Introduction. Merete Anderssen and Marit Westergaard University of Tromsø

Similar documents
Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Lecture 2: Quantifiers and Approximation

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

Proof Theory for Syntacticians

The International Coach Federation (ICF) Global Consumer Awareness Study

LNGT0101 Introduction to Linguistics

ROSETTA STONE PRODUCT OVERVIEW

Sample Goals and Benchmarks

Introduction to HPSG. Introduction. Historical Overview. The HPSG architecture. Signature. Linguistic Objects. Descriptions.

1/20 idea. We ll spend an extra hour on 1/21. based on assigned readings. so you ll be ready to discuss them in class

Language-Specific Patterns in Danish and Zapotec Children s Comprehension of Spatial Grams

Impact of Educational Reforms to International Cooperation CASE: Finland

On the Notion Determiner

Phenomena of gender attraction in Polish *

Language Center. Course Catalog

Approaches to control phenomena handout Obligatory control and morphological case: Icelandic and Basque

Cross-linguistic aspects in child L2 acquisition

(3) Vocabulary insertion targets subtrees (4) The Superset Principle A vocabulary item A associated with the feature set F can replace a subtree X

Underlying and Surface Grammatical Relations in Greek consider

Frequency and pragmatically unmarked word order *

Linguistics. Undergraduate. Departmental Honors. Graduate. Faculty. Linguistics 1

Som and Optimality Theory

CS 598 Natural Language Processing

03/07/15. Research-based welfare education. A policy brief

An Empirical and Computational Test of Linguistic Relativity

To appear in The TESOL encyclopedia of ELT (Wiley-Blackwell) 1 RECASTING. Kazuya Saito. Birkbeck, University of London

the contribution of the European Centre for Modern Languages Frank Heyworth

Language acquisition: acquiring some aspects of syntax.

The Acquisition of Person and Number Morphology Within the Verbal Domain in Early Greek

Today we examine the distribution of infinitival clauses, which can be

Language Acquisition Fall 2010/Winter Lexical Categories. Afra Alishahi, Heiner Drenhaus

Acquiring Competence from Performance Data

LING 329 : MORPHOLOGY

Cross Language Information Retrieval

An Interactive Intelligent Language Tutor Over The Internet

Chapter 9 Banked gap-filling

A Case Study: News Classification Based on Term Frequency

Informatics 2A: Language Complexity and the. Inf2A: Chomsky Hierarchy

Acquisition vs. Learning of a Second Language: English Negation

Pethau weird ac atmosphere gwych Conflict sites in Welsh-English mixed nominal constructions

NAME: East Carolina University PSYC Developmental Psychology Dr. Eppler & Dr. Ironsmith

English Language and Applied Linguistics. Module Descriptions 2017/18

Dissertation Summaries. The Acquisition of Aspect and Motion Verbs in the Native Language (Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, 2014)

FOREWORD.. 5 THE PROPER RUSSIAN PRONUNCIATION. 8. УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) 4 80.

Formulaic Language and Fluency: ESL Teaching Applications

Author: Justyna Kowalczys Stowarzyszenie Angielski w Medycynie (PL) Feb 2015

The Survey of Adult Skills (PIAAC) provides a picture of adults proficiency in three key information-processing skills:

HIGHLIGHTS OF FINDINGS FROM MAJOR INTERNATIONAL STUDY ON PEDAGOGY AND ICT USE IN SCHOOLS

Longitudinal family-risk studies of dyslexia: why. develop dyslexia and others don t.

Monitoring Metacognitive abilities in children: A comparison of children between the ages of 5 to 7 years and 8 to 11 years

Inleiding Taalkunde. Docent: Paola Monachesi. Blok 4, 2001/ Syntax 2. 2 Phrases and constituent structure 2. 3 A minigrammar of Italian 3

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

Derivational and Inflectional Morphemes in Pak-Pak Language

Age Effects on Syntactic Control in. Second Language Learning

Linking object names and object categories: Words (but not tones) facilitate object categorization in 6- and 12-month-olds

Words come in categories

Generative Second Language Acquisition & Foreign Language Teaching Winter 2009

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 154 ( 2014 )

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

GERM 3040 GERMAN GRAMMAR AND COMPOSITION SPRING 2017

Providing student writers with pre-text feedback

National Literacy and Numeracy Framework for years 3/4

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 141 ( 2014 ) WCLTA Using Corpus Linguistics in the Development of Writing

Entrepreneurial Discovery and the Demmert/Klein Experiment: Additional Evidence from Germany

Summary / Response. Karl Smith, Accelerations Educational Software. Page 1 of 8

Parsing of part-of-speech tagged Assamese Texts

Uncertainty concepts, types, sources

Summary results (year 1-3)

L1 and L2 acquisition. Holger Diessel

Chapter 3: Semi-lexical categories. nor truly functional. As Corver and van Riemsdijk rightly point out, There is more

An Evaluation of the Interactive-Activation Model Using Masked Partial-Word Priming. Jason R. Perry. University of Western Ontario. Stephen J.

The presence of interpretable but ungrammatical sentences corresponds to mismatches between interpretive and productive parsing.

Twenty years of TIMSS in England. NFER Education Briefings. What is TIMSS?

Theoretical Syntax Winter Answers to practice problems

Target Language Preposition Selection an Experiment with Transformation-Based Learning and Aligned Bilingual Data

Document number: 2013/ Programs Committee 6/2014 (July) Agenda Item 42.0 Bachelor of Engineering with Honours in Software Engineering

The recognition, evaluation and accreditation of European Postgraduate Programmes.

Developing Grammar in Context

IMPROVING SPEAKING SKILL OF THE TENTH GRADE STUDENTS OF SMK 17 AGUSTUS 1945 MUNCAR THROUGH DIRECT PRACTICE WITH THE NATIVE SPEAKER

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

Objectives. Chapter 2: The Representation of Knowledge. Expert Systems: Principles and Programming, Fourth Edition

MA Linguistics Language and Communication

IB Diploma Subject Selection Brochure

Introduction. Background. Social Work in Europe. Volume 5 Number 3

Heads and history NIGEL VINCENT & KERSTI BÖRJARS The University of Manchester

The role of prior experiential knowledge of adult learners engaged in professionally oriented postgraduate study: an affordance or constraint?

Developing a TT-MCTAG for German with an RCG-based Parser

Parallel Evaluation in Stratal OT * Adam Baker University of Arizona

Derivational: Inflectional: In a fit of rage the soldiers attacked them both that week, but lost the fight.

ELA/ELD Standards Correlation Matrix for ELD Materials Grade 1 Reading

Participate in expanded conversations and respond appropriately to a variety of conversational prompts

Construction Grammar. University of Jena.

Analyzing Linguistically Appropriate IEP Goals in Dual Language Programs

SOME IMPORTANT ASPECTS IN THE GRAMMAR TEACHING PROCESS

National Academies STEM Workforce Summit

Ling/Span/Fren/Ger/Educ 466: SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION. Spring 2011 (Tuesdays 4-6:30; Psychology 251)

1. Introduction. 2. The OMBI database editor

Approved Foreign Language Courses

Transcription:

Introduction Merete Anderssen and Marit Westergaard University of Tromsø The present volume constitutes the proceedings of the Language Acquisition workshop at the 22nd Scandinavian Conference of Linguistics held in Aalborg, Denmark on June 20-21, 2006. At this workshop there were as many as 21 papers presented by altogether 36 speakers from the Netherlands, Germany, Canada and the USA in addition to the Scandinavian countries Denmark, Norway and Sweden. Eleven of the 12 papers in this volume were presented at the workshop in addition we have included the paper by Paradowski, which was accepted for the workshop but unfortunately not presented in Aalborg. The papers represent a variety of current issues in both first and second language acquisition. The first three contributions focus on different aspects of second language acquisition. In the paper On verb second and the så-construction in two Mainland Scandinavian contact situations, Hilde Sollid and Kristin M. Eide investigate Norwegian data from areas of long-term language contact between a V2 (verb second) and a non-v2 language in Northern Norway. The data display unstable use of V2, and there is also a notable presence of the particle så. These facts are discussed in light of data on the så-construction in Standard Norwegian and Finland Swedish, as well as the particle ni in Finnish. It is suggested that some aspects of så in the data may be explained as one characteristic of L1 transfer, where existing L2 elements are recruited and assigned new functions in the learner s grammar. In the next paper, How to start a V2 declarative clause: Transfer of syntax vs. information structure in L2 German, Ute Bohnacker and Christina Rosén discuss V2 word order and information structure in Swedish, German and non-native German. Concentrating on the clause-initial position of V2 declaratives, the prefield, they investigate the extent of L1 transfer in a closely related L2. The prefield anchors the clause in discourse, and although almost any type of element can occur in this position, naturalistic text corpora show distinct language-specific patterns. Certain types of elements are more common than others in clause-initial position, and their frequencies in Swedish differ substantially from German. Nonnative cross-sectional production data from Swedish learners of German are compared with native control data, and it is found that the learners V2 syntax is largely targetlike, but the sentence-initial part is often unidio- Nordlyd 34.3: 1-6, Merete Anderssen and Marit Westergaard 2007 Papers from the Language Acquisition Workshop, SCL 2006 Edited by Merete Anderssen and Marit Westergaard CASTL, Tromsø. http://www.ub.uit.no/baser/nordlyd

MERETE ANDERSSEN AND MARIT WESTERGAARD matic. The learners have problems with the language-specific linguistic means that have an impact on information structure, and overapply the Swedish principle rheme later in their L2 German. This indicates that there is L1 transfer at the interface of syntax and discourse pragmatics, especially for structures that are frequent in the L1. The paper by Michał B. Paradowski, How do general psychological processes inform FLL pedagogy? Presenting a new instructional frame work, focuses on the instructional aspect of second language acqui-sition. He argues that learning invariably proceeds by relating new facts to what is already familiar and present in the conceptual structure. In the context of foreign language study the familiar is the student s mother tongue. For this reason it is argued that the target language should literally be taught in the framework of the learner s L 1, and the paper then presents the Language Interface Model (LIM). In this model instruction proceeds from an explication of how relevant rules operate in the students L 1 through an explanation of corresponding L 2 rules and subsequent interface formation, modifying the L 1 rule to accommodate L 2 data. Learners should first apply the foreign language rules to L 1 examples before moving to more traditional exercises. The end result is competence expansion which leads to the development of multicompetence and allows for the obliteration of the rules from the learner s conscious mind. The first paper on first language acquisition focuses on a classical issue in the field and takes A fresh look at root infinitives from a cross-linguistic perspective. In this paper Tanja Kupisch and Esther Rinke examine the intensity of the root-infinitive stage in child language in relation to the quantity and quality of the adult input. Child language data from English, French, German, Italian and Brazilian Portuguese are compared, and it is investigated whether children produce infinitives more extensively if the verbal morphology of the target language is ambiguous with respect to the distinction between finite and nonfinite forms, or whether the tokenfrequency of nonfinite verbs in the input is crucial. They conclude that the latter is not decisive. Rather, children seem to avoid the use of finite verb forms especially in languages whose verb paradigms are characterized by ambiguities. Root infinitives may thus be viewed as a temporary phenomenon occurring at a stage when children are learning the inflectional properties of their target language. The paper Mental State Talk by Danish Preschool Children by Ane Knüppel, Rikke Steensgaard and Kristine Jensen de López reports on a study where sixteen four- to six-year-old Danish children are videorecorded in spontaneous interaction with their family. The mental state talk of the children is identified and analyzed with respect to three mental 2

INTRODUCTION domains: desire, feeling and cognition, and this is compared to data from a similar study carried out with Canadian families. The results suggest some cross-cultural differences in children s mental state talk. First, Danish children produce more variation of mental state talk words than Canadian children do, and second, the distribution of mental state talk across the three domains differs for the two language groups. Semantic variation between Danish and English is identified, and this may partly explain the findings. The authors also present a usage-based approach to the investigation of children s development of psychological categories. In her contribution, A Perspective on Doubling Constructions in Dutch, Jaqueline van Kampen considers the acquisition of negation and wh-questions in L1 learners of Dutch, which she demonstrates is characterised by a developmental stage at which these elements are doubled. These doubling stages are shown to be preceded by a stage at which these elements are not doubled, and doubling is argued to be an attempt on the part of the children to maintain an older grammar while simultaneously introducing new structure. The loss of both doubling constructions is demonstrated to coincide with the acquisition of V2 and the subsequent association of the surface V2 structure with the underlying V-final structure. Van Kampen proposes that the existence of these kinds of doubling in child language show that the association between surface and underlying structure, which is taken for granted within the generative paradigm, in fact has to be acquired. In the paper On that One Poverty of the Stimulus Argument, Andrea Gualmini argues against the validity of results presented in Lidz, Waxman and Freedman (2003). Lidz et al carried out an experimental study in which 18-month-old children were first shown an image on a screen, such as a yellow bottle, while being told Look! A yellow bottle! Then, in the test phase, the children were shown two items, e.g. a yellow bottle and a blue bottle, and told Now look. Do you see another one?. In the test phase, the children preferred to look at the yellow bottle rather than the blue bottle, and by Lidz et al this was taken as an indication that children interpreted one to refer to yellow bottle, and not bottle. This was subsequently argued to show that children already at the age of 18 months know that one cannot be anaphoric on the head noun, but must be anaphoric on the N -level, even though this is not assumed to be learnable based on the input. Thus, Lidz et al claim to have reaffirmed the original Poverty of the Stimulus Argument (POSA). Gualmini argues against this conclusion on the grounds that in yellow bottle, both bottle and yellow bottle represent the N -level, and hence we would expect there to be no preference for the latter, as both represent valid referents for one. However, Gualmini maintains the validity 3

MERETE ANDERSSEN AND MARIT WESTERGAARD of the POSA with respect to the interpretation of anaphoric one, and spends the remainder of his paper considering what might cause children to prefer the higher N -level to the lower one in the experiment. Without committing to either explanation, he proposes that this might either be the result of a sentence processing mechanism or pragmatic factors. In the next paper, Helen Stickney undertakes Investigations into Children's Acquisition of the Partitive Structure in order to determine whether children recognise DP as a barrier to adjectival modification. In an experimental study, the contrast between the partitive and the pseudopartitive is exploited to test this. An adjective preceding a pseudopartitive can modify its head, meaning that in a moldy box of chocolates, moldy can modify chocolates, while an adjective preceding a partitive is prevented from doing so, meaning that in a moldy box of the chocolates, moldy cannot modify chocolates. This is argued to be the result of the presence of a lower DP (containing the) acting as a barrier to modification. Pairs of partitive and pseudopartitive constructions that differ only with regard to the presence versus the absence of the low determiner were constructed and tested in three different kinds of tasks. The results show that children between the age of three and five, unlike six-year-olds and adults, do not distinguish between partitives and pseudopartitives with regard to whether they allow an adjective to modify its head. This is taken as support for the assumption that up until the age of six, children s DPs are un-adult-like, and lack the features that make DP a barrier to modification. In Children s Ambiguous Understanding of Weak and Strong Quantifiers, Erik-Jan Smits, Tom Roeper and Bart Hollebrandse investigate children s comprehension of weak (many) versus strong (many of, all) quantifiers in English. It has been demonstrated in the acquisition literature that, unlike adults, children allow a so-called switched reading of strong quantifiers. This means that a sentence such as every cowboy is riding a horse can be interpreted as every horse is ridden by a cowboy by children, but not by adults. However, adult speakers do allow switched readings of weak quantifiers such as many, making many Tour de France winners are French a possible interpretation of many French have won the Tour de France. On the basis of results from an experimental Truth-Value Judgement Task, where the authors set out to test possible hypotheses with regard to the interpretation of quantifiers (and the switched reading of many in particular), they conclude that children have an ambiguous quantifier system for both weak and strong quantifiers. The cause of the non-target ambiguity with strong quantifiers in child English is argued to be related to the different types of quantifier raising. In adult language, weak quantifiers move by means of Q-raising, which means that the quantifier moves 4

INTRODUCTION without its argument, while strong quantifiers move by means of Quantifier Raising (QR), in which the quantifier moves together with its argument to the beginning of the sentence. Smits et al show that children, unlike adults, allow Q-raising and QR for both weak and strong quantifiers, thus giving rise to a larger choice of interpretations than in the target language. In their paper Determiner Use in Italian-Swedish and Italian-German Children: Do Swedish and German Represent the Same Parameter Setting?, Tanja Kupisch and Petra Bernardini compare the acquisition of determiners in two bilingual children acquiring Italian simultaneously with German and Swedish, both with the Germanic language as their dominant language. This situation represents an interesting test case for the Nominal Mapping Parameter (NMP, Chierchia, 1998), which argues for a parametric approach to article use that treats Germanic and Romance languages as representative of two different parameter settings. Due to a lower predictability with regard to the mapping between syntax and semantics, it is argued to take longer to reach the target level of determiner inclusion in a Germanic language, which predicts that articles should be acquired at a similar pace in Swedish and German. However, this prediction is not borne out, as Swedish articles are acquired at a much faster rate than the German ones, suggesting that other factors than the syntax-semantics mapping need to be taken into account with regard to the acquisition of articles. Yulia Rodina s paper asks the question What Do Asymmetries in Children s Performance Tell Us about the Nature of Their Underlying Knowledge?. The paper examines the course of acquisition of the semantic gender criterion in Russian by studying children s overregularization rates with two subtypes of Russian nouns: male kinship terms and male names ending in a. These nouns are semantically masculine, while their morphology indicates feminine gender. Twenty-five Russian children aged 2;6-4;0 participated in this empirical study. The asymmetry found in their agreement production for the individual male kinship terms is explained along the lines of the Words and Rules model (Pinker 1999). That is, the asymmetry is attributed to input frequencies of individual nouns. Yet, frequency is not the only factor which is responsible for the asymmetries in children s production. In addition, differences in the semantic representation of proper names vs. common nouns are argued to play a role. In the final paper, The Acquisition of Compositional Definiteness in Norwegian, Merete Anderssen considers the acquisition of so-called double definiteness in Norwegian. Double definiteness is a phenomenon found in a subset of the Scandinavian languages whereby definiteness is marked in two places in modified definite noun phrases (as in det svarte hus-et, the black house-the), while in simple, unmodified noun phrases it is 5

MERETE ANDERSSEN AND MARIT WESTERGAARD only marked once (hus-et, house-the). While all the Scandinavian varieties mark definiteness with a suffixal article in unmodified noun phrases, there is an interesting pattern of variation in modified definites; Norwegian and Swedish exhibit double definiteness and mark definites with a preadjectival free determiner and a suffixal postnominal article, while Danish has a preadjectival determiner only (det sorte hus, the black house) and Icelandic has a suffixal article only (svarta hús-ið, black house-the). In the acquisition of determiners in Norwegian, the suffixal article can be shown to be acquired at a very early stage, while the preadjectival determiner is attested considerably later and is optionally omitted for a very long period. Anderssen argues for a lexical insertion analysis of the adult Scandinavian languages which she claims explains the late acquisition of the preadjectival determiner. The modified definite noun phrases with no preadjectival determiner found in child language superficially resemble Icelandic modified definite noun phrases. Anderssen argues that this resemblance is not only superficial; according to her analysis of adult Scandinavian, the Icelandic lexicalisation of the functional structure is argued to be the simplest one, and hence it is the first option explored by the children. Tromsø, December 2007 Merete Anderssen Marit Westergaard 6