Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Prosodic phrasing and articulation rate variation

Similar documents
Collecting dialect data and making use of them an interim report from Swedia 2000

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

Perceived speech rate: the effects of. articulation rate and speaking style in spontaneous speech. Jacques Koreman. Saarland University

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Voiced-voiceless distinction in alaryngeal speech - acoustic and articula

Rhythm-typology revisited.

Word Stress and Intonation: Introduction

A survey of intonation systems

Revisiting the role of prosody in early language acquisition. Megha Sundara UCLA Phonetics Lab

A Cross-language Corpus for Studying the Phonetics and Phonology of Prominence

Acoustic correlates of stress and their use in diagnosing syllable fusion in Tongan. James White & Marc Garellek UCLA

Eyebrows in French talk-in-interaction

Copyright by Niamh Eileen Kelly 2015

Rachel E. Baker, Ann R. Bradlow. Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA

SEGMENTAL FEATURES IN SPONTANEOUS AND READ-ALOUD FINNISH

Atypical Prosodic Structure as an Indicator of Reading Level and Text Difficulty

Answers: Year 4 Textbook 3 Pages 4 10

The influence of metrical constraints on direct imitation across French varieties

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. VCV-sequencies in a preliminary text-to-speech system for female speech

Journal of Phonetics

THE PERCEPTION AND PRODUCTION OF STRESS AND INTONATION BY CHILDREN WITH COCHLEAR IMPLANTS

Running head: DELAY AND PROSPECTIVE MEMORY 1

L1 Influence on L2 Intonation in Russian Speakers of English

Speech Recognition at ICSI: Broadcast News and beyond

Accessing Higher Education in Developing Countries: panel data analysis from India, Peru and Vietnam

1. REFLEXES: Ask questions about coughing, swallowing, of water as fast as possible (note! Not suitable for all

Individual Differences & Item Effects: How to test them, & how to test them well

Pobrane z czasopisma New Horizons in English Studies Data: 18/11/ :52:20. New Horizons in English Studies 1/2016

The Effect of Discourse Markers on the Speaking Production of EFL Students. Iman Moradimanesh

The Acquisition of English Intonation by Native Greek Speakers

PROFESSIONAL TREATMENT OF TEACHERS AND STUDENT ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT. James B. Chapman. Dissertation submitted to the Faculty of the Virginia

Discourse Structure in Spoken Language: Studies on Speech Corpora

Review in ICAME Journal, Volume 38, 2014, DOI: /icame

Fluency Disorders. Kenneth J. Logan, PhD, CCC-SLP

Unvoiced Landmark Detection for Segment-based Mandarin Continuous Speech Recognition

NCEO Technical Report 27

Speech Emotion Recognition Using Support Vector Machine

English Language and Applied Linguistics. Module Descriptions 2017/18

Demonstration of problems of lexical stress on the pronunciation Turkish English teachers and teacher trainees by computer

PROJECT MANAGEMENT AND COMMUNICATION SKILLS DEVELOPMENT STUDENTS PERCEPTION ON THEIR LEARNING

Executive summary (in English)

Automatic intonation assessment for computer aided language learning

Voice conversion through vector quantization

Dyslexia/dyslexic, 3, 9, 24, 97, 187, 189, 206, 217, , , 367, , , 397,

PREDISPOSING FACTORS TOWARDS EXAMINATION MALPRACTICE AMONG STUDENTS IN LAGOS UNIVERSITIES: IMPLICATIONS FOR COUNSELLING

ANALYSIS: LABOUR MARKET SUCCESS OF VOCATIONAL AND HIGHER EDUCATION GRADUATES

VOL. 3, NO. 5, May 2012 ISSN Journal of Emerging Trends in Computing and Information Sciences CIS Journal. All rights reserved.

Think A F R I C A when assessing speaking. C.E.F.R. Oral Assessment Criteria. Think A F R I C A - 1 -

(Includes a Detailed Analysis of Responses to Overall Satisfaction and Quality of Academic Advising Items) By Steve Chatman

The Structure of the ORD Speech Corpus of Russian Everyday Communication

Principal vacancies and appointments

General rules and guidelines for the PhD programme at the University of Copenhagen Adopted 3 November 2014

A Study of Metacognitive Awareness of Non-English Majors in L2 Listening

Task Types. Duration, Work and Units Prepared by

Bachelor of Software Engineering: Emerging sustainable partnership with industry in ODL

ELA/ELD Standards Correlation Matrix for ELD Materials Grade 1 Reading

Phonological encoding in speech production

VIEW: An Assessment of Problem Solving Style

Journal of Phonetics

What do Medical Students Need to Learn in Their English Classes?

Psychometric Research Brief Office of Shared Accountability

Universal contrastive analysis as a learning principle in CAPT

Correspondence between the DRDP (2015) and the California Preschool Learning Foundations. Foundations (PLF) in Language and Literacy

Classroom Connections Examining the Intersection of the Standards for Mathematical Content and the Standards for Mathematical Practice

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Sound symbolism in deictic words

Bitonal lexical pitch accents in the Limburgian dialect of Borgloon

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

United states panel on climate change. memorandum

ROA Technical Report. Jaap Dronkers ROA-TR-2014/1. Research Centre for Education and the Labour Market ROA

Phonological Processing for Urdu Text to Speech System

AUTOMATIC DETECTION OF PROLONGED FRICATIVE PHONEMES WITH THE HIDDEN MARKOV MODELS APPROACH 1. INTRODUCTION

A Case Study: News Classification Based on Term Frequency

Beginning primarily with the investigations of Zimmermann (1980a),

**Note: this is slightly different from the original (mainly in format). I would be happy to send you a hard copy.**

One major theoretical issue of interest in both developing and

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Practical Research. Planning and Design. Paul D. Leedy. Jeanne Ellis Ormrod. Upper Saddle River, New Jersey Columbus, Ohio

CEFR Overall Illustrative English Proficiency Scales

Linking object names and object categories: Words (but not tones) facilitate object categorization in 6- and 12-month-olds

STA 225: Introductory Statistics (CT)

The My Class Activities Instrument as Used in Saturday Enrichment Program Evaluation

The Journey to Vowelerria VOWEL ERRORS: THE LOST WORLD OF SPEECH INTERVENTION. Preparation: Education. Preparation: Education. Preparation: Education

To appear in the Proceedings of the 35th Meetings of the Chicago Linguistics Society. Post-vocalic spirantization: Typology and phonetic motivations

English intonation patterns expressing politeness and their cross-language perception

Greek Teachers Attitudes toward the Inclusion of Students with Special Educational Needs

Third Misconceptions Seminar Proceedings (1993)

GROUP COMPOSITION IN THE NAVIGATION SIMULATOR A PILOT STUDY Magnus Boström (Kalmar Maritime Academy, Sweden)

03/07/15. Research-based welfare education. A policy brief

Modern TTS systems. CS 294-5: Statistical Natural Language Processing. Types of Modern Synthesis. TTS Architecture. Text Normalization

Does the Difficulty of an Interruption Affect our Ability to Resume?

General syllabus for third-cycle courses and study programmes in

The Effect of Extensive Reading on Developing the Grammatical. Accuracy of the EFL Freshmen at Al Al-Bayt University

Communication Disorders Program. Strategic Plan January 2012 December 2016

Automatization and orthographic development in second language visual word recognition

DIDACTIC MODEL BRIDGING A CONCEPT WITH PHENOMENA

EDUCATIONAL ATTAINMENT

Joint Study Application Japan - Outgoing

Peer Influence on Academic Achievement: Mean, Variance, and Network Effects under School Choice

PIRLS. International Achievement in the Processes of Reading Comprehension Results from PIRLS 2001 in 35 Countries

Transcription:

Dept. for Speech, Music and Hearing Quarterly Progress and Status Report Prosodic phrasing and articulation rate variation Hansson, P. journal: Proceedings of Fonetik, TMH-QPSR volume: 44 number: 1 year: 22 pages: 173-176 http://www.speech.kth.se/qpsr

TMH-QPSR Vol. 44 Fonetik 22 Prosodic phrasing and articulation rate variation Petra Hansson Department of Linguistics and Phonetics Lund University Abstract In this paper, results from two studies on articulation rate variation and prosodic phrasing are presented. Production data are presented that suggest a progressive articulation rate reduction over the course of the prosodic phrase in southern Swedish, and results from a perception experiment reveal the importance of articulation rate reduction for perceived boundary strength. Introduction Final lengthening is clearly visible in Swedish production data, and has also convincingly been shown to be an important cue in the perception of phrasing in standard Swedish (Bruce et al. 1992). However, the phonological and phonetic conventions of a standard variety are not always applicable to all varieties of a language, and there are several known differences in accent realization and accent distribution between the dialects of Sweden (e.g. between southern Swedish and the much more studied so-called standard variety of Swedish) that may have implications for the phrasing strategies used. The aim of the present paper is to shed some light on prosodic phrasing in southern Swedish. Southern Swedish share many prosodic properties with Danish, a language that has been claimed to lack phrase-final lengthening in some dialects (Grønnum Thorsen 1988). Like Danish, southern Swedish has no (high) phrase accent, the high turning point that in standard Swedish follows the word accent fall in focal and phrasefinal positions. Lyberg (1981) has suggested that the phrase-final lengthening phenomenon in Swedish is related to this characteristic phrase end contour of the fundamental frequency, the rise-fall gesture after the last word accent. In southern Swedish, there is no such gesture after the last word accent and there would therefore not be a need for slowing down the articulation rate phrase-finally. Secondly, like in Danish, there is no so-called default sentence accent (focal accent) at the end of the phrase in southern Swedish. The most prominent accent is not always found on the last word of the phrase. Even assuming that focal accentuation in itself (regardless of the tonal gesture associated with it) results in final lengthening, there is no reason to believe that final lengthening is obligatory in the south Swedish phrase. The research questions addressed in the present paper concern the articulation rate variation within the prosodic phrase in southern Swedish. Can a reduction of the articulation rate be observed over the course of the prosodic phrase in southern Swedish, and if so, has the variation in articulation rate any effect on the perceived boundary strength of prosodic phrase boundaries in southern Swedish? Methods and materials The production data study The speech material used is from the SweDia 2 database (Bruce et al. 1999). The speech of four female speakers (two from the younger generation of speakers recorded, and two from the older generation) and two male speakers (from the older generation) from four of the recording locations in Skåne has been analyzed. The first 1 prosodic phrases in the six speakers spontaneous recordings were initially chosen for the analysis. 447 of these were phrases without phrase internal pauses or fillers. Disfluent phrases were excluded since they may contain segmental lengthening associated with another domain than the prosodic phrase (see Dankovičová 1997). For the analysis of the articulation rate variation within the prosodic phrase, we followed Dankovičová 1997 and measured and compared the articulation rate (in syllables per second) in each prosodic word in the prosodic phrase. Swedish is not a language with a fixed stress position, but the by far most common 173

Speech, Music and Hearing stress pattern contains an initial stress. Therefore, the stressed syllables served as landmarks in the segmentation of the phrases into words. Only in phrase-initial position were unaccented syllables attached to the following accented syllable instead of the preceding. In counting the number of syllables in each prosodic word, care was taken to count the actual number of syllables, rather than the numbers of syllables the word contains in its citation form. The perception experiment The purpose of the perception experiment was to relate three known cues for prosodic phrasing (pausing, F reset and reduced articulation rate in the final part of the phrase) to perceived boundary strength. For this purpose, another 5 short speech fragments were chosen from the SweDia 2 recordings from Skåne (spontaneous speech from five female and five male speakers). All speech fragments (typically one or two utterances long) contain at least one prosodic phrase boundary. In the test, the listeners were presented with an orthographic transcription of the utterance(s) in which the prosodic boundary of interest was marked with a /. Their task was to indicate how strongly marked they perceived the indicated boundary to be in the recording. They indicated the boundaries strength on a computer-based Visual Analogue Scale (VAS). The VAS has been used in the measurement of clinical phenomena (such as pain) since the 192 s (Wewers and Lowe 199), but it is suitable for measuring a variety of subjective non-clinical phenomena as well. The subjects respond by placing a mark at a position that represents their current perception of a given phenomenon (in this case a given boundary s strength) between the labeled extremes of the 1 mm scale, see Figure 1. Figure 1. In the computer program Judge (Granqvist 1996), the listeners rated the strength of given prosodic phrase boundaries by adjusting a scrollbar. The VAS is then scored by measuring the distance (in mm) from one end of the scale to the subject s mark on the line. So far, only ten subjects have completed the perception test, and therefore the results presented below have to be considered preliminary. Results Articulation rate reduction in production There were 323 prosodic phrases containing more than one prosodic word in the material (134 2-word phrases, 118 3-word phrases, 5 4- word phrases, 18 5-word phrases and 3 6-word phrases). Due to the low number of 5- and 6- word phrases in the material, mainly those phrases containing 2 to 4 words have been analyzed. As a first step in the analysis of the data, we chose to rank order the words in the phrases according to their articulation rate. As shown in Figure 2, 113 (84%) of the 2-word phrases show an AB pattern, i.e. a reduction of the articulation rate where the first word (A) is articulated with a higher articulation rate than the second and final word (B). All speakers but one use the AB pattern more frequently than the BA pattern. Speaker 3 uses the BA pattern as often as the AB pattern. Number of phrases 4 3 2 1 1 2 3 4 5 6 Speaker AB BA Figure 2. Ordinal patterns in 2-word phrases. As shown in Figure 3, 59 (44%) of the 3-word phrases demonstrate an ABC pattern, i.e. a progressive slowing down. Another 36 phrases (27%) show a reduction of the articulation rate that is observable only in the comparison of the articulation rates in the second and third word, i.e. a BAC or CAB pattern. No phrase demonstrates a CBA pattern, i.e. a pattern in which the articulation rate is gradually increasing. 174

TMH-QPSR Vol. 44 Fonetik 22 Number of phrases 8 6 4 2 ABC BAC ACB CAB BCA CBA Ordinal pattern Figure 3. Ordinal patterns in 3-word phrases. The most common pattern in the 4-word phrases is the ABCD pattern (13 of 5), i.e. a gradual decrease in articulation rate over the course of the phrase. The ACBD, BACD and CBAD patterns are also relatively common (16 of 5). In the BACD the reduction of the articulation rate is visible in the last three words, and in the ACBD and CBAD patterns in the two last words. It is also clear that in the majority of the 4-word phrases, it is either the first or the second word in the phrase that is pronounced the fastest (in 38 cases of 5), and that the phrase-final word is either the word with the lowest or second lowest articulation rate in the phrase (in 41 cases of 5) The reduction of the articulation rate over the course of the phrase is also observable in the mean articulation rates in the material, as shown in Tables 1 and 2. Table 1. Mean articulation rate (in syllables/ second) and standard deviations for the words in 2- and 3-word phrases (all speakers). 2-word phrases 3-word phrases Mean SD Mean SD 1 st word 6.8 2.1 6.8 1.8 2 nd word 4.4 2. 6. 2.2 3 rd word 4.2 1.4 Table 2. Mean articulation rate (in syllables/ second) and standard deviations for the words in 4- and 5-word phrases (all speakers). 4-word phrases 5-word phrases Mean SD Mean SD 1 st word 7. 2.4 7.2 1.7 2 nd word 6.1 1.8 6.3 1.7 3 rd word 6. 3. 6.2 1.8 4 th word 3.7 1.1 5.5 1.5 5 th word 4.6 1.7 Typically, the articulation rate in the phraseinitial words is about 7 syllables per second and in the final word about 4 to 5 syllables per second. In order to test if a word s position in the phrase has an effect on its articulation rate, a GLM (general linear modeling) procedure was used. The dependent variable was articulation rate, and the two factors were position (word s position in phrase) and speaker. Each phrase type (the 2-, 3- and 4-word phrase) was analyzed separately. The factor of position was significant for all phrases: 2-word phrases (F(1, 256)=87.6, p<.1), 3-word phrases (F(2, 336)=67., p<.1) and 4-word phrases (F(3, 176)=22.4, p<.1). A posthoc Tukey revealed that the mean articulation rate of all three words were significantly different from each other at the.1 level in the 3-word phrases. In the 4-word phrases, on the other hand, significant differences in mean articulation rate were found only in the comparisons of the mean articulation rates in words 1 and 4, 2 and 4, and 3 and 4 (p <.1). In other words, the only significant difference in articulation rate between two successive words was found in the comparison between the final and penultimate word. The factor of speaker was significant at the.1 level in the 3-word phrases (F(5, 336)=5.5, p<.1), although the only speaker that demonstrated a significantly different mean articulation rate was speaker 3 (significantly different from all other speakers ). The two-way interaction position by speaker was significant at the.1 level only in the 2-word phrases (F(5, 256)=6.1, p<.1), indicating that the speakers were not, in general, different in their patterns of articulation rate. Articulation rate reduction in perception Two thirds of the prosodic phrase boundaries in the perception experiment (33 of 5) were associated with pauses (a silent interval) ranging in duration from 13 ms to 286 ms. The F resets across the phrase boundaries ranged from a 19 Hz large reset of F to a lowering of F across the phrase boundary of 51 Hz. If expressed in percentages of the speakers F ranges (measured as the difference in F between the highest and the lowest F value in the recording), the F resets ranged from a reset of 94% to a lowering of 76%. The normalized difference in articulation rate (the difference was divided by the mean articulation rate in the two words being compared) ranged from 1.3 (corresponding to a change in articulation rate 175

Speech, Music and Hearing from 7.7 syllables/second to 1.8 syllables/ second) to.3 (corresponding to a change from 4.2 syllables/second to 5.4 syllables/second). None of the three cues investigated are related to each other. There is no statistically significant correlation between the duration of the pause, the size of the F reset and/or the change in articulation rate. Apparently speakers do not maximize the use of several cues simultaneously to increase the strength of a boundary. This finding is partly consistent with the trading relation between pause duration and final lengthening reported on in Horne, Strangert and Heldner 1995. Statistical analyses revealed that the listeners in the perception experiment agree very well in their perceptual judgments. The Pearson correlation coefficient of each pairwise combination of listeners was significant at the.1 level. Therefore, we have chosen to follow Sanderman (1996) and pool the scores of the listeners for each boundary and calculate a mean to obtain an estimate of each boundary s perceived strength (in mm). Possible relationships between the perceived boundary strength and the cues for prosodic phrasing can then be investigated. The correlation between pause duration and perceived boundary strength proved very strong (r=.9, r 2 =.81). As shown in Figure 4, the longer the pause is, the stronger the listeners perceived the boundary. The only clear exceptions are pauses longer than 2 seconds, which were not perceived stronger than pauses of 1.5 to 2 seconds. 1 PBS (mm) 8 6 4 2 1 2 Pause duration (ms) 3 Figure 4. Scatterplot demonstrating the relationship between pause duration and perceived boundary strength (PBS). Attempts made to relate the pauses duration to the speakers speaking rate did not strengthen the correlation further. Finally, no statistically significant correlation was found between change in articulation rate and perceived boundary strength, or between F reset and perceived boundary strength. Conclusions The results presented in this paper show that the articulation rate in south Swedish phrases is significantly lower in phrase-final words than in preceding words, i.e. that some sort of phrasefinal lengthening exists in southern Swedish. The results also indicate that the reduction in articulation rate between successive words in the prosodic phrase is not restricted to the final part of the phrase. However, preliminary results from a perception experiment suggest that the amount of articulation rate reduction has no effect on the perceived strength of prosodic phrase boundaries. References Bruce G, Elert C-C, Engstrand O, Eriksson A and Wretling P (1999). Database tools for a prosodic analysis of the Swedish dialects. Proceedings Fonetik 99. The Swedish Phonetics Conference. June 2-4 1999. Gothenburg Papers in Theoretical Linguistics 81: 37-4. Bruce G, Granström B, Gustafson K and House D (1992). Interaction of F and duration in the perception of prosodic phrasing in Swedish. Nordic Prosody VI. Papers from a Symposium, Stockholm, August 12-14 1992, 7-22. Dankovičová J (1997). The domain of articulation rate variation in Czech. Journal of Phonetics 25: 287-312. Granqvist S (1996). Enhancements to the Visual Analogue Scale, VAS, for listening tests. TMH- QPSR, 4/1996: 61-62. Grønnum Thorsen N (1988). Intonation on Bornholm Between Danish and Swedish. ANNUAL REPORT of the Institute of Phonetics, University of Copenhagen, 25-138. Horne M, Strangert E and Heldner M (1995). Prosodic Boundary Strength in Swedish: Final Lengthening and Silent Interval Duration. Proceedings of The XVIIIth International Congress of Phonetic Sciences ICPhS 95. Stockholm, Sweden. 13-19 August, 1995, 1: 17-173. Lyberg B (1981). Some consequences of a model for segment duration based on F-dependence. Journal of Phonetics 9: 97-13. Sanderman A (1996). Prosodic phrasing. Production, perception, acceptability and comprehension. Doctoral dissertation. Eindhoven University of Technology. Wewers M E and Lowe N K (199). A Critical Review of Visual Analogue Scales in the Measurement of Clinical Phenomena. Research in Nursing & Health, 13: 227-236. 176