mismatch Between Learning Styles of Prep-Class EFL Students and EFL Teachers

TEACHER'S TRAINING IN A STATISTICS TEACHING EXPERIMENT 1

Principal vacancies and appointments

AGENDA LEARNING THEORIES LEARNING THEORIES. Advanced Learning Theories 2/22/2016

College Pricing. Ben Johnson. April 30, Abstract. Colleges in the United States price discriminate based on student characteristics

AGS THE GREAT REVIEW GAME FOR PRE-ALGEBRA (CD) CORRELATED TO CALIFORNIA CONTENT STANDARDS

OCR for Arabic using SIFT Descriptors With Online Failure Prediction

INTRODUCTION TO DECISION ANALYSIS (Economics ) Prof. Klaus Nehring Spring Syllabus

Anglia Ruskin University Assessment Offences

THEORY OF PLANNED BEHAVIOR MODEL IN ELECTRONIC LEARNING: A PILOT STUDY

A. What is research? B. Types of research

Financial Education and the Credit Behavior of Young Adults

Transcription:

ABSTRACT in the classroom John Hathorn Metropolitan State College of Denver Lesley G Hathorn Metropolitan State College of Denver Business professionals recognize that teamwork is an essential skill and that there are numerous situations that require cooperation in achieving common goals. In order to prepare future professionals, educators implement strategies of cooperative learning to hone skills required for teamwork. However, individuals in groups do not automatically work harder, produce better products, nor learn more than those working alone (Gillies & Boyle, 2010; Kaye, 1992; Sharan, 2010). Group members often show less motivation and application than those working alone, and satisfaction with the group and effort may be inversely correlated. Students who worked hardest may be the least satisfied (Hathorn, 2000). Evidence of this dissatisfaction can be seen in the low rates of students who strongly prefer cooperative over individual learning (Lindquist, 1995). In the present study, 136 students participated in an experiment in which group problem solving and cooperative learning were examined. The results show that students did benefit from working in cooperative groups in that their investment decisions were significantly different from that of a naïve investor. However, students did not consistently reach the same decision as the group. The low effort students based their decisions on the group outcome, indicating social loafing whilst equal effort students were furthest from the group decision indicating a lack of support for the group findings. Key words: Cooperative learning, decision-making, classroom, teamwork

INTRODUCTION The ability to work in teams is demonstrated in sports, at work, in classrooms, and on The Apprentice. Educators in many disciplines, including those in accounting, have implemented the instructional strategies of cooperative learning and group problem-solving in the belief that these strategies lead to both improved learning and also the development of skills required to work in teams. However, these are two separate issues. The purpose of the present study was to evaluate the effectiveness of a group project, as well as the amount of learning that individuals experienced in the cooperative learning task. LITERATURE REVIEW Research on the effectiveness of group work has been mixed. Groups may work more efficiently at obtaining required information and exhibit more on-task behavior than individuals (Cavalier & Klein, 1998). Also, group members achieve higher grades than students working individually (Brush, 1997). Despite these general findings, however, research indicates that collaborative group work is not equally effective in all situations. Individuals in groups often show less motivation and apply less effort (i.e., engage in more social loafing or free riding) than those working alone (Delucchi, 2007; Karau & Williams, 1993; Maiden & Perry, 2011). Not surprisingly, social loafing is more common in large groups rather than small groups and the effectiveness of the group decreases as the size increases (Lu, Yuan, & McLeod, 2012). In addition, low achievers do not work well in collaborative settings with high achievers (Dillenbourg, Baker, Blaye, & O Malley, 1996). It is a robust finding that brainstorming in groups is ineffective. Groups do not produce as many unique ideas as individuals, and people in groups tend to discuss common information rather than produce new information (for a complete discussion, please see Lu et al., 2012). Consequently, many students view group meetings as a waste of time (Kayes, Kayes, & Kolb, 2005). Collaborative learning may lead to higher academic achievement, improved relationships, and better social skills (Johnson, Johnson, & Smith, 1998). However, learning is not equally effective in all situations (Gillies & Boyle, 2010; Kaye, 1992; Sharan, 2010). One reason for apparently contradictory results in research findings may be that average scores are measured across groups without regard to individual performance. When individuals in groups are assigned a project and individual grades are based on the project grade, individual learning is not being assessed. For example, cooperative learning does not lead to higher exam scores for individuals (Delucchi, 2007). Research in accounting education has shown similar mixed results for cooperative learning. As all students who join study groups know, individuals in study groups learn material better than groups who only listen to the instructor (Hwang, Lui, & Tong, 2005). However, incentives to improve individual performance by combining individual and group assessment techniques are effective only under some circumstances (Maiden & Perry, 2012; Ravenscroft, Buckless, McCombs, & Zuckerman, 1995). Clearly, in many groups, some individuals are benefiting more than others. Low achievers benefit from being in a group with hard working high achievers and raise their grades with little effort. Low ability students are tutored by high ability students and learn material and study strategies from them. Averages improve but not all participants in the group benefit to the

same degree. This can lead to resentment of group work (Hathorn & Ingram, 2002). Satisfaction with the group and effort may be inversely correlated. Those who enjoy working in a group are not necessarily the ones who did the most work. Those who worked the hardest may be the least satisfied (Hathorn, 2000). Student attitude to cooperative learning may depend on the method of assessment with free riders agreeing most strongly that they enjoy group work compared to high achievers who perceive grading based on group performance as unfair (Lindquist, 1995; Ravenscroft, Buckless, McCombs, & Zuckerman, 1997). Other studies, however, have indicated no significant difference in performance or attitudes of individuals versus groups (Cavalier & Klein, 1998; Lancaster & Strand, 2001). The present study examined both the product of the group and individual learning in a cooperative project in an accounting class. Students were randomly assigned into groups of three or more. The group project required students to provide a detailed analysis of a company and to assess the investment potential of that company. Individual grades were based on the group outcome weighted by the contribution of individuals (as assessed by peers in the group). Students then answered a surprise final examination question on the company in order to assess how much they had learned about the company. It was expected that students would demonstrate varying levels of expertise on the company in the final examination due to varying individual contributions to the project. It was also expected that high achieving students would identify the low achieving students since social loafing is often considered by high achieving students to be unfair. It was also hypothesized that groups of three would have less social loafing than larger groups (Lu et al., 2012). METHOD Participants The participants in this study were 136 accounting students from Metropolitan State College of Denver who were randomly allocated to 29 groups of between three and seven members. Each group was required to analyze a publicly traded company as part of their class grade. Materials and procedure The final phase of the project required the group to analyze and determine the investment potential of the company that they had selected. The specific requirement was: Based upon the information you've collected in the first three phases, assess the company's major strengths and weaknesses. You will likely want to address this analysis in terms of investment potential. Since only the final project was graded, it was necessary to obtain a measure of the participation by each group member. Consequently, as a measure of the contribution to the group, each member was asked to distribute a sum of one hundred dollars to the group members (other than themselves) based on the contribution of the individual members. The distribution meant that any member allocated less that $100 was viewed by others in the group as social loafing and conversely, all allocation above $100 went to those seen as high achievers. In order to assess individual learning students were given a surprise question in the final examination in which they were asked to distribute 50,000 dollars (in the proportion of their choosing) between the company that they had studied and a mutual fund. They were told that the

funds would be invested for a period of 10 years with no opportunity of withdrawing the funds. This question assessed how much students had learned about the long-term investment potential of the company. Students who engaged in social loafing and did not learn from the high achievers would be expected to invest as a naïve investor. There were 13 participants who did not complete this question and data from 124 participants was analyzed further. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION The mean of the investment amount that students allocated was compared to the expected investment amount that naïve investors would invest in the company. Naïve investors are individuals who allocate investments evenly between all the available options without regard to the potential future return and/or risk of the investment (e.g., see De Wit, 1998). This is known as the 1/n heuristic and there is ample evidence that individuals invest according to this naïve investment strategy (Benartzi & Thaler, 2001). Thus in our scenario a naïve investor would allocate 25,000 thousand dollars to the company and the mutual fund. Of the 29 groups analyzed, 26 endorsed their companies as having strong investment potential and three groups determined that an investment in their companies was too risky. In the latter situation reverse scoring was used to determine the amount of the investment to be made in the company. A one-sample t-test showed a significant difference between informed students (with a mean of $29,191) and naïve investors (with a mean of $25,000 based on the 1/n heuristic), t (122) = 3.69, p <.001. This result supports the hypothesis that there is some degree of cooperative learning taking place in the group setting. That is, individuals did endorse the group decision overall in that they suggested a significantly higher investment in the company than a naïve investor. For participants from the three groups who did not endorse an investment in their analyzed company, a separate one-sample t-test showed a significant difference between the participants mean of $17,115 and zero, t(12) = 2.05, p =.02. This indicates that, although cooperative learning occurred, the level of decision-making was not high. Given that the groups had determined that the company they evaluated was not a worthwhile investment, then it would be economically rational for the group members to invest close to the entire amount of $50,000 in the mutual fund and zero in the company. The implications of whether the amount of individual effort and group size affect the mean amount invested in the companies chosen by the groups were also examined. Individuals were further assigned to low, equal and high effort groups based on peer evaluations. Individuals were classified as low effort (n = 42) when peers allocated them less than 100 dollars, equal effort (n = 25) when peers allocated them exactly 100 dollars, and high effort (n = 56) when peers allocated them more than 100 dollars. Means of the amount invested in the companies were calculated for each effort category. A one-way ANOVA showed a significant difference between the three groups in the amount individuals were willing to invest in the companies F(2, 120) = 5.271, p =.006. Figure 1 shows that the mean investment $20,800 for the equal effort group was significantly lower than the mean investment for the other two effort groups (means of $30,810 and $31,723 for the low and high effort groups respectively). This indicates that individuals in the equal effort group were less supportive of the group conclusion. The low effort group outcome was indicative of social loafing in that individuals in this group appeared to follow their group s conclusion. The high effort group appeared to be

confident of the group conclusion which is consistent with the fact that individuals in this group invested a proportionately greater amount of effort in the project. Follow up analysis used a one-way ANOVA which showed no significant difference between the mean investment of the groups based on the number of students in each group, F(4, 118) = 1.445, p =.224. Figure 2 graphs the mean investment for each group size with the number of groups is shown in parentheses. An analysis of group size on the effort of the individual group members found that less than 10 percent of the participants from the groups of three members were classified as low effort. This is significantly lower than the number of participants who were classified as low effort in the other groups the percentages ranged from 28 to 40. These findings are consistent with the literature which has documented that the optimal group size is three members (Lu et al., 2012). There is evidence of cooperative learning occurring in the groups with some level of individual informed decision-making. The low effort students base individual decisions on the group decision, indicating social loafing while the equal effort students were furthest from the group decision indicating a lack of support for the group findings. Prior research on social loafing was supported since there was an increase in social loafing in groups larger than three members. REFERENCES Benartzi, S., & Thaler, R.H. (2001). Naive diversification strategies in defined contribution saving plans. The American Economic Review, 91(1), 79-98. Brush, T. A. (1997). The effects on student achievement and attitudes when using integrated learning systems with cooperative pairs. Educational Technology Research and Development, 45(1), 51-64. Cavalier, J.C., & Klein, J.D. (1998). Effects of cooperative versus individual learning and orienting activities during computer-based instruction. Educational Technology Research and Development, 46(1), 5-17. Delucchi, M. (2007). Assessing the impact of group projects on examination performance in social statistics. Teaching in Higher Education, 12(4), 447-460. De Wit, D.P.M. (1998). Naïve diversification. Financial Analysts Journal,54(4), 95-100. Dillenbourg, P., Baker, M., Blaye, A., & O Malley, C. (1996). The evolution of research on collaborative learning. In P. Reinman & H. Spada (Eds.), Learning in humans and machines: Towards an interdisciplinary learning science (pp. 189-211). New York: Pergamon. Gillies, R.M., & Boyle, M. (2010). Teachers reflections on cooperative learning: Issues in implementation. Teaching and Teacher Education, 26(4), 933-940. Hathorn, L.G. (2000). Cooperation and collaboration using computer-mediated communication in a distance education class. Unpublished master s thesis. Kent State University, Kent, Ohio. Hathorn, L. G. & Ingram, A. L. (2002). Cooperation and collaboration using computermediated communication. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 26(3), 325-247. Hwang, N. R., Lui, G., & Tong, M. Y. (2005). An empirical test of cooperative learning in a passive learning environment. Issues in Accounting Education, 20(2), 151-165. Johnson, D. W., Johnson, R. T., & Smith, K. A. (1998). Cooperative learning returns to college. Change, 30(4), 26-35.

Karau, S.J., & Williams, K.D. (1993). Social loafing: A meta-analytic review and theoretical integration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(4), 681-706. Kaye, A. (1992). Learning together apart. In A.R. Kaye (Ed.), Collaborative learning through computer conferencing (pp.117-136). Berlin: Springer-Verlag. Kayes, A.B., Kayes, C., & Kolb, D.A. (2005). Experiential learning in teams. Simulation and Gaming, 36, 330-354. Lancaster, K.A. S., & Strand, C.A. (2001). Using the team-learning model in a managerial accounting class: An experiment in cooperative learning. Issues in Accounting Education, 16(4), 549-567. Lindquist, T. M. (1995). Traditional versus contemporary goals and methods in accounting education: Bridging the gap with cooperative learning. Journal of Education for Business, 70(5), 278-284. Lu, L., Yuan, Y.C., & McLeod, P.L. (2012). Twenty-five years of hidden profiles in group decision making: A meta-analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 16(1), 54-75. Maiden, B., & Perry, B. (2011). Dealing with free-riders in assessed group work: Results from a study at a UK university. Assessment & Evaluation in Higher Education, 36(4), 451-464. Ravenscroft, S. P., Buckless, F.A., McCombs, G. B., & Zuckerman, G.J. (1995). Incentives in student team learning: An experiment in cooperative group learning. Issues in Accounting Education, 10(1), 97-109. Ravenscroft, S. P., Buckless, F.A., McCombs, G. B., & Zuckerman, G.J. (1997). Student team learning Replication and extension. Accounting Education: A Journal of Theory, Practice and Research, 2(2), 151-172. Sharan, Y. (2010). Cooperative learning for academic and social gains: Valued pedagogy, problematic practice. European Journal of Education, 45, 300-313.

FIGURE 1. Mean investment for each effort group. 35000 30000 25000 Mean investment 20000 15000 10000 5000 0 Less effort Equal effort More effort

FIGURE 2. Mean investment for each group size. 50000 45000 40000 Mean investment 35000 30000 25000 20000 15000 10000 5000 0 3 (4) 4 (8) 5 (11) 6 (5) 7 (1) Group size with the number of groups in parentheses