How to violate the HMC in Kitharaka. Jochen Zeller, University of KwaZulu-Natal August 2010

Similar documents
Phrasal movement inside Bantu verbs

SOME MINIMAL NOTES ON MINIMALISM *

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Argument structure and theta roles

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

Pseudo-Passives as Adjectival Passives

Som and Optimality Theory

Approaches to control phenomena handout Obligatory control and morphological case: Icelandic and Basque

An Introduction to the Minimalist Program

UCLA UCLA Electronic Theses and Dissertations

Derivational and Inflectional Morphemes in Pak-Pak Language

Noun incorporation in Sora: A case for incorporation as morphological merger TLS: 19 February Introduction.

LING 329 : MORPHOLOGY

Multiple case assignment and the English pseudo-passive *

(3) Vocabulary insertion targets subtrees (4) The Superset Principle A vocabulary item A associated with the feature set F can replace a subtree X

Parallel Evaluation in Stratal OT * Adam Baker University of Arizona

The Structure of Relative Clauses in Maay Maay By Elly Zimmer

Korean ECM Constructions and Cyclic Linearization

Underlying and Surface Grammatical Relations in Greek consider

Update on Soar-based language processing

cambridge occasional papers in linguistics Volume 8, Article 3: 41 55, 2015 ISSN

Chapter 3: Semi-lexical categories. nor truly functional. As Corver and van Riemsdijk rightly point out, There is more

The presence of interpretable but ungrammatical sentences corresponds to mismatches between interpretive and productive parsing.

Heads and history NIGEL VINCENT & KERSTI BÖRJARS The University of Manchester

Dependency, licensing and the nature of grammatical relations *

The optimal placement of up and ab A comparison 1

Citation for published version (APA): Veenstra, M. J. A. (1998). Formalizing the minimalist program Groningen: s.n.

Introduction to HPSG. Introduction. Historical Overview. The HPSG architecture. Signature. Linguistic Objects. Descriptions.

The Inclusiveness Condition in Survive-minimalism

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

Using a Native Language Reference Grammar as a Language Learning Tool

Progressive Aspect in Nigerian English

Constraining X-Bar: Theta Theory

THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF HUMANITIES & SOCIAL STUDIES

The Acquisition of Person and Number Morphology Within the Verbal Domain in Early Greek

Interfacing Phonology with LFG

Theoretical Syntax Winter Answers to practice problems

Linguistic Variation across Sports Category of Press Reportage from British Newspapers: a Diachronic Multidimensional Analysis

Derivations (MP) and Evaluations (OT) *

Basic Syntax. Doug Arnold We review some basic grammatical ideas and terminology, and look at some common constructions in English.

LNGT0101 Introduction to Linguistics

Language Acquisition Fall 2010/Winter Lexical Categories. Afra Alishahi, Heiner Drenhaus

The Strong Minimalist Thesis and Bounded Optimality

LIN 6520 Syntax 2 T 5-6, Th 6 CBD 234

1/20 idea. We ll spend an extra hour on 1/21. based on assigned readings. so you ll be ready to discuss them in class

CHILDREN S POSSESSIVE STRUCTURES: A CASE STUDY 1. Andrew Radford and Joseph Galasso, University of Essex

Case government vs Case agreement: modelling Modern Greek case attraction phenomena in LFG

Developing a TT-MCTAG for German with an RCG-based Parser

Frequency and pragmatically unmarked word order *

California Department of Education English Language Development Standards for Grade 8

Phenomena of gender attraction in Polish *

Words come in categories

Agree or Move? On Partial Control Anna Snarska, Adam Mickiewicz University

Lower and Upper Secondary

FOCUS MARKING IN GREEK: SYNTAX OR PHONOLOGY? Michalis Georgiafentis University of Athens

The analysis starts with the phonetic vowel and consonant charts based on the dataset:

Dissertation title: Grammaticalization and lateral grammaticalization: a new perspective on linguistic interfaces and functional categories

Universal Grammar 2. Universal Grammar 1. Forms and functions 1. Universal Grammar 3. Conceptual and surface structure of complex clauses

Syntactic types of Russian expressive suffixes

Disharmonic Word Order from a Processing Typology Perspective. John A. Hawkins, U of Cambridge RCEAL & UC Davis Linguistics

Degree Phrases* J.L.G. Escribano University of Oviedo Revista Alicantina de Estudios Ingleses 15 (2002): 49-77

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

On the final vowel in Kikae

On Labeling: Principle C and Head Movement

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

On the Notion Determiner

5 Minimalism and Optimality Theory

Lexical phonology. Marc van Oostendorp. December 6, Until now, we have presented phonological theory as if it is a monolithic

Dissertation Summaries. Headedness in Word Formation and Lexical Semantics: Evidence from Italiot and Cypriot (University of Patras, 2014)*

Inleiding Taalkunde. Docent: Paola Monachesi. Blok 4, 2001/ Syntax 2. 2 Phrases and constituent structure 2. 3 A minigrammar of Italian 3

UKLO Round Advanced solutions and marking schemes. 6 The long and short of English verbs [15 marks]

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

Hindi Aspectual Verb Complexes

Opportunities for Writing Title Key Stage 1 Key Stage 2 Narrative

Advanced Grammar in Use

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

Optimality Theory and the Minimalist Program

Tagged for Deletion: A Typological Approach to VP Ellipsis in Tag Questions

AN INTRODUCTION (2 ND ED.) (LONDON, BLOOMSBURY ACADEMIC PP. VI, 282)

DOWNSTEP IN SUPYIRE* Robert Carlson Societe Internationale de Linguistique, Mali

Today we examine the distribution of infinitival clauses, which can be

The Pennsylvania State University. The Graduate School. College of the Liberal Arts THE TEACHABILITY HYPOTHESIS AND CONCEPT-BASED INSTRUCTION

Parsing of part-of-speech tagged Assamese Texts

Derivational: Inflectional: In a fit of rage the soldiers attacked them both that week, but lost the fight.

The Discourse Anaphoric Properties of Connectives

Dissertation Summaries. The Acquisition of Aspect and Motion Verbs in the Native Language (Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, 2014)

Taught Throughout the Year Foundational Skills Reading Writing Language RF.1.2 Demonstrate understanding of spoken words,

5/29/2017. Doran, M.K. (Monifa) RADBOUD UNIVERSITEIT NIJMEGEN

SINGLE DOCUMENT AUTOMATIC TEXT SUMMARIZATION USING TERM FREQUENCY-INVERSE DOCUMENT FREQUENCY (TF-IDF)

Software Maintenance

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 154 ( 2014 )

Minding the Absent: Arguments for the Full Competence Hypothesis 1. Abstract

Providing student writers with pre-text feedback

A Computational Evaluation of Case-Assignment Algorithms

ON SOME FUNCTIONAL CATEGORIES IN ÒGBAHÙ DIALECT OF ÌGBÒ

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

18 The syntax phonology interface

Houghton Mifflin Reading Correlation to the Common Core Standards for English Language Arts (Grade1)

Program in Linguistics. Academic Year Assessment Report

1 st Quarter (September, October, November) August/September Strand Topic Standard Notes Reading for Literature

Transcription:

How to violate the HMC in Kitharaka Jochen Zeller, University of KwaZulu-Natal August 2010 1 Introduction The standard analysis of head movement assumes that a head X moves to a head position Y by adjoining to Y, (1a). 1 The so-called Head Movement Constraint (Travis 1984) states that a head X must not skip an intervening head Z when moving to Y, (1b): g g (1) a. YP b. * YP Y XP Y ZP 2 X Y X Z XP X In section 2 of this short paper, I address data from the Bantu language Kitharaka which constitute violations of the HMC. I then show in section that the theory developed in Matushansky (2006), which analyses head movement as consisting of a syntactic and a morphological step, is capable of handling these data. In section 4 I conclude that HMCviolations do not provide evidence against a head movement analysis of syntactic word formation processes in Bantu. 2 HMC violations in Kitharaka Muriungi (2008) presents a detailed analysis of the morphosyntactic properties of the Bantu language Kitharaka (spoken in Kenya). Muriungi adopts the view that the various verbal affixes in Kitharaka head their own projections in the syntax. However, he provides evidence that the correct order of affixes cannot be derived by head movement without violating the HMC. Below I illustrate Muringui's key argument with just one example; Muriungi (2008) discusses numerous other affixes that raise problems of a similar nature. The example that I focus on here concerns the relation between a subset of the derivational suffixes which can attach to the verb in Kitharaka, namely the neuter 1 I ignore alternative proposals, which distinguish head adjunction from other types of head movement, such as head substitution (i.e. movement into an existing, but "empty" head position) or "morphological" head movement triggered by affixal "sub-zero" heads (i.e. an affixal head Y -1 attracts X as its morphological host). See e.g. Rizzi & Roberts (1989), Roberts (1991, 1992) and van Riemsdijk (1998) for discussion. 1

morpheme -îk- (glossed as ABLE by Muriungi), -ang- (glossed as ERR, for erratic) and -ir- (PERF; perfect tense): 2 (2) Mûûro n-û-ring-îk-ang-ir-e. [Kitharaka; Muriungi 2008: 6] river Foc-SM-cross-ABLE-ERR-PERF-FV 'The river was easy to cross irregularly.' In order to determine the hierarchy of the syntactic projections that correspond to the verb and the three derivational affixes in (2), Muriungi investigates their scope relations. He notes (2008: 6) that the meaning of the sentence in (2) is adequately described by (a), but not by (b): () a. The river was easy to cross at 7a.m., 7.0 a.m., 7.45 a.m. the river was not flooded, so I could always walk across. (ABLE > ERRATIC) b. * Only at 7 a.m, 7.0 a.m., 7.45 a.m. was the river easy to cross other times, the temporary bridge was removed and one had to walk across the strong currents of the flooded river. (*ERRATIC > ABLE) The interpretation of (2) implies therefore that the Able-suffix takes scope over the Erratic-suffix, not the other way around. Given that scope is typically represented via c- command, Muriungi concludes that the relative hierarchy of affixes in Kitharaka must be as shown in (4): (4) PerfP Perf AbleP -ir- Able ErrP -îk - -ang- g V -ring- The problem pointed out by Muriungi is that, on the basis of (4), successive-cyclic head movement would derive the wrong order of morphemes. If the verb moves and adjoins to Err, the verbal root -ring- forms a complex head with -ang- (-ring-ang-, assuming leftadjunction). This complex head would then move and adjoin to the neuter suffix -ik-, deriving a stem like *-ring-ang-ik- or *-ik-ring-ang. However, (2) shows that the affix -ik- intervenes between the erratic marker and the verb. In order for this affix order to be derived by head movement, the verb in (4) would have to move to Able in one step, 2 The other morphemes attached to the verb in (2) are the focus marker (FOC), the subject agreement marker (SM), and the "final vowel" (FV), a suffix which in Bantu languages can be associated with a variety of grammatical functions, such as tense, aspect or polarity. The representation in (4) is slightly simplified; it ignores other derivational morphemes discussed by Muriungi and only considers the suffixes present in example (2). See chapter 1 of Muriungi (2008) for more details. 2

across the Err-head. However, this derivation would violate HMC. Consequently, Muriungi (2008) interprets these (and other, similar) data from Kitharaka as evidence against a head movement approach to word formation in Bantu. His alternative proposal is based on a phrasal movement-analysis in the style of Cinque (2005) and therefore relies crucially on the idea that complex words are not represented as complex heads. Instead, in Muriungi's (2008) analysis, the affixal parts of a single word appear in many different head and specifier positions, and the complex word is spread across the whole clause (see Muriungi 2008 for details, and Julien 2002 for similar ideas). However, in Zeller (2010), I discuss data from the Bantu language Shona (spoken in Zimbabwe) which show that this "clausal word"-idea has unwelcome consequences in constructions in which the morphologically complex verb moves as one constituent. In Shona object relatives clauses, the complex verb moves to a clause-initial position as a single word: (5) mbatya [dza-[va-ka-son-er-a] i vakadzi t i mwenga] clothes10 REL10-SM2-PAST-sew-APPL-FV woman2 bride1 'the clothes which the women sewed for the bride' [Shona; Demuth & Harford 1999: 42] The standard word order in Shona is SVO. In (5), however, the verb vakasonera, which includes the inflectional past tense and subject agreement affixes as well as a derivational affix (the applicative marker), has moved in front of the subject to combine with the relative complementiser dza- (see Demuth & Harford 1999). The resulting word order is VSO. While an analysis which treats the verb in (5) as a complex head has no trouble explaining this word order in terms of head movement to C, a "clausal word"-approach such as Muriungi's cannot easily explain these cases of subject-verb inversion. The problem is that, although the head movement approach works better for Shona, it does not seem to be able to explain the Kitharaka data. However, in the following section I show that, in contrast to the conclusion drawn by Muriungi, the Kitharaka verb in example (2) can be analysed as a complex verbal head which has been derived by head movement on the basis of the underlying representation in (4). The account I propose is based on the theory of head movement developed in Matushansky (2006). Head movement as phrasal movement and morphological merger As was noted in the introduction, the standard view of head movement is that if a head X is attracted by another head Y, X moves and adjoins to Y in one step, forming a complex head which includes both X and Y. However, the view that head movement targets head positions is problematic from a minimalist point of view, because it violates the Extension Condition, which states that movement must always target the root of a syntactic object, and because the higher copy of the moved head does not c-command the lower copy. In order to solve this problem, Matushansky (2006) proposes a theory in which a head movement operation is regarded as consisting of two independent steps, one syntactic and one morphological. According to her theory, if X undergoes head movement to Y, it first moves to the specifier of the head Y. This movement step, shown in (6a) below, is in accordance with the Extension Condition, and X also c-commands its

lower copy. In Matushansky's analysis, the movement step in (6a) is triggered by c- selection: the attracting head Y is assumed to have an uninterpretable categorial feature [ux] corresponding to the category of the attracted head. This feature licenses both Merge of Y and XP and movement of X to [Spec, Y]. What makes head movement special is the second step, which according to Matushansky is a purely morphological operation called "m-merger". M-merger combines X and Y to form the familiar head adjunction structure in (6b): (6) a. YP b. YP X Y' Y XP 2 Y XP X Y X ( ) X ( ) Importantly, as discussed by Matushansky (2006, sec. 5.1.), the analysis in (6) is in principle capable of accommodating constructions in which the HMC is violated. In order to see why this is possible, suppose that YP in (6) is selected by a head Z and that the syntactic step in (6a) is not followed by m-merger in (6b). In this case, the head X in [Spec, Y] can undergo further movement to [Spec, Z], (7a), and subsequently m-merge with Z, (7b). The outcome is a configuration in which head movement of X to Z has skipped Y: (7) a. ZP b. ZP X Z Z YP 2 Z YP X Z X Y X Y Y XP Y XP X ( ) X ( ) Since heads are allowed to move to specifiers, successive-cyclic movement of the head X in (7a) must be permitted. In fact, this type of movement could only be ruled out by the stipulation that m-merger must apply automatically as soon as a head has moved into a specifier position. Without such a stipulation, the derivation in (7a), and subsequent m- merger, is possible, giving rise to representations which violate the HMC. The idea that heads, like phrases, move to the specifier positions of their attracting heads can now be used to explain the Kitharaka data. The derivation proceeds as follows. First, the erratic marker merges with the VP, in accordance with the hierarchy in (4). According to Matushansky, the initial step of head movement is syntactic movement of the head to the specifier of the attracting category. Since Err c-selects the VP, the verb can be attracted by Err's [uv]-feature and move to [Spec, Err]: 4

(8) ErrP V Err' -ring- -ang- 6 -ring- The next step of a head movement operation is usually m-merger, which, if applied to (8), would create a complex head with the morpheme order *-ring-ang-. Importantly, however, nothing in the theory forces m-merger to follow syntactic movement in (8). M- merger creates complex heads which are required for morphological reasons; but if morphology does not require (or indeed, not permit) two affixes to be merged as complex heads, then this means that m-merger may simply not apply. Consequently, although syntactic c-selection has forced V to move to [Spec, Err] in (8), the verb may remain in this position when the next head is merged. As shown in (4), this head is the neuter affix, Able. Since Able c-selects ErrP, it can also trigger head movement of Err. This means that Err first moves to [Spec, Able], (9a); m-merger then creates the complex Able-head which incorporates Err, (9b): (9) a. AbleP b. AbleP Err Able Able ErrP -ang- 2 Able ErrP Able Err V Err' -îk - -îk - -ang- -ring- V Err' -ring- -ang- 6 -ang- 6 In (9b), the verb is still in the specifier of the Err-head. Importantly, from this position, it can now undergo further movement to [Spec, Able]. In order for this to happen, all we need to assume is that Able has an [uv]-feature, which attracts the closest verbal category. First, the verb moves to [Spec, Able], (10a), and then m-merger can apply and create a complex Able-head, (10b). This head includes the verb, the erratic and the neuter affix in the right order: (10) a. AbleP b. AbleP V Able Able ErrP -ring- 2 Able ErrP V Able V Err' 2 -ring- 2 -ring- Able Err V Err' Able Err -îk - -ang- -ring- -îk - -ang- -ang- 6 -ang- 6 5

The complex Able-head in (10b) looks as if it had been derived anti-cyclically, because the verb has merged with Able after the erratic marker has moved and adjoined to Able as a suffix. However, what makes this derivation legitimate is the fact that the verb is actually in [Spec, Err] when the Err-head moves to Able (see (9)). Since nothing prevents movement of a head across its own specifier, (9) is unproblematic. (10b) is also derived without excorporation (no complex head was "destroyed" by moving out one of its parts). However, (10b) violates the HMC: the verb has skipped the Err-head when moving to Able. But Matushansky's theory predicts such violations, at least if heads are allowed to move cyclically from specifier to specifier before they incorporate as heads via m-merger (cf. the discussion of long head movement in Matushansky 2006, sec. 5.1.). Therefore, if Matushansky's (2006) theory is adopted, the derivation outlined in (8)-(10) is an entirely legitimate way of deriving a complex head corresponding to the verb stem -ring-ik-angon the basis of the syntax in (4). The analysis shown in (8)-(10) implies that the possibility of a derivation which violates the HMC is the result of lexical properties associated with the elements involved in the derivation. Since head movement is triggered by c-selection, the mismatch between the syntactic hierarchy in (4) and the morpheme order in (2) must be due to a selectional property of the Able-head: it c-selects two syntactic categories, namely Err (which determines syntactic merger of Able and ErrP and head movement of Err) and the verb (which determines head movement of V from [Spec, Err]). In order to avoid overgeneration, one may want to regard multiple c-selection and the corresponding HMC-violations as exceptional. Interestingly, however, the assumption that a syntactic head which corresponds to an affix can c-select more than one category may be independently needed. Given that the use of any particular derivational affix is optional, there cannot be a fixed hierarchy of syntactic projections representing these affixes. The syntax corresponding to morphologically complex verbs in Bantu sometimes includes and sometimes omits certain categories. For example, in constructions in which the Kitharaka verb comes with a neuter suffix, but without an erratic marker, Able must be allowed to merge with VP directly, which means that Able must also c-select V. The derivation in (8)-(10) can then be regarded as a way of satisfying both c-selectional properties of the Able-head (i.e. checking its [uerr]- and its [uv]-feature) within the same derivation. 4 Conclusion The theory proposed in Matushansky (2006) implements the crucial insight that head movement is a syntax-morphology interface operation: syntactic movement (head-tospecifier) feeds a morphological word formation process (m-merger). The fact that this theory can also derive data which have been interpreted as a challenge for traditional head movement accounts not only provides evidence for Matushansky's approach, but also supports the idea that syntactic word formation in Bantu be analysed in terms of head movement. In light of the empirical advantages of head movement analyses discussed in section 2 (and in more detail in Zeller 2010), I consider this a welcome conclusion. 6

5 References Cinque, G. (2005). Deriving Greenberg's Universal 20 and its exceptions. Linguistic Inquiry 6 (): 15-. Demuth, K. and Harford, C. (1999). Verb raising and subject inversion in Bantu relatives. Journal of African Languages and Linguistics 20, 41-61. Julien, M. (2002). Syntactic Heads and Word Formation. Oxford University Press: Oxford. Matushansky, O. (2006). Head Movement in Linguistic Theory. Linguistic Inquiry 7/1, 69-109. Muriungi, P. (2008). Phrasal movement inside Bantu verbs: Deriving affix scope and order in Kîîtharaka. PhD-thesis, University of Tromsø. Riemsdijk, H.C. van (1998). Head Movement and Adjacency. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 16, 6-678. Rizzi, L and Roberts, I. (1989). Complex Inversion in French. Probus 1, 1-0. Roberts, I. (1991). Excorporation and Minimality. Linguistic Inquiry 22, 209-218. Roberts, I. (1992). Verbs and Diachronic Syntax. Dordrecht: Kluwer. Travis, L. (1984). Parameters and effects of word order variation. Ph.D.-dissertation. Cambridge: MIT. Zeller, J. (2010). In defence of head movement: evidence from Bantu. Manuscript, University of KwaZulu-Natal. 7