Center for Advanced Language Proficiency Education and Research Position paper October 2007

Similar documents
Candidates must achieve a grade of at least C2 level in each examination in order to achieve the overall qualification at C2 Level.

Lower and Upper Secondary

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 141 ( 2014 ) WCLTA Using Corpus Linguistics in the Development of Writing

5. UPPER INTERMEDIATE

VOCABULARY INSTRUCTION

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

Text and task authenticity in the EFL classroom

CEFR Overall Illustrative English Proficiency Scales

CELTA. Syllabus and Assessment Guidelines. Third Edition. University of Cambridge ESOL Examinations 1 Hills Road Cambridge CB1 2EU United Kingdom

Language Acquisition Chart

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 154 ( 2014 )

Copyright Corwin 2015

Intermediate Academic Writing

LITERACY ACROSS THE CURRICULUM POLICY

AN INTRODUCTION (2 ND ED.) (LONDON, BLOOMSBURY ACADEMIC PP. VI, 282)

English Language and Applied Linguistics. Module Descriptions 2017/18

Introduction to the Common European Framework (CEF)

Intensive Writing Class

and secondary sources, attending to such features as the date and origin of the information.

The Common European Framework of Reference for Languages p. 58 to p. 82

The Effect of Extensive Reading on Developing the Grammatical. Accuracy of the EFL Freshmen at Al Al-Bayt University

International Conference on Education and Educational Psychology (ICEEPSY 2012)

Aviation English Training: How long Does it Take?

Metacognitive Strategies that Enhance Reading Comprehension in the Foreign Language University Classroom

Graduate Program in Education

Review in ICAME Journal, Volume 38, 2014, DOI: /icame

LANGUAGE IN INDIA Strength for Today and Bright Hope for Tomorrow Volume 11 : 12 December 2011 ISSN

Learning and Retaining New Vocabularies: The Case of Monolingual and Bilingual Dictionaries

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Publisher Citations. Program Description. Primary Supporting Y N Universal Access: Teacher s Editions Adjust on the Fly all grades:

Motivation to e-learn within organizational settings: What is it and how could it be measured?

FOREWORD.. 5 THE PROPER RUSSIAN PRONUNCIATION. 8. УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) 4 80.

Lexical Collocations (Verb + Noun) Across Written Academic Genres In English

Corpus Linguistics (L615)

Perception of Lecturer on Intercultural Competence and Culture Teaching Time (Case Study)

BULATS A2 WORDLIST 2

OVERVIEW OF CURRICULUM-BASED MEASUREMENT AS A GENERAL OUTCOME MEASURE

Using Moodle in ESOL Writing Classes

Second Language Acquisition in Adults: From Research to Practice

Summary results (year 1-3)

The Effect of Written Corrective Feedback on the Accuracy of English Article Usage in L2 Writing

EQuIP Review Feedback

This Performance Standards include four major components. They are

Arizona s English Language Arts Standards th Grade ARIZONA DEPARTMENT OF EDUCATION HIGH ACADEMIC STANDARDS FOR STUDENTS

Multimedia, Subtitles, and Native Language Vocabulary Acquisition

Philosophy of Literacy. on a daily basis. My students will be motivated, fluent, and flexible because I will make my reading

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

Author: Justyna Kowalczys Stowarzyszenie Angielski w Medycynie (PL) Feb 2015

Is There a Role for Tutor in Group Work: Peer Interaction in a Hong Kong EFL Classroom

21st Century Community Learning Center

Syntactic and Lexical Simplification: The Impact on EFL Listening Comprehension at Low and High Language Proficiency Levels

10.2. Behavior models

Advanced Grammar in Use

Project in the framework of the AIM-WEST project Annotation of MWEs for translation

Missouri Mathematics Grade-Level Expectations

RED 3313 Language and Literacy Development course syllabus Dr. Nancy Marshall Associate Professor Reading and Elementary Education

TESL/TESOL DIPLOMA PROGRAMS VIA TESL/TESOL Diploma Programs are recognized by TESL CANADA

The Effect of Discourse Markers on the Speaking Production of EFL Students. Iman Moradimanesh

To appear in The TESOL encyclopedia of ELT (Wiley-Blackwell) 1 RECASTING. Kazuya Saito. Birkbeck, University of London

The History of Language Teaching

Linguistic Variation across Sports Category of Press Reportage from British Newspapers: a Diachronic Multidimensional Analysis

Monitoring Metacognitive abilities in children: A comparison of children between the ages of 5 to 7 years and 8 to 11 years

A THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK FORA TASK-BASED SYLLABUS FOR PRIMARY SCHOOLS IN SOUTH AFRICA

Information for Candidates

Stefan Engelberg (IDS Mannheim), Workshop Corpora in Lexical Research, Bucharest, Nov [Folie 1] 6.1 Type-token ratio

South Carolina English Language Arts

A survey of university students self-reflections on English register awareness

Running head: LISTENING COMPREHENSION OF UNIVERSITY REGISTERS 1

Student Name: OSIS#: DOB: / / School: Grade:

Foreign Languages. Foreign Languages, General

Investigating the Effectiveness of the Uses of Electronic and Paper-Based Dictionaries in Promoting Incidental Word Learning

Degree Qualification Profiles Intellectual Skills

Age Effects on Syntactic Control in. Second Language Learning

COMMUNICATIVE LANGUAGE TEACHING

Assessing Functional Relations: The Utility of the Standard Celeration Chart

Think A F R I C A when assessing speaking. C.E.F.R. Oral Assessment Criteria. Think A F R I C A - 1 -

The Potential of Corpus-Informed L2 Pedagogy. Jonathon Reinhardt University of Arizona

Why PPP won t (and shouldn t) go away

Assessing speaking skills:. a workshop for teacher development. Ben Knight

ESL Curriculum and Assessment

Criterion Met? Primary Supporting Y N Reading Street Comprehensive. Publisher Citations

INTERNATIONAL BACCALAUREATE AT IVANHOE GRAMMAR SCHOOL. An Introduction to the International Baccalaureate Diploma Programme For Students and Families

DOES RETELLING TECHNIQUE IMPROVE SPEAKING FLUENCY?

Welcome to MyOutcomes Online, the online course for students using Outcomes Elementary, in the classroom.

Effect of Word Complexity on L2 Vocabulary Learning

Physics 270: Experimental Physics

Algebra 1, Quarter 3, Unit 3.1. Line of Best Fit. Overview

Taking into Account the Oral-Written Dichotomy of the Chinese language :

TAIWANESE STUDENT ATTITUDES TOWARDS AND BEHAVIORS DURING ONLINE GRAMMAR TESTING WITH MOODLE

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 143 ( 2014 ) CY-ICER Teacher intervention in the process of L2 writing acquisition

Extending Place Value with Whole Numbers to 1,000,000

DISCLAIMER: This document does not meet the current format guidelines of. the Graduate School at. The University of Texas at Austin.

MSW POLICY, PLANNING & ADMINISTRATION (PP&A) CONCENTRATION

World Languages Unpacked Content for Classical Language Programs What is the purpose of this document?

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

On document relevance and lexical cohesion between query terms

CONSULTATION ON THE ENGLISH LANGUAGE COMPETENCY STANDARD FOR LICENSED IMMIGRATION ADVISERS

The Effects of Strategic Planning and Topic Familiarity on Iranian Intermediate EFL Learners Written Performance in TBLT

Reviewed by Florina Erbeli

*Net Perceptions, Inc West 78th Street Suite 300 Minneapolis, MN

Transcription:

1 Center for Advanced Language Proficiency Education and Research Position paper October 2007 Assessing Development of Advanced Proficiency Through Learner Corpora by Michael McCarthy CALPER Corpus Project, Director In the past several decades, the use of language corpora has resulted in considerable advances in our understanding of the quantitative aspects of languages. In the case of English, for instance, most corpora show that around 2,000 word-forms are used very frequently in everyday spoken and written language (O Keeffe et al, 2007). These word-forms may be said to represent the core of the lexicon. Equally, by measuring how frequent and how widely distributed grammatical structures are in general corpora, we can arrive at a set of core grammatical structures. In the case of the lexicon, the remaining, non-core vocabulary occurs with relatively low frequency but is massive in size (between 30-50,000 word-forms being in use in everyday talk, and considerably more in everyday written texts, perhaps up to 80,000 in the case of English). It would seem non-controversial to suggest that, in language acquisition, the core lexicon and grammar represent a clear target for the elementary level and lower-intermediate level. Language teaching programs have now begun to acknowledge the role of corpora in the definition of these levels; for example, the Touchstone adult English language learning series (McCarthy et al, 2004-2006) uses spoken and written corpora to define all four levels of its syllabus from elementary to lower intermediate, as well as evidence from learner corpora. The advanced level represents a different problem which in the end takes us beyond quantification. In the case of the lexicon, the long tail of non-core vocabulary is simply too big to learn virtually everything is low frequency, making sub-division and syllabus organisation difficult. It may be possible to say at what point a learner could be said to enter the advanced level in terms of number of words (perhaps after 6,000 words have been acquired), but little more can be said in terms of quantity of vocabulary. After the intermediate level, there remains an enormous number of words to be learnt, too many to teach and practise in any secondary or

2 tertiary language program. The grammar is a more finite entity and advanced, in the sense of low-frequency structures, can be delimited and listed in a fairly exhaustive way. However, what corpora crucially show us is that advanced language proficiency cannot be a simple matter of measuring quantity of vocabulary and grammar known. The evidence of corpora shows that what is at stake in the low-frequency zones is more to do with new kinds of understandings, understandings not only of what is to be learnt, but what one needs to learn about what is to be learnt, and what kinds of learning approaches are most effective for such an open-ended task. The advanced level, therefore, becomes not a teleologic endeavour, but a particular type of development which can be traced over time. Quantitative features of advanced language proficiency The purely quantitative aspects of language learning at the advanced level can be represented through frequency lists for both the grammar and the lexicon. For instance, a corpus frequency list can show the increments in text coverage (i.e. comprehensibility using one s receptive vocabulary) offered by adding further 2,000-word bands to the core 2,000. Unfortunately, the leaps required to get to 97% (expert-user level) coverage are not evenly spaced. A 6,000-word upper intermediate vocabulary offers 91% comprehension of typical English texts. Adding another 4,000 words (from the 6,000 to 10,000 word level) accounts for only a 3% gain in coverage, and the next 6,000-word increment (from the 10,000 to 16,000 word level, not shown in the graph) only brings with it a meagre 2% gain, and so on. Nation (2001:147-8) argues that for full, pleasurable engagement with the meaning of a text, comprehension in the region of 97-98% must be the desired threshold, which would seem to be beyond the grasp of most learners in most language programs, requiring a receptive vocabulary of much greater than 10,000 words. And yet many learners achieve impressive, expert-user levels of knowledge and fluency in all four language skills, either through intensive learning or through periods spent in the target-language environment. Clearly there is more at stake than simply how many words one knows. On the evidence of corpora, much advanced level vocabulary acquisition will be concerned with less frequent, extended and metaphorical senses of words, and the creation of new relationships among words. The expansion of associations and the forging of new networks is seen as a central aspect of being an advanced learner or user by researchers such as Wolter

3 (2001; 2002), and Wilks and Meara (2002). Additionally, single-word frequency lists alone do not tell the whole story, and must be weighed alongside the frequency of fixed expressions as evidenced in all corpora. Many fixed expressions equal or exceed in frequency the single-word forms. The ability to retrieve a repertoire of fixed expressions is the hallmark of fluency and must be counted as a central feature of advanced proficiency. Corpora also show that spokenwritten differences may be wider and more important at the advanced level. For example, in the case of English syntax, subject ellipsis is of low frequency generally, and especially in written texts (so rendering it outside of the core, high-frequency structures taught at elementary and intermediate levels) but is much more common in informal conversations. Sensitivity to such register- and genre-based distinctions would seem to be a key element of advanced proficiency. Another characteristic of words in the low frequency bands is that they seem less capable of innocent, neutral use, and a great deal of focus will necessarily be on the connotations of words in their typical contexts of occurrence, over and above grappling with semantic issues. The connotations of words and their characteristic environments of use (their semantic prosody, as it has been called; see Sinclair, 1991) seem to be more foregrounded. Qualitative features: Breadth versus depth Corpus evidence suggests that the quest for an ever larger and larger vocabulary reflects a rather one-dimensional view of advanced level achievement. A focus simply on linear increase in vocabulary size (or vocabulary breadth as it is often termed) produces diminishing returns as far as text coverage is concerned. What needs to happen alongside the increase in breadth is an increase in depth of knowledge, i.e. the knowledge of the various aspects of use of a word, including, beyond its formal properties, its collocations, its sub-senses, and its semantic prosody. Such knowledge ultimately contributes to the learner s ability to create associations between words and to place them meaningfully within various networks in relation to other words (Haastrup & Henriksen, 2000; Henriksen, 1999; Meara, 1996). Depth of knowledge is not simply a second-best to ever-increasing breadth: Qian (2002), for instance, found that vocabulary depth was as significant as vocabulary size in predicting performance on academic reading. And since the vocabulary learning task is open-ended and impossible to complete in a typical institutional program, the implication is that the advanced level should also be defined by the extent to which the learner is able to operate independently

4 with a set of skills and strategies for processing new vocabulary at this level. Such a learner may not have a massive vocabulary but may be better equipped to use and explore the vocabulary of the target language than one who simply adds more and more words without building an integrated lexicon and without developing that learner agency so often discussed in sociocultural theory (Lantolf and Appel, 1994), which can enable the learner to surpass instructional intervention and become a better, self-regulated learner. Conclusion To develop awareness and skills that will stand the learner in good stead for becoming an autonomous vocabulary-learner is a question of developing activities alongside the actual learning of words which introduce to the learner notions such as collocation, metaphor, connotation, etc. For example, in the case of English, many learners have an awareness of idioms of the verb+complement type (hit the sack, carry the can, jump on the bandwagon), but probably few are aware of the pervasiveness in everyday language of binomial idioms (rough and ready, part and parcel, out and about, down and out). Explicit focus on such items may be necessary to tune the learner s antennae to be receptive to new ones, and to foster learner agency and independence. Vocabulary skills include ways of maximising learning opportunities during interaction (e.g. asking for paraphrases, probing the meaning of unfamiliar items with one s interlocutor, etc.). The advanced level learner will not be defined only by his/her vocabulary size or absolute coverage of all syntactic patterns vis-à-vis native speakers, but rather more by his/her ability to develop depth of knowledge and the tools and strategies to pursue vocabulary learning independently. With a combination of corpus-based research and the pursuit of strategic training for learners who will have to complete the task for themselves, we may go a long way towards defining advanced language proficiency in terms of traceable development on axes other than the purely quantitative increase of acquired structures and words. References Haastrup, K., & Henriksen, B. (2000). Vocabulary acquisition: Acquiring depth of knowledge through network building. International Journal of Applied

5 Linguistics 10(2): 221-240. Henriksen, B. (1999) Three dimensions of vocabulary development. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 21: 303-317. Lantolf, J. P. and Appel, G. (eds.) (1994) Vygotskian approaches to second language research. Norwood, NJ: Ablex. McCarthy, M. J., McCarten, J. and Sandiford, H. (2005-2006) Touchstone. Student s Books 1-4. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Meara, P. (1996). The dimensions of lexical competence. In G. Brown, K. Malmkjaer & J. Williams (Eds.), Performance and competence in second language acquisition (pp. 35-53). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Nation, I. S. P. (2001) Learning Vocabulary in Another Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. O Keeffe, A., McCarthy, M. J. & Carter, R. A. (2007) From Corpus to Classroom. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Qian, D. D. (2002) Investigating the relationship between vocabulary knowledge and academic reading performance: An assessment perspective. Language Learning, 52 (3): 513-536. Wilks C. and Meara P. (2002) Untangling word webs: graph theory and the notion of density in second language word association networks. Second Language Research 18 (4): 303-324. Wolter, B. (2001) Comparing the L1 and L2 mental lexicon: a depth of individual word knowledge model. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 23(1): 41-69. Wolter, B. (2002) Assessing proficiency through word associations: is there still hope? System 30(3): 315-329. The Center for Advanced Language Proficiency Education and Research (CALPER) at the Pennsylvania State University is one of 15 National Language Resource Centers funded by the U.S. Department of Education (CFDA 84.229 P229A060003).