The Behaviours of the General Nasal /N/in Indonesian Active Prefixed Verbs

Similar documents
The analysis starts with the phonetic vowel and consonant charts based on the dataset:

Pobrane z czasopisma New Horizons in English Studies Data: 18/11/ :52:20. New Horizons in English Studies 1/2016

Consonants: articulation and transcription

Underlying Representations

Universal contrastive analysis as a learning principle in CAPT

The Perception of Nasalized Vowels in American English: An Investigation of On-line Use of Vowel Nasalization in Lexical Access

Lexical phonology. Marc van Oostendorp. December 6, Until now, we have presented phonological theory as if it is a monolithic

Markedness and Complex Stops: Evidence from Simplification Processes 1. Nick Danis Rutgers University

Phonological Processing for Urdu Text to Speech System

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

**Note: this is slightly different from the original (mainly in format). I would be happy to send you a hard copy.**

Books Effective Literacy Y5-8 Learning Through Talk Y4-8 Switch onto Spelling Spelling Under Scrutiny

Linguistics 220 Phonology: distributions and the concept of the phoneme. John Alderete, Simon Fraser University

Phonology Revisited: Sor3ng Out the PH Factors in Reading and Spelling Development. Indiana, November, 2015

On the nature of voicing assimilation(s)

Language Acquisition by Identical vs. Fraternal SLI Twins * Karin Stromswold & Jay I. Rifkin

DOWNSTEP IN SUPYIRE* Robert Carlson Societe Internationale de Linguistique, Mali

Parallel Evaluation in Stratal OT * Adam Baker University of Arizona

CS224d Deep Learning for Natural Language Processing. Richard Socher, PhD

Derivational and Inflectional Morphemes in Pak-Pak Language

Phonetics. The Sound of Language

Prevalence of Oral Reading Problems in Thai Students with Cleft Palate, Grades 3-5

Contrastiveness and diachronic variation in Chinese nasal codas. Tsz-Him Tsui The Ohio State University

English Language and Applied Linguistics. Module Descriptions 2017/18

Demonstration of problems of lexical stress on the pronunciation Turkish English teachers and teacher trainees by computer

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. VCV-sequencies in a preliminary text-to-speech system for female speech

First Grade Curriculum Highlights: In alignment with the Common Core Standards

CLASSIFICATION OF PROGRAM Critical Elements Analysis 1. High Priority Items Phonemic Awareness Instruction

Large Kindergarten Centers Icons

SEGMENTAL FEATURES IN SPONTANEOUS AND READ-ALOUD FINNISH

Contrasting English Phonology and Nigerian English Phonology

ELA/ELD Standards Correlation Matrix for ELD Materials Grade 1 Reading

have to be modeled) or isolated words. Output of the system is a grapheme-tophoneme conversion system which takes as its input the spelling of words,

DEVELOPING ENGLISH MATERIALS FOR THE SECOND GRADE STUDENTS OF MARITIME VOCATIONAL SCHOOL

To appear in the Proceedings of the 35th Meetings of the Chicago Linguistics Society. Post-vocalic spirantization: Typology and phonetic motivations

English for Life. B e g i n n e r. Lessons 1 4 Checklist Getting Started. Student s Book 3 Date. Workbook. MultiROM. Test 1 4

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

ABSTRACT. Some children with speech sound disorders (SSD) have difficulty with literacyrelated

A Neural Network GUI Tested on Text-To-Phoneme Mapping

Coast Academies Writing Framework Step 4. 1 of 7

DIBELS Next BENCHMARK ASSESSMENTS

Holy Family Catholic Primary School SPELLING POLICY

Word Stress and Intonation: Introduction

THE PHONOLOGICAL WORD IN STANDARD MALA Y

DEVELOPING A PROTOTYPE OF SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL FOR VOCABULARY FOR THE THIRD GRADERS OF ELEMENTARY SCHOOLS

1 st Quarter (September, October, November) August/September Strand Topic Standard Notes Reading for Literature

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Voiced-voiceless distinction in alaryngeal speech - acoustic and articula

IMPROVING SPEAKING SKILL OF THE TENTH GRADE STUDENTS OF SMK 17 AGUSTUS 1945 MUNCAR THROUGH DIRECT PRACTICE WITH THE NATIVE SPEAKER

source or where they are needed to distinguish two forms of a language. 4. Geographical Location. I have attempted to provide a geographical

TEKS Comments Louisiana GLE

DEVELOPMENT OF LINGUAL MOTOR CONTROL IN CHILDREN AND ADOLESCENTS

Multilingual Speech Data Collection for the Assessment of Pronunciation and Prosody in a Language Learning System

On the Formation of Phoneme Categories in DNN Acoustic Models

Linguistic Variation across Sports Category of Press Reportage from British Newspapers: a Diachronic Multidimensional Analysis

LING 329 : MORPHOLOGY

CORRECT YOUR ENGLISH ERRORS BY TIM COLLINS DOWNLOAD EBOOK : CORRECT YOUR ENGLISH ERRORS BY TIM COLLINS PDF

Constraining X-Bar: Theta Theory

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Florida Reading Endorsement Alignment Matrix Competency 1

CROSS-LANGUAGE MAPPING FOR SMALL-VOCABULARY ASR IN UNDER-RESOURCED LANGUAGES: INVESTIGATING THE IMPACT OF SOURCE LANGUAGE CHOICE

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

Primary English Curriculum Framework

The ABCs of O-G. Materials Catalog. Skills Workbook. Lesson Plans for Teaching The Orton-Gillingham Approach in Reading and Spelling

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

Effect of Cognitive Apprenticeship Instructional Method on Auto-Mechanics Students

REVIEW OF CONNECTED SPEECH

Handout #8. Neutralization

The Structure of Relative Clauses in Maay Maay By Elly Zimmer

Houghton Mifflin Reading Correlation to the Common Core Standards for English Language Arts (Grade1)

Radical CV Phonology: the locational gesture *

A Fact in Historical Phonology from the Viewpoint of Generative Phonology: The Underlying Schwa in Old English

Guidelines for Writing an Internship Report

Keynounce. A Game for Pronunciation Generation through Crowdsourcing

IS SABAH MALAY A REAL LANGUAGE? By: Jane Wong Kon Ling, Ph.D Centre for the Promotion of Knowledge and Language Learning Universiti Malaysia Sabah

In closed syllables the /e/ is short, [e] and is pronounced like the e in the word bet. menteri minister /men.te.ri/ pendek short /pen.

Sample Goals and Benchmarks

Using a Native Language Reference Grammar as a Language Learning Tool

Laporan Penelitian Unggulan Prodi

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 143 ( 2014 ) CY-ICER Teacher intervention in the process of L2 writing acquisition

The Implementation of Interactive Multimedia Learning Materials in Teaching Listening Skills

SIE: Speech Enabled Interface for E-Learning

On Developing Acoustic Models Using HTK. M.A. Spaans BSc.

AN ANALYSIS OF THE FIRST YEAR STUDENTS ABILITY TO COMPREHEND NEWS ITEM TEXT AT SMAN 7 PADANG.

UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report (2014) What (else) Depends on Phonology?

Entrepreneurial Discovery and the Demmert/Klein Experiment: Additional Evidence from Germany

Derivational: Inflectional: In a fit of rage the soldiers attacked them both that week, but lost the fight.

Speech Segmentation Using Probabilistic Phonetic Feature Hierarchy and Support Vector Machines

The Indian English of Tibeto-Burman language speakers*

Reading Horizons. A Look At Linguistic Readers. Nicholas P. Criscuolo APRIL Volume 10, Issue Article 5

Opportunities for Writing Title Key Stage 1 Key Stage 2 Narrative

Consonant-Vowel Unity in Element Theory*

Morphotactics as Tier-Based Strictly Local Dependencies

Journal of Phonetics

Speech Recognition using Acoustic Landmarks and Binary Phonetic Feature Classifiers

What the National Curriculum requires in reading at Y5 and Y6

Lower and Upper Secondary

University of Indonesia

Author: Justyna Kowalczys Stowarzyszenie Angielski w Medycynie (PL) Feb 2015

Transcription:

The Behaviours of the General Nasal /N/in Indonesian Active Prefixed Verbs Sukarno English Department - Faculty Humanities University of Jember Indonesia Abstract This paper investigates how a general nasal /N/ in the morpheme of the active verb prefix /mǝn-/ in Indonesian behaves as it is combined with simple verb forms to produce the prefixed verbs. The data were mainly collected from an Indonesian standardized dictionary Kamus Besar Bahasa Indonesia and analysed phonologically. The research reveals that the general nasal /N/ most assimilates to the initial sound of the simple verb into labial /m/, alveolar /n/, palatal /ɲ/, and velar /ŋ/, and is deleted if it is followed by approximant (w, y), central (l), or retroflex (r). In contrast, the general nasal /N/ can delete a voiceless stop consonant (p, t, k). Next, to generate the correct surface representations from the underlying representations can be done by applying nasal assimilation and deletion rules. The right rule ordering is the nasal assimilation rule should be applied prior to the deletion rule. Apart from the regularity, it is also found a small number of data which do not follow these rules. Keywords: general nasal /N/, assimilation, deletion, distribution, underlying representation, surface representation 1.Introduction Indonesian (or Bahasa Indonesia) is one of western Austronesian languages which is officially used in the Republic of Indonesia. This language is spoken by more than 250 million speakers spread out all over Indonesia. It is used for many different purposes, such as for education, offices, trades and a means of communication among ethnic groups. Since language is mostly used to communicate orally, there have been many publications on the phonological aspects of Indonesian. Among of them are Peter (2001) who studied on the Austronesian nasal substitution and other NC effects, Sneddon (2006) who examined colloquial Jakarta Indonesian, and Andi- Pallawa and Alan (2013) who compared the phonological systems between English and Indonesian. Despite the fact that there are many preliminary researches on phonological aspects in Indonesian, researches on the behaviours of the general nasal /N/ in the Indonesian active verb prefixes have received comparatively less attention, if not yet done. In fact, the general nasal sound /N/ in the active verb prefixes are productively used in this language. In this article, I will describe the distribution of various allaphones as the realization of the general nasal /N/ according to the environments or contexts that govern the distributions, and present a set of phonological rules to explain the sound alterations. 2. Data The data of the research were mainly collected from a standardized dictionary Kamus Besar Bahasa Indonesia (Tim Pustaka Phoenix, 2010), and partly developed by the researcher who is a native speaker of Indonesian. A variety of words with the target of the nasal sounds in the active verb prefixes which occur in diverse environments were needed to meet the distributions of the allophones. The following table presents the data to depict how the behaviours of the general nasal /N/ as an Indonesian active verb prefix when it is combined with the simple verb forms (roots) to produce the prefixed forms. 48

ISSN 2374-8850 (Print), 2374-8869 (Online) Center for Promoting Ideas, USA www.ijllnet.com Table 1: Indonesian Active Verb Prefixes N-prefix + (Simple forms) Prefixed Forms Gloss 1. / mən + bəli / / məmbəli / to purchase 2. /mən + pakai / / məmakai / to use or to dress 3. / mən + pukul / / məmukul / to hit 4. / mən + fasilitas / / məmfasilitasi / to fisilate 5. / mən + tulis / / mənulis / to write 6. / mən + dapat / / məndapat / to get 7. / mən + dʒawab / / məndʒawab / to answer 8. / mən + sapu / / məɳapu / to sweep 9. / mən + siram / / məɳiram / to water 10. / mən + kunci / / məŋunci / to lock 11. / mən + guntiŋ / / məŋguntiŋ / to cut with scissors 12. / mən + hituŋ / / məŋhituŋ / to count 13. / mən + ambil / / məŋambil / to take 14. / mən + undaŋ / / məŋundaŋ / to invite 15. / mən + edʒa / / məŋedʒa / to spell 16. / mən + opər / / məŋopər / to pass over 17. / mən + isi / / məŋisi / to fill 18. / mən + wakil / / məwakili / to represent 19. / mən + yakin / / məyakini / to believe 20. / mən + lamar / / məlamar / to apply 21. / mən + rudʒuk / / mərudʒuk / to refer to 3. Analysis The data above (Table 1) demonstrate that the Indonesian active verb prefix is realized by a morpheme /mən-/ where /N/ is a general nasal. When the morpheme is attached to a simple verb form (a root), it may be realized into many different forms, such as: [m, n, ɲ, ŋ] or deleted [Ø] in the prefixed verbs. As the basic form, the general nasal /N/ can be claimed as a phoneme (Clark and Yallop, 1991: 125), and the realization of the phonemes, [m, n, ɳ, ŋ, Ø], are named allophones. The behaviours of the general nasal /N/ as a phoneme into its allophones can be explained through phonological rules. The first step producing the phonological rules is to build the distribution of the allophones of the phoneme according to the context which governs the distribution. Referring to Table 1, the distribution of the general nasal /N/ into its allophones can be presented in Table 2: The Distribution of Allophones Phoneme Allophones Distribution/context [m] [n] if it is followed by a labial sound (p, b, f), and they occur in different morphemes if it is followed by an alveolar stop sound (t, d) and they occur in different morphemes /N/ [ɳ] if it is followed by a voiceless stop sound (s) and they occur in different morphemes [ŋ] [Ø] if it is followed by a velar sound (k, g, h, vowel), and they occur in different morphemes. Elsewhere Second, following Hales and Clements (1983: 119), the general nasal /N/ as the basic form or the phoneme is called underlying representation (UR), and its allophone is named surface representation (SR). Accordingly, the general nasal /N/ is classified as the underlying representation (UR), while the surface representation (SR) is realized by the following allophones: [m, n, ɳ, ŋ, and Ø]. 49

Next, the realization of the allophones (SRs) from the same phoneme (UR) can be depicted in a phonological rule. Finegan, Besnier, Blair, and Collins (1992: 60), and Hales and Clements (1983: 119) state that the basic formulism of phonological rule can be written as: A B / C D (where A is the affected sound or the UR, B is the output or the SR, C and D are the context or the distribution, and the dash symbol ( ) representsthe location, where the affected sound exactly takes place). Furthermore, if the affected sound and thecontext occur in different morphemes, it is marked by a plus symbol (+). As much, the general phonological rule above can be rewritten in more specifically as: A B / + D or A B / C + In case of Indonesian, the general nasal /N/ as a phoneme mostly assimilates to the following sounds regarding to its place of articulations into bilabial nasal /m/, alveolar nasal /n/, palatal nasal / ɳ /, velar nasal /ŋ/, and it is also deleted (Ø). Therefore, the important rules to generate the SRs are nasal assimilation rule, deletion rule, and assimilation rule followed by deletion rule. 3.1 Nasal Assimilation Rule The general nasal /N/ mostly assimilates to the following sound according to its places of articulation. If the following sound is labial, alveolar, palatal, and velar, the general nasal /N/ will be assimilated into labial nasal [m], alveolar nasal [n], palatal nasal [ɲ], and velar nasal [ŋ] respectively. Based on this context, the nasal assimilation rule in Indonesian prefixed verbs can be written in the following rules: /N/ [m] / + (+ labial sounds, e.g. p, b, f) as shown by data no. 1, 2, 3, and 4. /N/ [n] / + (+alveolar, e.g. t, d, ʤ) as shown by data no. 5, 6, and 7. /N/ [ɳ] / + (+ a voiceless fricative alveolar /s/) as shown by data no. 8 and 9. /N/ [ŋ] / + (+ velar /k, g/, glottal /h/, vowel /a, u,e, o, i) as shown by data no.10, 11, 12, 13, 14, 15, 16, and 17. 3.2 Nasal Rule In addition to assimilate to the following sound, the general nasal /N/ will be deleted if it is followed by approximant /w, y/, lateral /l/, or retroflex /r/as demonstrated by the following rule: /N/[Ø] / + (+ approximant, lateral, retroflex), as shown by data no. 18, 19, 20, and 21. This rule says that delete a nasal sound if it is followed by an approximant, lateral, or retroflex. 3.3 Stop Rule The deletion rule does not only apply to nasal sound but it also applies to voiceless stop consonants /p, t, k/ if they are preceded by a nasal sound, as shown by data no. 2, 3, 5 and 10. This voiceless stop deletion rule can be written into the following phonological rule: (-Voice, -son, -cont) (Ø) / (+ Nasal) +.This rule dictates that every voiceless stop consonant is deleted if the consonant is preceded by a nasal, and they occur in different morphemes. 3.4 Assimilation and Rules Some data undergo more than one rule to generate the correct SRs. They are simple verb forms which begin with voiceless stop sounds /p, t, k/. Generating the correct SRs for these verbs should be done through two phonological rules. First, the nasal /N/ sound assimilates to the stop sounds into [m] data no. 2 and 3, [n] data no. 5, and [ŋ] data no. 10. The next process is the voiceless stop consonants are deleted (because they are preceded by a nasal sound). Having been proposed, the phonological rules can be applied to generate the correct SRs from the URs. if the rule does not affect the datain the process of derivation (see Table 3a and 3b), it is marked with NA meaning not applicable. Table 3a: Derivation (where deletion rules are not applicable) 50 Gloss to buy to get to cut to take

ISSN 2374-8850 (Print), 2374-8869 (Online) Center for Promoting Ideas, USA www.ijllnet.com URs /mən + bəli/ /mən + dapat/ /mən+guntiŋ/ /mən+ambil/ Nasal assimilation /məmbəli/ /məndapat/ /məŋguntiŋ/ /məŋambil/ Nasal Stop *NA NA NA NA NA NA NA NA SRs /məmbəli/ /məndapat/ /məŋguntiŋ/ /məŋambil/ *NA = not applicable Table 3a shows that a general nasal /N/ assimilates to the following sound (the initial sound of the simple verb) according to its place of articulation. Those data are not affected by deletion rules (NA). In the following table (Table 3b), the data assimilate to the initial position, but not the verb /mən+yakini/ to believe because it commences with an approximant /y/ in which the general nasal should be affected by the nasal deletion rule, whereas the others are not (NA). Finally, the voiceless stop sound rules deletes the initial sound of the simple verbs, but not the last data /mə+yakini/ because the nasal has been dropped. Table 3b: Derivation (where deletion rule(s) is applicable) Gloss to hit to write to water to beleive Urs: /mən + pukul/ /mən + tulis/ /mən+siram/ /mn+yakini/ Nasal assimilation /məmpukul/ /məntulis/ /məɳsiram/ NA Nasal NA NA NA /məyakini/ Stop /məmukul/ /mənulis/ /məɳiram/ NA SRs: /məmukul/ /mənulis/ /məɳiram/ /məyakini/ Since there are more than one phonological rule to generate the SRs from the URs, the ordering of the rules is crucial to pay attention, otherwise it will result the incorrect SR. The reason is that one rule may suck another rule which will make the rule does not function anymore as demonstrated in Table 3c, in which the incorrect SRs are marked by asterisk (*). Table 3c: Derivation (deletion rules precede nasal assimilation rule) Nasal Gloss to hit to write to water to lock Urs: /mən + pukul/ /mən + tulis/ /mən+siram/ /mən+kunci/ Stop NA NA NA NA mənukul mənulis məniram mənunci Nasal assi- Milimation NA NA NA NA SRs: /*mənukul / /*mənulis/ /*məniram/ /*mǝnunci/ 4. Discussion 51

As commonly happen in many other (local) languages in Indonesia, as well as in English, the general nasal /N/ assimilates to the neighbouring sounds according to its places of articulation. In addition to assimilate, the general nasal sound /N/ is also deleted it is followed by approximant, lateral, or retroflex. In contrast, the general nasal can delete voiceless stop consonants. Therefore, to generate the correct SRs is needed a nasal assimilation rule, and deletion rules. Because there is more than one phonological rule to generate the SRs, it is necessary to put the rules in the right order, otherwise it will result the incorrect SRs. In case of the behaviours of the nasal sound in Indonesian active verb prefix /mən-/, the nasal assimilation rule should precede the deletion rules. In Indonesian, however, these general rules can also be violated by a small number of data because they do not follow these rules. Some simple verbs commencing with the plosive bilabial voiceless consonant /p, t, s, k/ in which the voiceless plosive stop consonants/sonoran are normally dropped in the prefixed verbs because they are preceded by a nasal sound (see data 2, 3, 5, 8, and 10), the sounds are be preserved. The sound /p/ in the simple forms as in the verbs /puɳa/ to own and /pərsuasi/ to pərsuade will not be dropped when these simple verbs get the active prefix /mən/. Accordingly, they become /məmpuɳai/ and /məmpərsuasi/ rather than /*məmuɳai/ and /*məmərsuasi/ respectively. Another phenomena is that the pronunciation variation which occurs to some verbs when they commence with the consonant /t, s, k/ as occurring in the simple verbs /taat/ to obey, /sucikan/ to wash or to make holy and /kadʒi/ to analyze or to read al-qur an. The initial sounds /t, s, k/ of the verbs may preserve or drop. The variation may (or not) change the meanings of the prefixed verbs. The verb /taat/ can be either pronounced with or without /t/ in the prefixed forms as either /məntaati/ or /mənaati/ with exactly the same meaning. In contrast, the simple verb /suci/ when it receives the active prefix /mən/. This verb will be read /mənsuci(kan)/ meaning to make holy and /məɳuci/ meaning to clean/to wash. The same phenomena also happens to the simple verb form /kadʒi/ which can be pronounced differently either with the plosive stop sound /k/ as in the prefixed form /məŋkadʒi/ meaning to study or without /k/ as in the prefixed form /məŋadʒi/ meaning to read the holy Qur an. 5. Conclusion The most common sound alteration in Indonesian is the general nasal of the prefix verb meaning active /mən/. This prefix is added to simple verb forms to produce the active verbs. The general nasal /N/ has certain behaviours as it will change into labial, alveolar, palatal, and velar depending on the features of the following sound or the initial sound of the verb, or it is deleted. The correct SRs can be derived by applying nasal assimilation rule; voiceless stop deletion, and nasal assimilation followed by deletion. However, there are a small number of data which do not follow these rules (they can be considered as exception or irregular forms). The data may show the pronunciation variation only, whereas the other may distinguish the meanings of the verbs. Acknowledgement There are many people who helped me to conduct this research. They are especially my colleagues to whom I always consult and check the pronunciation and distribution of the general nasal /N/ as an active prefix verb in Indonesian in many different contexts. I also thank to the reviewers of this paper for their comments to improve it. References Andi-Pallawa, B.AndAlam, A.F.A. (2013). A comparative analysis between English and Indonesian phonologicalsystems.international Journal of Language Education 1(3), 103-129. Carr, P. (1993). Phonology. London: The Macmillan Press Ltd. Finegan, E., Besner, N, Blair,D., and Collins, P. (1992).Language: Its Structure and Use. Sydney: HBJ Publishers. Halle, M. and Clement, G.N. (1983). Problem Book in Phonology. Cambridge: The MIT Press. Pater, J. (2001). Austronesian Nasal Substitution and Other NC Effects.In H. Van derhulstet. al. (eds.) The Prosody Morphology Interface. Cambrdige: CUP. Sneddon, J. (2006). Colloquial Jakarta Indonesian. Cambridge, ACT: Pacific Linguisitics Tim Pustaka Phoenix, (2010). KamusBesarBahasa Indonesia. Jakarta: PT Media Pustaka Phoenix Jakarta. 52