On the Effect of Immediate Feedback

Similar documents
Cognitive Apprenticeship Statewide Campus System, Michigan State School of Osteopathic Medicine 2011

Developing Autonomy in Language Learners: Diagnostic Teaching. LEARN Workshop July 28 and 29, 2015 Ra ed F. Qasem

Special Education Program Continuum

Lab 1 - The Scientific Method

A Comparison of the Effects of Two Practice Session Distribution Types on Acquisition and Retention of Discrete and Continuous Skills

Testing protects against proactive interference in face name learning

Introduction to Simulation

What s in a Step? Toward General, Abstract Representations of Tutoring System Log Data

Running head: DELAY AND PROSPECTIVE MEMORY 1

Predicting Students Performance with SimStudent: Learning Cognitive Skills from Observation

STA 225: Introductory Statistics (CT)

Introduction to Ensemble Learning Featuring Successes in the Netflix Prize Competition

AGENDA LEARNING THEORIES LEARNING THEORIES. Advanced Learning Theories 2/22/2016

Lecturing in the Preclinical Curriculum A GUIDE FOR FACULTY LECTURERS

SOFTWARE EVALUATION TOOL

Edexcel GCSE. Statistics 1389 Paper 1H. June Mark Scheme. Statistics Edexcel GCSE

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

An Empirical and Computational Test of Linguistic Relativity

The New Theory of Disuse Predicts Retrieval Enhanced Suggestibility (RES)

Algebra 1, Quarter 3, Unit 3.1. Line of Best Fit. Overview

DIDACTIC MODEL BRIDGING A CONCEPT WITH PHENOMENA

Age Effects on Syntactic Control in. Second Language Learning

Cognitive Modeling. Tower of Hanoi: Description. Tower of Hanoi: The Task. Lecture 5: Models of Problem Solving. Frank Keller.

Downey, California, USA Published online: 08 Feb 2013.

P-4: Differentiate your plans to fit your students

Foundations of Knowledge Representation in Cyc

Developing True/False Test Sheet Generating System with Diagnosing Basic Cognitive Ability

How Does Physical Space Influence the Novices' and Experts' Algebraic Reasoning?

Adult Education and Learning Theories Georgios Giannoukos, Georgios Besas

Mathematics Scoring Guide for Sample Test 2005

Going to School: Measuring Schooling Behaviors in GloFish

Sociology 521: Social Statistics and Quantitative Methods I Spring 2013 Mondays 2 5pm Kap 305 Computer Lab. Course Website

INSTRUCTIONAL FOCUS DOCUMENT Grade 5/Science

Innovative Methods for Teaching Engineering Courses

The Acquisition of Person and Number Morphology Within the Verbal Domain in Early Greek

Computerized Adaptive Psychological Testing A Personalisation Perspective

Maximizing Learning Through Course Alignment and Experience with Different Types of Knowledge

Levels of processing: Qualitative differences or task-demand differences?

teacher, peer, or school) on each page, and a package of stickers on which

Linking object names and object categories: Words (but not tones) facilitate object categorization in 6- and 12-month-olds

Good Enough Language Processing: A Satisficing Approach

AGS THE GREAT REVIEW GAME FOR PRE-ALGEBRA (CD) CORRELATED TO CALIFORNIA CONTENT STANDARDS

Pedagogical Content Knowledge for Teaching Primary Mathematics: A Case Study of Two Teachers

Joe Public ABC Company

PSYC 620, Section 001: Traineeship in School Psychology Fall 2016

Recommended Guidelines for the Diagnosis of Children with Learning Disabilities

A comparative study on cost-sharing in higher education Using the case study approach to contribute to evidence-based policy

WORK OF LEADERS GROUP REPORT

Average Daily Membership Proposed Change to Chapter 8 Rules and Regulations for the Wyoming School Foundation Program

Informatics 2A: Language Complexity and the. Inf2A: Chomsky Hierarchy

Sociology 521: Social Statistics and Quantitative Methods I Spring Wed. 2 5, Kap 305 Computer Lab. Course Website

Within the design domain, Seels and Richey (1994) identify four sub domains of theory and practice (p. 29). These sub domains are:

A student diagnosing and evaluation system for laboratory-based academic exercises

An extended dual search space model of scientific discovery learning

Instructional Intervention/Progress Monitoring (IIPM) Model Pre/Referral Process. and. Special Education Comprehensive Evaluation.

Why PPP won t (and shouldn t) go away

SCHEMA ACTIVATION IN MEMORY FOR PROSE 1. Michael A. R. Townsend State University of New York at Albany

Discussion Data reported here confirm and extend the findings of Antonucci (2009) which provided preliminary evidence that SFA treatment can result

* IMTESTING DURING TRAINING: WHY DOES IT ENHANCE LONG-TERM MOTOR TASK RETENTION?

Full text of O L O W Science As Inquiry conference. Science as Inquiry

GROUP COMPOSITION IN THE NAVIGATION SIMULATOR A PILOT STUDY Magnus Boström (Kalmar Maritime Academy, Sweden)

Presentation Format Effects in a Levels-of-Processing Task

GCSE Mathematics B (Linear) Mark Scheme for November Component J567/04: Mathematics Paper 4 (Higher) General Certificate of Secondary Education

A Critique of Running Records

Student Mobility Rates in Massachusetts Public Schools

Language Acquisition Chart

An Evaluation of the Interactive-Activation Model Using Masked Partial-Word Priming. Jason R. Perry. University of Western Ontario. Stephen J.

Source-monitoring judgments about anagrams and their solutions: Evidence for the role of cognitive operations information in memory

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

A Study of Metacognitive Awareness of Non-English Majors in L2 Listening

DOES RETELLING TECHNIQUE IMPROVE SPEAKING FLUENCY?

Inquiry Practice: Questions

Developing Students Research Proposal Design through Group Investigation Method

Unit: Human Impact Differentiated (Tiered) Task How Does Human Activity Impact Soil Erosion?

The open source development model has unique characteristics that make it in some

Second Language Acquisition in Adults: From Research to Practice

Characterizing Mathematical Digital Literacy: A Preliminary Investigation. Todd Abel Appalachian State University

Learning By Asking: How Children Ask Questions To Achieve Efficient Search

Lecturing Module

Hypermnesia in free recall and cued recall

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

To appear in The TESOL encyclopedia of ELT (Wiley-Blackwell) 1 RECASTING. Kazuya Saito. Birkbeck, University of London

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 209 ( 2015 )

Comparison Between Three Memory Tests: Cued Recall, Priming and Saving Closed-Head Injured Patients and Controls

White Paper. The Art of Learning

Predicting One s Own Forgetting: The Role of Experience-Based and Theory-Based Processes

Describing Motion Events in Adult L2 Spanish Narratives

have to be modeled) or isolated words. Output of the system is a grapheme-tophoneme conversion system which takes as its input the spelling of words,

Is Event-Based Prospective Memory Resistant to Proactive Interference?

Diagnostic Test. Middle School Mathematics

Student Morningness-Eveningness Type and Performance: Does Class Timing Matter?

How the Guppy Got its Spots:

Effect of Cognitive Apprenticeship Instructional Method on Auto-Mechanics Students

re An Interactive web based tool for sorting textbook images prior to adaptation to accessible format: Year 1 Final Report

Teaching for Generalization in the Natural Environment

Executive Summary. Sidney Lanier Senior High School

A Study of the Effectiveness of Using PER-Based Reforms in a Summer Setting

Beeson, P. M. (1999). Treating acquired writing impairment. Aphasiology, 13,

UNDERSTANDING DECISION-MAKING IN RUGBY By. Dave Hadfield Sport Psychologist & Coaching Consultant Wellington and Hurricanes Rugby.

Transcription:

On the Effect of Immediate Feedback Ido Erev! "! # Adi Luria @! "! # Anan Erev Previous studies of the effect of immediate feedback on learning reveal inconsistent findings. Whereas most field studies demonstrate the value of immediate feedback (see Kulik & Kulik, 1988), most direct laboratory studies document a positive effect of delayed feedback (see Schmidt & Bjork, 1992; Schmidt, Young, Swinnen & Shapiro, 1989). Our review of the literature suggests that the inconsistency can be resolved with the assertion that immediate feedback triggers two obvious but important positive effects, as well as a cognitively interesting negative effect. The positive effects involve the decision to practice, and the association of outcomes with their causes. That is, immediate feedback increases motivation (Skinner, 1958) and facilitates understanding of the relationship between actions and their results (Herrnstein, Loewenstein, Prelec & Vaughan, 1993). The cognitively interesting negative effect is captured by the guidance hypothesis (see Schmidt et al., 1989): Immediate feedback can lead to reliance on information that is not available during transfer. In certain settings this tendency implies counterproductive guessing behavior. Under this logic the apparent inconsistency in the literature is a result of the fact that most direct studies controlled the positive motivation and association effects, and focused on the cognitively interesting guidance effects. We present two studies of computer assisted learning that show the effects of immediate feedback when left uncontrolled. Introduction The role of feedback in education and instruction has been studied for many years. As early as 1911 E.L. Thorndike suggested that feedback served to connect responses to their preceding stimuli, and in the early 1920 s Sidney Pressey began to emphasize the role of feedback in error correction. These early works plus significant advancements in technology set the stage for B.F. Skinner s teaching machine approach, and since the early 1960 s both programmed instruction and feedback have become pertinent, developing areas for research and application (Mory, 2003). However, despite the many years of research, previous studies of the effect of immediate feedback on learning reveal inconsistent findings. Whereas most field studies demonstrate the value of immediate feedback (see Kulik & Kulik, 1988), most direct laboratory studies document a positive effect of delayed feedback (see Schmidt & Bjork, 1992; Schmidt, Young, Swinnen & Shapiro, 1989).

:4. 7 * & 4 6 8 * 6 ) - ) - 7 * & The current research attempts to shed light on these results. Our review of the literature suggests that the inconsistency can be resolved with the assertion that immediate feedback triggers two obvious but important positive effects, as well as a cognitively interesting negative effect. The positive effects involve the decision to practice, and the association of outcomes with their causes. That is, immediate feedback increases motivation (Skinner, 1958) and facilitates understanding of the relationship between actions and their results (Herrnstein, Loewenstein, Prelec & Vaughan, 1993). The cognitively interesting negative effect is captured by the guidance hypothesis (see Schmidt et al., 1989): Immediate feedback can lead to reliance on information that is not available during transfer. In certain settings this tendency implies counterproductive guessing behavior. Generally speaking, we suggest that the negative effects of immediate feedback were predominantly demonstrated in studies in which at least one of (the boring) positive effects of immediate feedback had been "controlled out." In addition, we hypothesize that the boring positive effects of immediate feedback tend to be larger than the interesting negative effects. Thus, when the positive effects are not controlled, the total effect of immediate feedback is likely to be positive. This hypothesis is evaluated in two studies. Study 1 focuses on the role of the association effect, and Study 2 focuses on the motivation effect. Study 1 In their classic study, Schmidt, Young, Swinnen & Shapiro (1989) compared the effects of several 'knowledge of results' intervals on performance in a ballistic timing task. Subjects were required to move a slide between specified target zones (including reversal of direction) and maintain a performance time as close to 550 ms as possible. Feedback was provided either after 1 trial (meaning KR after every trial), every 5 trials, 10 trials, or 15 trials. After each trail, subjects' constant error for that trail was plotted on a graph of performance against trials, but shown to them only after the appropriate number of trials for their experimental group (1, 5, 10, or 15 trials). All subjects were required to perform 90 trials during the practice phase, and then 25 more trials ("retention phase") without any KR. In addition, subjects returned two days later for a delayed retention test, during which they received no KR. Schmidt et al.'s (1989) results show that during the practice phase, performance decreased as feedback interval increased. That is, subjects in the 1-trial summary (immediate feedback) condition showed the best practice performance. However, the opposite pattern was observed during retention. In the twoday retention test, the participants trained with immediate feedback performed worse than the participants trained with delayed feedback. These and similar results are often used to support the assertion that immediate feedback leads to shallow learning (see Swinnen, Schmidt, Nicholson & Shapiro, 1990; Schmidt & Bjork, 1992). The current analysis leads to very different interpretation of Schmidt et al. s results. It suggests that the negative guidance effect is interesting, but is likely to be less general than suggested by some of the efforts to apply it to education and E-learning. We hypothesize that the implications of the guidance effect are highly sensitive to the simplified, one-response, experimental procedure

implemented by Schmidt et al. in order to control the association effect. Specifically, we contend that a slight modification of the Schmidt et al. s paradigm, an increase in the number of required responses, is expected to contribute to the relative value of immediate feedback in two ways. First, when the subject is asked to learn more than one response, the association effect is expected to impair the value of delayed feedback. In addition, the assortment of possible responses decreases reliance on preceding response. In order to test this hypothesis we conducted a replication of Schmidt et al. in which the participants were asked to learn four distinct responses. As in the original study, the different responses involved the performance of a simple motor task at a particular speed. The motor task involved two key presses. The different tasks differed with respect to the required time interval between the two presses: It was 300, 600, 900, or 1200 ms. The experiment included two control and two experimental conditions. The two control conditions involved a simplified replication of Schmidt et al. (1989). The participants were asked to learn a single motor response: To press a key twice such that the time between the two keys will be 750ms. The participants received immediate feedback after each choice in the Immediate control condition, and summary feedback in groups of 5 trials, in the Delayed control condition. As in the original study, the experiment included 90 practice trials, a 10 minute retention test, and a 2-day retention test. The two experimental conditions were identical to the control conditions with the exception that the participants were asked to learn four time intervals: 300, 600, 900, or 1200 ms. In each of the trials they received an instruction concerning the required time interval between the two presses. The results in the control conditions show the pattern observed by Schmidt et al. (1989). Immediate feedback facilitated performance during transfer, but impaired performance during retention. The results in the experimental conditions (4 possible response) show a very different pattern. The requirement to consider four tasks had two main effects. First, it impaired performance in the delayed feedback condition. This result demonstrates the association effect. Second, the added tasks decreased the difference between practice and retention in the immediate feedback condition. Overall, the immediate feedback condition lead to much better performance during practice and transfer. Study 2 Most studies of the effect of immediate feedback in computer assisted learning control the effect of the decision to practice by ensuring fixed practice time. In order to evaluate the implication of this research method, we compared two versions of an electronic notebook designed to facilitate practice of long division. Version Immediate provided feedback after each step. Version delayed provided delayed feedback for sets of five problems. The participants were elementary school students. They were invited to the laboratory for a 2-hour experiment. During this time they had five opportunities to select between practicing long division, and playing games on the web. The results revealed a motivation effect. The participants practiced

:4. 7 * & 4 6 8 * 6 ) - ) - 7 * & more in the immediate feedback condition. In addition, the results show that for each level of practice time (when the motivation effect is controlled), the immediate feedback lead to lower performance (more errors). This result is consistent with the guidance hypothesis. However, this negative effect is not observed on the aggregate. Conclusions Previous studies of the effect of immediate feedback on learning appear to lead to contradicting results. The current research highlights a simple and sufficient explanation of this inconsistency. According to this explanation, the inconsistency is a result of the fact that many of the experimental studies controlled the positive effects, and focused on the negative effect of immediate feedback. The current results suggest that this research method might lead to incorrect generalizations. The fact that the positive effects of immediate feedback are less interesting does not mean that they are less important.

References Herrnstein, R. J., Loewenstein, G. F., Prelec, D., & Vaughan, W., Jr. (1993). Utility maximization and melioration: internalities in individual choice. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 6, 149 185. Kulik, J.A. & Kulik, C.C. (1988). Timing of feedback and verbal learning. Review of Educational Research,58(1), 79-97. Mory, E.H. (2003). Feedback Research Revisited. In D.H. Jonassen (ed.), Handbook of research for educational communications and technology, 2cnd Edition (pp. 745-783). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Schmidt, R.A. & Bjork, R.A. (1992). New Conceptualizations of practice: Common principles in three paradigms suggest new concepts for training. Psychological Science, 3(4), 207-217. Schmidt, R.A., Young, D.E., Swinnen, S. & Shapiro, D.C. (1989). Summary knowledge of results for skill acquisition: Support for the guidance hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15(2), 352-359. Skinner, B.F. (1958). Teaching Machines. Science, 128 (3330), 969-977. Swinnen, S.P., Schmidt, R.A., Nicholson, D.E. & Shapiro, D.C. (1990). Information feedback for skill acquisition: Instantaneous knowledge of results degrades learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 16(4), 706-716.