Homophones, Lexical Retrieval, and Sensitivity to Detail

Similar documents
Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

An Evaluation of the Interactive-Activation Model Using Masked Partial-Word Priming. Jason R. Perry. University of Western Ontario. Stephen J.

Processing Lexically Embedded Spoken Words

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

Rhythm-typology revisited.

Phonological encoding in speech production

The Perception of Nasalized Vowels in American English: An Investigation of On-line Use of Vowel Nasalization in Lexical Access

THE INFLUENCE OF TASK DEMANDS ON FAMILIARITY EFFECTS IN VISUAL WORD RECOGNITION: A COHORT MODEL PERSPECTIVE DISSERTATION

Rachel E. Baker, Ann R. Bradlow. Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA

Infants learn phonotactic regularities from brief auditory experience

Phonological Encoding in Sentence Production

2,1 .,,, , %, ,,,,,,. . %., Butterworth,)?.(1989; Levelt, 1989; Levelt et al., 1991; Levelt, Roelofs & Meyer, 1999

A Cross-language Corpus for Studying the Phonetics and Phonology of Prominence

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

Revisiting the role of prosody in early language acquisition. Megha Sundara UCLA Phonetics Lab

The phonological grammar is probabilistic: New evidence pitting abstract representation against analogy

Running head: DELAY AND PROSPECTIVE MEMORY 1

Acoustic correlates of stress and their use in diagnosing syllable fusion in Tongan. James White & Marc Garellek UCLA

Test Blueprint. Grade 3 Reading English Standards of Learning

Developing a College-level Speed and Accuracy Test

Lexical Access during Sentence Comprehension (Re)Consideration of Context Effects

Effects of Open-Set and Closed-Set Task Demands on Spoken Word Recognition

Atypical Prosodic Structure as an Indicator of Reading Level and Text Difficulty

Running head: FAST MAPPING SKILLS IN THE DEVELOPING LEXICON. Fast Mapping Skills in the Developing Lexicon. Lisa Gershkoff-Stowe. Indiana University

SOUND STRUCTURE REPRESENTATION, REPAIR AND WELL-FORMEDNESS: GRAMMAR IN SPOKEN LANGUAGE PRODUCTION. Adam B. Buchwald

Perceived speech rate: the effects of. articulation rate and speaking style in spontaneous speech. Jacques Koreman. Saarland University

Dyslexia/dyslexic, 3, 9, 24, 97, 187, 189, 206, 217, , , 367, , , 397,

Speech Recognition at ICSI: Broadcast News and beyond

Universal contrastive analysis as a learning principle in CAPT

Which verb classes and why? Research questions: Semantic Basis Hypothesis (SBH) What verb classes? Why the truth of the SBH matters

SEGMENTAL FEATURES IN SPONTANEOUS AND READ-ALOUD FINNISH

Aging and the Use of Context in Ambiguity Resolution: Complex Changes From Simple Slowing

Language Acquisition by Identical vs. Fraternal SLI Twins * Karin Stromswold & Jay I. Rifkin

Unraveling symbolic number processing and the implications for its association with mathematics. Delphine Sasanguie

The Representation of Concrete and Abstract Concepts: Categorical vs. Associative Relationships. Jingyi Geng and Tatiana T. Schnur

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

Lecture 2: Quantifiers and Approximation

Visual processing speed: effects of auditory input on

raıs Factors affecting word learning in adults: A comparison of L2 versus L1 acquisition /r/ /aı/ /s/ /r/ /aı/ /s/ = individual sound

Journal of Phonetics

Introduction to the Common European Framework (CEF)

A Bootstrapping Model of Frequency and Context Effects in Word Learning

Florida Reading Endorsement Alignment Matrix Competency 1

Session 2B From understanding perspectives to informing public policy the potential and challenges for Q findings to inform survey design

COMPUTATIONAL COMPLEXITY OF LEFT-ASSOCIATIVE GRAMMAR

have to be modeled) or isolated words. Output of the system is a grapheme-tophoneme conversion system which takes as its input the spelling of words,

Figuration & Frequency: A Usage-Based Approach to Metaphor

The influence of metrical constraints on direct imitation across French varieties

Effect of Word Complexity on L2 Vocabulary Learning

Curriculum Vitae. Sara C. Steele, Ph.D, CCC-SLP 253 McGannon Hall 3750 Lindell Blvd., St. Louis, MO Tel:

Deliberate Learning and Vocabulary Acquisition in a Second Language

A Case Study: News Classification Based on Term Frequency

Effects of Vocabulary and Phonotactic Probability on 2-Year-Olds Nonword Repetition

The Common European Framework of Reference for Languages p. 58 to p. 82

Think A F R I C A when assessing speaking. C.E.F.R. Oral Assessment Criteria. Think A F R I C A - 1 -

Language Acquisition Chart

Cued Recall From Image and Sentence Memory: A Shift From Episodic to Identical Elements Representation

Stages of Literacy Ros Lugg

Speech Segmentation Using Probabilistic Phonetic Feature Hierarchy and Support Vector Machines

Formulaic Language and Fluency: ESL Teaching Applications

Inhibitory control in L2 phonological processing

Learners Use Word-Level Statistics in Phonetic Category Acquisition

Automatization and orthographic development in second language visual word recognition

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

On the Formation of Phoneme Categories in DNN Acoustic Models

Understanding and Supporting Dyslexia Godstone Village School. January 2017

ELA/ELD Standards Correlation Matrix for ELD Materials Grade 1 Reading

Demonstration of problems of lexical stress on the pronunciation Turkish English teachers and teacher trainees by computer

Sensitivity to second language argument structure

Phenomena of gender attraction in Polish *

Perceptual scaling of voice identity: common dimensions for different vowels and speakers

Degeneracy results in canalisation of language structure: A computational model of word learning

Voice conversion through vector quantization

Enhancing Unlexicalized Parsing Performance using a Wide Coverage Lexicon, Fuzzy Tag-set Mapping, and EM-HMM-based Lexical Probabilities

Individual Differences & Item Effects: How to test them, & how to test them well

On the nature of voicing assimilation(s)

The influence of orthographic transparency on word recognition. by dyslexic and normal readers

Word Stress and Intonation: Introduction

SOFTWARE EVALUATION TOOL

Age Effects on Syntactic Control in. Second Language Learning

5. UPPER INTERMEDIATE

Multiple Route Model of Lexical Processing

AP PSYCHOLOGY VACATION WORK PACKET UNIT 7A: MEMORY

How to Judge the Quality of an Objective Classroom Test

12- A whirlwind tour of statistics

Journal of Phonetics

OVERVIEW OF CURRICULUM-BASED MEASUREMENT AS A GENERAL OUTCOME MEASURE

Houghton Mifflin Reading Correlation to the Common Core Standards for English Language Arts (Grade1)

Psychology of Speech Production and Speech Perception

Syntactic surprisal affects spoken word duration in conversational contexts

REVIEW OF CONNECTED SPEECH

DIBELS Next BENCHMARK ASSESSMENTS

Evidence for a Limited-Cascading Account of Written Word Naming

Different Task Type and the Perception of the English Interdental Fricatives

On building models of spoken-word recognition: When there is as much to learn from natural oddities as artificial normality

+32 (0)

Word Segmentation of Off-line Handwritten Documents

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

CEFR Overall Illustrative English Proficiency Scales

Transcription:

Homophones, Lexical Retrieval, and Sensitivity to Detail Chelsea Sanker Linguistic Society of America Salt Lake City, Utah 6 January 2018 1 / 29

Introduction Lexical storage of homophones Do homophones have shared phonological representations? /s2n/ son sun 2 / 29

Introduction Lexical storage of homophones Do they have independent lexical entries that happen to have identical phonological representations? son /s2n/ sun /s2n/ 3 / 29

Introduction Lexical storage of homophones Evidence for separate phonological entries: Distinct frequency effects in lexical access (e.g. Caramazza et al. 2001, Simpson & Burgess 1985, Grainger et al. 2001) Weak or absent priming between homophone mates (e.g. Schvaneveldt et al. 1976, Masson & Freedman 1990) Phonetic differences, based on frequency and part of speech (Gahl 2008, Guion 1995) 4 / 29

Introduction Lexical storage of homophones There is evidence for influence of homophones on each other (e.g. Jescheniak & Levelt 1994) But they could interact without having shared representations son /s2n/ /s2n/ sun 5 / 29

Introduction This study An AX discrimination task, to determine: How listeners knowledge of homophones influences interpretations of phonological forms Listeners sensitivity to acoustic details, particularly as potential cues for discriminating between homophone mates 6 / 29

Experimental Design Listeners 23 native speakers of American English (mean age 21.6; 7 male) No reported speech or hearing disorders 7 / 29

Experimental Design Same-Different Task Listeners heard pairs of words and pressed a button to decide whether they were the same or different Counter-balanced for right-left responses Response time measured from the beginning of the second word 8 / 29

Experimental Design Stimuli Words produced in isolation, in randomized order, recorded in a sound-attenuated booth Five types of pairs 1 homophone-homophone pairs (e.g. sun-son) 2 same pairs for a word with a homophone (e.g. sun-sun) 3 same pairs for a word with no homophone (e.g. cat-cat) 4 different pairs, with a single segmental contrast (e.g. pat-cat) The ratio of apparent same pairs (1-3) to different pairs (4) was equal Two speakers; in all word-pairs, the two words were from different speakers 9 / 29

Experimental Design Blocks Within a block, differences were always in the same position: Onset (e.g. pat-cat), nucleus (e.g. kit-cat), coda (e.g. cap-cat) Each listener completed three blocks, one of each contrast type Block order was balanced across participants 10 / 29

Hypotheses Hypotheses: Lexical influence Hypothesis 1: Homophones are processed as lexically distinct items, even in non-semantic tasks, and thus will act like competitors in processing Counter-Hypothesis 1: Homophones have shared phonological representations, and will behave as a single unit rather than competitors 11 / 29

Hypotheses Hypotheses: Phonetic detail Hypothesis 2: Listeners are sensitive to non-contrastive acoustic detail (cf. E.g. Babel & Johnson 2010), and will accordingly respond more quickly and with higher accuracy to phonologically identical pairs that are more acoustically similar Counter-Hypothesis 2: Listeners are not influenced by non-contrastive acoustic detail, and thus their response patterns will not be influenced by acoustic distance between items of a pair 12 / 29

Results Summary Linear mixed effects model for log response times, excluding different pairs Estimate Std. Error t value p value (Intercept) 1.3e-01 4.5e-02 2.9 0.0065** Type Non-hom -8.7e-02 2.7e-02-3.2 0.0012** Type Hph-Hph -3.5e-02 3.0e-02-1.2 0.24 ContrastType C 5.5e-02 8.7e-03 6.3 < 0.001*** ContrastType O -1.2e-02 8.6e-03-1.3 0.18 Response same -1.6e-01 2.3e-02-6.8 < 0.001*** TypeNon-hom:ResponseSame 1.0e-01 2.8e-02 3.7 < 0.001*** TypeHph-Hph:ResponseSame 4.0e-02 3.2e-02 1.2 0.21 Intercept: Type = Same hom; ContrastType = N; Response = different 13 / 29

Results Effects of Context within the Block Contrast type Significant differences in mean response time based on contrast type in different pairs (p < 0.001 for all comparisons) Also significant in phonologically non-contrastive items for coda blocks vs. others (p < 0.001), but not between onset and nucleus blocks (p = 0.18) Different Pairs Same & Hph-hph Pairs log seconds -1.0-0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 log seconds -1.0-0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 onset nucleus coda Figure: Response Times by Contrast Type 14 / 29

Results Pair Type Homophone mate pairs: Same or different? Hph-hph pairs patterned like same pairs: The majority of responses were same (89.3%; 90.2% for same pairs and 4.0% for different pairs) same responses were significantly faster than different responses (1044 ms vs. 1469 ms, p < 0.001), paralleling faster responses of same for same pairs (1058 ms vs. 1354 ms, p < 0.001) 15 / 29

Results Pair Type Decision patterns by pair type Lexically unambiguous (cat-cat) same pairs were identified as same more frequently (91.1%) than lexically ambiguous (sun-sun) same pairs (88.3%) or hph-hph (sun-son) pairs (89.3%); the latter two types did not differ * n.s *** % 'same' responses 0.60 0.70 0.80 0.90 hph-hph (sun-son) same (sun-sun) same (cat-cat) Figure: % same Responses by Type 16 / 29

Results Pair Type Response times by pair type Response times exhibited the same pattern as responses, largely due to speed of different responses Hph-hph Pairs (sun-son) Same Pairs (sun-sun) Same Pairs (cat-cat) log seconds -1.5-1.0-0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 same different log seconds -1.5-1.0-0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 log seconds -1.5-1.0-0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 Figure: Response Time by Type and Response 17 / 29

Results Pair Type Lexically ambiguous phonologically matching pairs Hesitance to identify the same word as same when it was a word for which a homophone exists may reflect uncertainty about whether homophones are identical or just close phonological neighbors cf. slower responses for words with high neighborhood density (e.g. Vitevitch & Luce 1999) 18 / 29

Results Shape of the lexicon Frequency: same pairs Response time was negatively correlated with word frequency in same pairs (sun-sun, cat-cat): r(248) = -0.16, p =.01 response time (z-scored) -0.3-0.1 0.0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0e+00 1e+05 2e+05 3e+05 4e+05 5e+05 6e+05 word frequency Figure: Response Time by Word Frequency, All Same Pairs 19 / 29

Results Shape of the lexicon Frequency: Lexically unambiguous same pairs The correlation between response time and frequency was weaker considering only lexically unambiguous pairs (cat-cat): r(170) = -0.083, p = 0.28 May in part reflect duration differences in production, though the correlation between response time and word duration did not reach significance: r(170) = 0.11, p = 0.15 The correlation between word duration and frequency was also weak: r(170) = -0.086, p = 0.26 20 / 29

Results Shape of the lexicon Frequency: Lexically ambiguous same pairs The correlation between response time and frequency was strongest among words with homophones (sun-sun): r(78) = -0.22, p = 0.045 Likely reflects duration differences in production, given that listeners could not discriminate between homophone mates There was a positive correlation between response time and word duration: r(78) = 0.26, p = 0.016 Among words with homophones, there was a negative correlation between word duration and frequency: r(78) = -0.21, p = 0.056 21 / 29

Results Shape of the lexicon Neighborhood density: Lexically ambiguous items Negative correlation between neighborhood density and response time among lexically ambiguous items (r(158) = -0.29, p < 0.001) response time (z-scored) -0.3-0.1 0.1 0.3 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 neighborhood density Figure: Response Time by Neighborhood Density 22 / 29

Results Shape of the lexicon Neighborhood density The result for lexically ambiguous items is counter to competition-based explanations of inhibitory neighborhood density effects in other tasks Might suggest that more neighbors facilitate faster evaluation of phonological category contrasts in that region due to greater representation Weaker correlation among lexically unambiguous items (r(170) = -0.092, p = 0.23) 23 / 29

Results Acoustic detail Acoustic details: Lexically ambiguous pairs Acoustic differences between items had a consistent positive correlation with response time for lexically ambiguous phonologically matching pairs Though it only reached significance in some measures F0 mean F0 range vowel dur. spectral tilt hph-hph (sun-son) 0.28** -0.0085 0.13 0.35*** same, hph (sun-sun) 0.091-0.0054 0.045 0.15 Table: Acoustic Correlations with RT 24 / 29

Results Acoustic detail Acoustic details: Lexically unambiguous pairs This trend was not present in lexically unambiguous pairs Suggests that attention to these details is mediated by listeners expecting differences F0 mean F0 range vowel dur. spectral tilt same, non-hph (cat-cat) 0.071 0.011 0.032 0.045 Table: Acoustic Correlations with RT 25 / 29

Conclusions Lexical Models Homophones must be stored as separate lexical items, along with separate phonological entries Having separate phonological entries creates some uncertainty about phonological contrasts, resulting in slower decisions and more different responses to lexically ambiguous pairs 26 / 29

Conclusions Frequency and neighborhood density Frequency is negatively correlated with response time, but likely only as a result of the correlation between frequency and word duration Negative correlation between neighborhood density and response time phonological contrasts benefit when supported by lexical contrasts 27 / 29

Conclusions Attention to detail At least when evaluating words produced in isolation, listeners are more influenced by phonological contrasts than phonetic details However, greater acoustic distance in multiple measures is correlated with response time, for lexically ambiguous items 28 / 29

References Babel, M., & Johnson, K. 2010. Accessing psycho-acoustic perception and language-specific perception with speech sounds. Laboratory Phonology, 1, 179 205. Caramazza, A., Costa, A., Miozzo, M., & Bi, Y. 2001. The specific-word frequency effect: Implications for the representation of homophones in speech production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27(6), 1430 1450. Gahl, S. 2008. Time and thyme are not homophones: The effect of lemma frequency on word durations in spontaneous speech. Language, 84(3), 474 498. Grainger, J., Van Kang, M., & Segui, J. Cross-modal repetition priming of heterographic homophones. Memory & Cognition, 29(1), 53 61. Guion, S. 1995. Word frequency effects among homonyms. Texas Linguistic Forum, 35, 103 116. Jescheniak, J., & Levelt, W. 1994. Word frequency effects in speech production: Retrieval of syntactic information and of phonological form. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(4), 824 843. Masson, M., & Freedman, L. 1990. Fluent identification of repeated words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16(3), 355 373. Schvaneveldt, R., Mayer, D., & Becker, C. 1976. Lexical ambiguity, semantic context, and visual word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2(2), 243 256. Simpson, G., & Burgess, C. 1985. Activation and selection processes in the recognition of ambiguous words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11(1), 28 39. Vitevitch, M., & Luce, P. 1999. Probabilistic phonotactics and neighborhood activation in spoken word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 374 408. 29 / 29