Language-specific Aptitude and the Role of Phonological Shortterm Memory in Second Language Acquisition

Similar documents
Essentials of Ability Testing. Joni Lakin Assistant Professor Educational Foundations, Leadership, and Technology

Linking the Common European Framework of Reference and the Michigan English Language Assessment Battery Technical Report

TEKS Correlations Proclamation 2017

Dyslexia/dyslexic, 3, 9, 24, 97, 187, 189, 206, 217, , , 367, , , 397,

Cognitive bases of reading and writing in a second/foreign language. DIALUKI (

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

CEFR Overall Illustrative English Proficiency Scales

A Study of Metacognitive Awareness of Non-English Majors in L2 Listening

Candidates must achieve a grade of at least C2 level in each examination in order to achieve the overall qualification at C2 Level.

Effects of Vocabulary and Phonotactic Probability on 2-Year-Olds Nonword Repetition

ANGLAIS LANGUE SECONDE

Student Morningness-Eveningness Type and Performance: Does Class Timing Matter?

EUROPEAN DAY OF LANGUAGES

Short-term memory in Down syndrome: Applying the working memory model

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

Basic German: CD/Book Package (LL(R) Complete Basic Courses) By Living Language

Linguistics. Undergraduate. Departmental Honors. Graduate. Faculty. Linguistics 1

raıs Factors affecting word learning in adults: A comparison of L2 versus L1 acquisition /r/ /aı/ /s/ /r/ /aı/ /s/ = individual sound

Effect of Word Complexity on L2 Vocabulary Learning

Developing phonological awareness: Is there a bilingual advantage?

Laporan Penelitian Unggulan Prodi

Exams: Accommodations Guidelines. English Language Learners

THE ROBUSTNESS OF APTITUDE EFFECTS IN NEAR-NATIVE SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 146 ( 2014 )

Reading Horizons. A Look At Linguistic Readers. Nicholas P. Criscuolo APRIL Volume 10, Issue Article 5

Linguistics Program Outcomes Assessment 2012

Creating Travel Advice

Correspondence between the DRDP (2015) and the California Preschool Learning Foundations. Foundations (PLF) in Language and Literacy

International Conference on Education and Educational Psychology (ICEEPSY 2012)

Age Effects on Syntactic Control in. Second Language Learning

Understanding and Supporting Dyslexia Godstone Village School. January 2017

The Effect of Extensive Reading on Developing the Grammatical. Accuracy of the EFL Freshmen at Al Al-Bayt University

Comparison Between Three Memory Tests: Cued Recall, Priming and Saving Closed-Head Injured Patients and Controls

Think A F R I C A when assessing speaking. C.E.F.R. Oral Assessment Criteria. Think A F R I C A - 1 -

Myths, Legends, Fairytales and Novels (Writing a Letter)

English Language and Applied Linguistics. Module Descriptions 2017/18

Rhythm-typology revisited.

Evidence for Reliability, Validity and Learning Effectiveness

ROSETTA STONE PRODUCT OVERVIEW

To appear in The TESOL encyclopedia of ELT (Wiley-Blackwell) 1 RECASTING. Kazuya Saito. Birkbeck, University of London

The Perception of Nasalized Vowels in American English: An Investigation of On-line Use of Vowel Nasalization in Lexical Access

Foreign Languages. Foreign Languages, General

Language Center. Course Catalog

Turkish- and German-speaking bilingual 4-to-6-year-olds living in Sweden: Effects of age, SES and home language input on vocabulary production

OVERVIEW OF CURRICULUM-BASED MEASUREMENT AS A GENERAL OUTCOME MEASURE

IB Diploma Program Language Policy San Jose High School

Probabilistic principles in unsupervised learning of visual structure: human data and a model

CELTA. Syllabus and Assessment Guidelines. Third Edition. University of Cambridge ESOL Examinations 1 Hills Road Cambridge CB1 2EU United Kingdom

Levels of processing: Qualitative differences or task-demand differences?

Busuu The Mobile App. Review by Musa Nushi & Homa Jenabzadeh, Introduction. 30 TESL Reporter 49 (2), pp

University of New Orleans

Abstractions and the Brain

A Quasi-Universal Nonword Repetition Task as a Diagnostic Tool for Bilingual Children learning Dutch as a Second Language

University of Pittsburgh Department of Slavic Languages and Literatures. Russian 0015: Russian for Heritage Learners 2 MoWe 3:00PM - 4:15PM G13 CL

Rachel E. Baker, Ann R. Bradlow. Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA

Title: Language Impairment in Bilingual children: State of the art 2017

The History of Language Teaching

Psychometric Research Brief Office of Shared Accountability

NAME: East Carolina University PSYC Developmental Psychology Dr. Eppler & Dr. Ironsmith

Speech Recognition at ICSI: Broadcast News and beyond

Algebra 1, Quarter 3, Unit 3.1. Line of Best Fit. Overview

CHAPTER 5: COMPARABILITY OF WRITTEN QUESTIONNAIRE DATA AND INTERVIEW DATA

Revisiting the role of prosody in early language acquisition. Megha Sundara UCLA Phonetics Lab

LIMITED COMMON GROUND, UNLIMITED COMMUNICATIVE SUCCESS: AN EXPERIMENTAL STUDY INTO LINGUA RECEPTIVA USING ESTONIAN AND RUSSIAN

On-the-Fly Customization of Automated Essay Scoring

+32 (0)

Automatization and orthographic development in second language visual word recognition

The impact of using electronic dictionary on vocabulary learning and retention of Iranian EFL learners

Making Sales Calls. Watertown High School, Watertown, Massachusetts. 1 hour, 4 5 days per week

Teacher: Mlle PERCHE Maeva High School: Lycée Charles Poncet, Cluses (74) Level: Seconde i.e year old students

Teaching ideas. AS and A-level English Language Spark their imaginations this year

Computerized training of the correspondences between phonological and orthographic units

SOFTWARE EVALUATION TOOL

An Empirical and Computational Test of Linguistic Relativity

Listening and Speaking Skills of English Language of Adolescents of Government and Private Schools

Rote rehearsal and spacing effects in the free recall of pure and mixed lists. By: Peter P.J.L. Verkoeijen and Peter F. Delaney

MATH Study Skills Workshop

Strategic Practice: Career Practitioner Case Study

Learning Disability Functional Capacity Evaluation. Dear Doctor,

Text and task authenticity in the EFL classroom

CLASSIFICATION OF PROGRAM Critical Elements Analysis 1. High Priority Items Phonemic Awareness Instruction

Feature-oriented vs. Needs-oriented Product Access for Non-Expert Online Shoppers

Running head: DELAY AND PROSPECTIVE MEMORY 1

The Effects of Strategic Planning and Topic Familiarity on Iranian Intermediate EFL Learners Written Performance in TBLT

The Effect of Written Corrective Feedback on the Accuracy of English Article Usage in L2 Writing

GCSE English Language 2012 An investigation into the outcomes for candidates in Wales

The Acquisition of English Grammatical Morphemes: A Case of Iranian EFL Learners

Organizing Comprehensive Literacy Assessment: How to Get Started

TEKS Comments Louisiana GLE

Running head: METACOGNITIVE STRATEGIES FOR ACADEMIC LISTENING 1. The Relationship between Metacognitive Strategies Awareness

The Oregon Literacy Framework of September 2009 as it Applies to grades K-3

Teacher intelligence: What is it and why do we care?

Why PPP won t (and shouldn t) go away

Concept Acquisition Without Representation William Dylan Sabo

AGENDA LEARNING THEORIES LEARNING THEORIES. Advanced Learning Theories 2/22/2016

LEXICAL COHESION ANALYSIS OF THE ARTICLE WHAT IS A GOOD RESEARCH PROJECT? BY BRIAN PALTRIDGE A JOURNAL ARTICLE

Introduction to the Common European Framework (CEF)

Understanding the Relationship between Comprehension and Production

Cued Recall From Image and Sentence Memory: A Shift From Episodic to Identical Elements Representation

Transcription:

Language-specific Aptitude and the Role of Phonological Shortterm Memory in Second Language Acquisition Vita V. Kogan 1 Abstract The aim of this investigation was to address three questions: 1) does the phonological short-term memory capacity predict vocabulary learning outcomes; (2) does the phonological memory capacity vary across unfamiliar foreign languages; and (3) does language learning experience affect the phonological memory capacity? Thirty participants were tested before and after a five-hour languagelearning course. We used nonword repetition tests with English, Russian and Indonesian-sounding words to measure phonological memory in these languages. We found that the phonological memory capacity in a specific language predicts vocabulary-learning outcomes in this language. Participants did not show a phonological bias toward a particular language, but homogeneously scored higher on the Russian nonword repetition test. All participants improved on nonword repetition tests across all languages after learning either Russian or Indonesian. These findings support the following conclusions. First, nonword repetition tests in specific languages can be used in aptitude batteries to detect a propensity for these languages. Second, individual bias towards the phonology of a specific language is rare and gets overridden by the phonological distance between a mother tongue and a target language. Third, phonological memory is affected by language experience and this effect can be transferred to other languages, including L1. 1. Introduction Learning a new language is less challenging for some than it is for others. Such inconsistency can be partially explained by language learning aptitude, the concept first introduced in the 1960s [1]. Since then, aptitude research has been concerned with the effects of individual differences on learning L2, where L2 is a uniform phenomenon, i.e. any language, regardless of its unique structural properties [2]. However, as more languages have been discovered and analyzed, the idea that languages differ fundamentally on every level of description has become salient [3]. Whereas all languages on an abstract level share similar underlying principles, structural profiles of languages are so diverse that, for example, learning Indonesian might call for a different set of cognitive and perceptual skills than learning Russian would. Because of the different demands imposed by idiosyncratic properties of a language, each language offers a unique challenge to the learner. Languages typologically similar to a learner s mother tongue are thought to be easier to acquire. The U.S. Foreign Service Institute developed a ranking list for an English native speaker, assigning every foreign language a level of learning difficulty. For example, Spanish is in level I, German in level II, Russian in level III, and Chinese and Arabic are in level IV. However, often individuals demonstrate different aptitude capacities for languages within the same level of difficulty: for example, the same learner might have a significantly higher aptitude for learning Arabic than Chinese, even though both languages belong to level IV. Thus, the learner-language idiosyncratic interaction should be taken into consideration, as well as the learner s cognitive abilities and the overall level of language difficulty. Chan, Skehan and Gong [4] note that existing aptitude tests assume all languages place the same demands on learners. For more precise predictions, they suggest incorporating something specific about the language to be learned into the test. We follow their suggestion and introduce the nonword repetition tests that mimic Russian and Indonesian. Nonword repetition tests employ nonsense words presented in aural mode and subsequently recalled by a participant [5]. It is a commonly used test to measure phonological-short term memory (PSTM), one of the proposed components of language aptitude. PSTM plays a central role with vocabulary learning [6] and functions in a language-specific way by exploiting relevant long-term phonological knowledge from prior lexical experience [7]. One aspect of PSTM that current L2 aptitude theories have not yet addressed is the degree to which PSTM capacity varies across unfamiliar languages. In the situation where both L2s are unfamiliar to a learner, even a small perceptual bias for processing particular phonotactics can expedite vocabulary acquisition. 1 University of Edinburgh, United Kingdom

In our study we concentrate on three issues: (1) the predictive power of PSTM in determining L2 vocabulary learning outcomes; (2) the variability of PSTM capacity across unfamiliar L2s; and (3) the effects of language learning experience on PSTM. 2. Method 2.1. Participants The participants were native speakers of English, students at the University of Edinburgh, U.K., who did not know either Russian or Indonesian. Five participants withdrew after the first testing session. 2.2. Stimuli and Procedures The participants were tested on two occasions: before (T1) and after (T2) completing a five-hour language-learning course (Table 1). 2.2.1. Pimsleur language-learning audio course. Participants were required to study either Russian (n=) or Indonesian (n=5) using the Pimsleur audio course. Both courses have identical contents and consist of 10 lessons, minutes each. The Russian and Indonesian languages were selected because of their relatively equal phonological distance from English; Russian and Indonesian are also phonologically distant from each other. Table 1. The layout of the experiment Tasks T1 (45 min) T2 (45 min) PSTM Nonverbal ability L2 performance The Raven Advanced Progressive Matrices Receptive Vocabulary in Russian or Indonesian 2.2.2. Nonword repetition tests. Three nonword repetition tests (NWR) were administered: Russian, Indonesian, and English. The nonwords from each test conformed to the phonotactic rules of a corresponding language. The performance of the participants was measured in terms of nonword span, which is the highest number of nonwords that a person could repeat correctly. Two native speakers of each language evaluated the responses (k = 0.65). 2.2.3. Nonverbal ability. The short version of the Raven Advanced Progressive Matrices Test [8] was administered to measure nonverbal intelligence [9]. The test consists of a series of homogeneous images of abstract patterns with each image missing a piece. Selecting from the eight options provided, participants were required to choose the piece that best completed the image. 2.2.4. L2 performance measure. To measure L2 performance we designed a receptive vocabulary test containing words randomly taken from the Pimsleur course. The participants heard each word spoken once and then had to select the corresponding English equivalent from the four options provided on screen.

3. Results 3.1. Descriptive statistics Table 2. Summary of performance across all tasks Task Scoring n min max mean median s.d. Max = 14 8. 9.00 11.92 12.63 12.00 13.00 1.33 1.26 7.00 5.75 13.50 10.14 10.91 10.00 11.00 1.82 1.86 4.50 4.50 11.50 12.00 7.49 7.87 7.38 7.00 1.89 2.23 Nonverbal ability Max = 12 7 12 10.60 11.00 1.45 Vocabulary (Ru + Ind) Max = 17.00.00 24.36.00 3.71 3.2. General connections The Pearson and Spearman correlations were performed to investigate the associations between the predictors and the lexical outcome variables. There were significant positive correlations between and the Russian vocabulary score (r =.43, ρ =.45, p <.05). Since we had only three participants who studied Indonesian, the Indonesian results lacked statistical power. Pearson s correlational analysis revealed that nonverbal ability was related significantly to the performance on the vocabulary test (r =.0.44, p <.05), however, this was not the case when the Spearman test was applied. 3.3. Relationships between skills To answer the first research question, a linear trend analysis was conducted, in which the NWR and nonverbal ability measures were entered into the model. accounted for.7% of the variance in vocabulary learning (R 2 = 0.7, F (2, 19) = 6.929, p <.01). The nonverbal ability scores showed no significant effects. To answer the second research question, we performed a paired Student s t-test. Having a mean score of 10.14 on and 7.49 on, it is clear that the participants performed better repeating the Russian-sounding nonwords. A paired-samples Student s t-test showed that the difference is significant (t(29) = 6.60, p < 0.01). It seems that, in comparison to, was easier. However, when the individual data was examined, the outcomes revealed that some individuals exhibited a reverse pattern. Participants 18, 23 and 28 (Figure 1) performed better on the task, than on the Russian one. Finally, we investigated the practice effects on PSTM. The average improvement on the test was 0.71 point (t(24) = 2.69, p < 0.05), and on the test was 0.77 point (t(24) = 3.07, p < 0.01). The improvement of 0.38 point on the test was not significant (t(24) = 0.91, p = 0.37). A Wilcoxon signed-rank test analysis yielded the same pattern of results.

Fig 1. NWR scores for each subject. 4. Discussion Our first research question concerned the role of PSTM in L2 acquisition. We found strong relationships between NWR performance and lexical attainment. These relationships were especially significant when the phonology of a specific language was used. Thus, the ability to repeat Russiansounding nonwords predicted vocabulary-learning outcomes in this language better than the ability to repeat English- or Indonesian-sounding words. The construction of nonword repetition language aptitude tests that exploit the phonology of specific languages may be more predictively effective than traditional aptitude measures. The second question had to do with the language-specificity of PSTM when unfamiliar languages are used. Our results indicate that the majority of participants perform better with the language which sound system is closer to their native one (the Russian prosodic structure resembles English). However, some individuals processed the phonotactics of Indonesian better than Russian, which might indicate the existence of a perceptual bias for the Indonesian phonology. Our third question addressed the issue of the stability of PSTM capacity. The results show that although the NWR performance was slightly different on the two testing occasions the difference was not significant for Indonesian. Considering that most of the participants were learning Russian, these findings are not surprising. Five hours of language exposure could provide enough material for developing an abstract phonological repertoire of the Russian language, which enhanced the NWR performance in this language. It remains unclear why the performance improved even though the participants learned a different language. It could be that there is a common mechanism that governs NWR performance in all languages and, thus, a transfer is possible from language to language. In fact, recent research on bilingualism has revealed that learning a foreign language has effects on general cognition beyond the language domain [10], i.e. it is possible to see an improvement in the areas not directly related to the language of study.

References [1] Carroll, J., & Sapon, S. Modern language aptitude test, 1959. [2] Robinson, P. Aptitude and second language acquisition, Annual Review of Applied Linguistics,, 2005, 46-73. [3] Evans, N., & Levinson, S. The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science, Behavioral and brain sciences, 32(05), 2009, 429-448. [4] Chan, E., Skehan, P., & Gong, G. Working memory, phonemic coding ability and foreign language aptitude: potential for construction of specific language aptitude tests-the case of Cantonese, Journal of English Language, Literatures in English and Cultural Studies, 60, 2011, 45-74. [5] Baddeley, A., Gathercole, S., & Papagno, C. The phonological loop as a language learning device, Psychological review, 105(1), 1998, 158. [6] Martin, K., & Ellis, N. The roles of phonological short-term memory and working memory in L2 grammar and vocabulary learning, Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 34(03), 2012, 379-413. [7] Gathercole, S. Nonword repetition and word learning: The nature of the relationship, Applied Psycholinguistics, 27(04), 2006, 513-543. [8] Raven, J. "Court, JH, & Raven, J.(1996). Manual for Raven s Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales. Section 3, Standard progressive matrices", London, Lowis, 1996. [9] Arthur, W., & Day, D. Development of a short form for the Raven Advanced Progressive Matrices Test, Educational and Psychological measurement, 54(2), 2012, 394-403. [10] Bak, T., Long, M., Vega-Mendoza, M., & Sorace, A. Novelty, Challenge, and Practice: The Impact of Intensive Language Learning on Attentional Functions, PloS one, 11(4), 2006, e0153485.