Syllables and Phonotactics. Ling 110 Guest Lecture Jonathan Manker October 27, 2014

Similar documents
The analysis starts with the phonetic vowel and consonant charts based on the dataset:

Phonological Processing for Urdu Text to Speech System

ADDIS ABABA UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF GRADUATE STUDIES MODELING IMPROVED AMHARIC SYLLBIFICATION ALGORITHM

Pobrane z czasopisma New Horizons in English Studies Data: 18/11/ :52:20. New Horizons in English Studies 1/2016

Speech Recognition using Acoustic Landmarks and Binary Phonetic Feature Classifiers

Consonant-Vowel Unity in Element Theory*

Consonants: articulation and transcription

On the Formation of Phoneme Categories in DNN Acoustic Models

Phonetics. The Sound of Language

**Note: this is slightly different from the original (mainly in format). I would be happy to send you a hard copy.**

Language Acquisition by Identical vs. Fraternal SLI Twins * Karin Stromswold & Jay I. Rifkin

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Universal contrastive analysis as a learning principle in CAPT

To appear in the Proceedings of the 35th Meetings of the Chicago Linguistics Society. Post-vocalic spirantization: Typology and phonetic motivations

SOUND STRUCTURE REPRESENTATION, REPAIR AND WELL-FORMEDNESS: GRAMMAR IN SPOKEN LANGUAGE PRODUCTION. Adam B. Buchwald

Linguistics 220 Phonology: distributions and the concept of the phoneme. John Alderete, Simon Fraser University

Similarity Avoidance in the Proto-Indo-European Root

A Fact in Historical Phonology from the Viewpoint of Generative Phonology: The Underlying Schwa in Old English

SEGMENTAL FEATURES IN SPONTANEOUS AND READ-ALOUD FINNISH

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

Lexical phonology. Marc van Oostendorp. December 6, Until now, we have presented phonological theory as if it is a monolithic

Unvoiced Landmark Detection for Segment-based Mandarin Continuous Speech Recognition

English for Life. B e g i n n e r. Lessons 1 4 Checklist Getting Started. Student s Book 3 Date. Workbook. MultiROM. Test 1 4

Contrastiveness and diachronic variation in Chinese nasal codas. Tsz-Him Tsui The Ohio State University

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Voiced-voiceless distinction in alaryngeal speech - acoustic and articula

An argument from speech pathology

Manner assimilation in Uyghur

age, Speech and Hearii

The Indian English of Tibeto-Burman language speakers*

Revisiting the role of prosody in early language acquisition. Megha Sundara UCLA Phonetics Lab

Markedness and Complex Stops: Evidence from Simplification Processes 1. Nick Danis Rutgers University

Sounds of Infant-Directed Vocabulary: Learned from Infants Speech or Part of Linguistic Knowledge?

Stages of Literacy Ros Lugg

Handout #8. Neutralization

Radical CV Phonology: the locational gesture *

Rhythm-typology revisited.

Phonology Revisited: Sor3ng Out the PH Factors in Reading and Spelling Development. Indiana, November, 2015

Speech Segmentation Using Probabilistic Phonetic Feature Hierarchy and Support Vector Machines

source or where they are needed to distinguish two forms of a language. 4. Geographical Location. I have attempted to provide a geographical

Word Stress and Intonation: Introduction

Christine Mooshammer, IPDS Kiel, Philip Hoole, IPSK München, Anja Geumann, Dublin

Phonological encoding in speech production

I propose an analysis of thorny patterns of reduplication in the unrelated languages Saisiyat

Partial Class Behavior and Nasal Place Assimilation*

Infants learn phonotactic regularities from brief auditory experience

have to be modeled) or isolated words. Output of the system is a grapheme-tophoneme conversion system which takes as its input the spelling of words,

Florida Reading Endorsement Alignment Matrix Competency 1

Richardson, J., The Next Step in Guided Writing, Ohio Literacy Conference, 2010

The Perception of Nasalized Vowels in American English: An Investigation of On-line Use of Vowel Nasalization in Lexical Access

ABSTRACT. Some children with speech sound disorders (SSD) have difficulty with literacyrelated

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

Program in Linguistics. Academic Year Assessment Report

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. VCV-sequencies in a preliminary text-to-speech system for female speech

The Acquisition of English Intonation by Native Greek Speakers

THE PHONOLOGICAL WORD IN STANDARD MALA Y

Clinical Application of the Mean Babbling Level and Syllable Structure Level

Precedence Constraints and Opacity

Segregation of Unvoiced Speech from Nonspeech Interference

Parallel Evaluation in Stratal OT * Adam Baker University of Arizona

Acoustic correlates of stress and their use in diagnosing syllable fusion in Tongan. James White & Marc Garellek UCLA

Basic concepts: words and morphemes. LING 481 Winter 2011

Large Kindergarten Centers Icons

Listener-oriented phonology

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

RP ENGLISH AND CASTILIAN SPANISH DIPHTHONGS REVISITED FROM THE BEATS-AND-BINDING PERSPECTIVE

DEVELOPMENT OF LINGUAL MOTOR CONTROL IN CHILDREN AND ADOLESCENTS

Automatic English-Chinese name transliteration for development of multilingual resources

CLASSIFICATION OF PROGRAM Critical Elements Analysis 1. High Priority Items Phonemic Awareness Instruction

Classification. Universals

The Bruins I.C.E. School

GOLD Objectives for Development & Learning: Birth Through Third Grade

The Journey to Vowelerria VOWEL ERRORS: THE LOST WORLD OF SPEECH INTERVENTION. Preparation: Education. Preparation: Education. Preparation: Education

The ABCs of O-G. Materials Catalog. Skills Workbook. Lesson Plans for Teaching The Orton-Gillingham Approach in Reading and Spelling

Affricates. Affricates, nasals, laterals and continuants. Affricates. Affricates. Study questions

A Cross-language Corpus for Studying the Phonetics and Phonology of Prominence

Improving the Quality of MT Output using Novel Name Entity Translation Scheme

Get Your Hands On These Multisensory Reading Strategies

On the nature of voicing assimilation(s)

A Trio of Phonetic Details in Homalco

THE PHONOLOGY OF YUROK GLOTTALIZED SONORANTS: SEGMENTAL FISSION UNDER SYLLABIFICATION 1. Juliette Blevins

Contrasting English Phonology and Nigerian English Phonology

Underlying Representations

Is French Optimal?* 1 Introduction. 2 Two output-driven processes

2,1 .,,, , %, ,,,,,,. . %., Butterworth,)?.(1989; Levelt, 1989; Levelt et al., 1991; Levelt, Roelofs & Meyer, 1999

Different Task Type and the Perception of the English Interdental Fricatives

A Neural Network GUI Tested on Text-To-Phoneme Mapping

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

LING 329 : MORPHOLOGY

Books Effective Literacy Y5-8 Learning Through Talk Y4-8 Switch onto Spelling Spelling Under Scrutiny

Modern TTS systems. CS 294-5: Statistical Natural Language Processing. Types of Modern Synthesis. TTS Architecture. Text Normalization

GEMINATION STRATEGIES IN L1 AND ENGLISH PRONUNCIATION OF POLISH LEARNERS

DOWNSTEP IN SUPYIRE* Robert Carlson Societe Internationale de Linguistique, Mali

Correspondence between the DRDP (2015) and the California Preschool Learning Foundations. Foundations (PLF) in Language and Literacy

THE RECOGNITION OF SPEECH BY MACHINE

Journal of Phonetics

Multilingual Speech Data Collection for the Assessment of Pronunciation and Prosody in a Language Learning System

Perceived speech rate: the effects of. articulation rate and speaking style in spontaneous speech. Jacques Koreman. Saarland University

LNGT0101 Introduction to Linguistics

Journal of Phonetics

Documenting incipient obsolescence: a multi-pronged approach to Dhao, eastern Indonesia

Transcription:

Syllables and Phonotactics Ling 110 Guest Lecture Jonathan Manker October 27, 2014

Syllables, Phonotactics, and your course project Being able to describe the syllable structure and phonotactics of a language may be relevant to sections 4 and 5 of your course project. Section 4, Prosody. Syllables are often relevant for understanding systems of stress placement and tone restrictions (Do certain types of syllables attract stress, or disallow certain tones?) Section 5, Phonology. Often phonological rules (which we ll see more examples of later in the week) are sensitive to syllable structure, for example, a rule may apply only in syllable codas but not onsets, etc.

What is a syllable? Let s consult the textbook first which provides us with a satisfactory definition of the syllable: A unit of speech for which there is no satisfactory definition. Syllables seem to be necessary units in the mental organization and production of utterances. (Ladefoged & Johnson pg. 318). Syllables are something that most people seem to agree exist but are hard to define. If there is no easy way to define syllables then will this prove to be a useful concept to talk about?

Phonetic Definitions of the Syllable Now that that is clear (not really), we can start with a purely phonetic description of the syllable: One possible theory is that peaks of syllabicity coincide with peaks of sonority (pg. 214). What are peaks of sonority? Recall the sonority hierarchy from last week s lecture. More sonorant sounds will be higher in amplitude. Phonetically speaking (not necessarily in terms of phonological features): low vowels > high vowels > approximants > liquids > nasals > voiced fricatives > voiceless fricatives > voiced stops > voiceless stops

This chart from ACIP show the relative sonorities of different English sounds. Notice how much acoustic energy voicing and sonority adds.

Syllables as Peaks in Sonority Let s test out our phonetic definition of syllables, that they coincide with peaks in sonority. This will essentially mean that at the point when the amplitude switches from decreasing to increasing, that we are at a syllable boundary. Amplitude can be seen in the waveform. Thus we might expect something like this:

Syllables as seen in waveforms Sometimes less clear--- how many syllables does this word have?

Syllables in waveforms What about this one? Sonority or prominence peaks seem to play a role but this theory of syllables doesn t always work. Ideal syllables follow the sonority sequencing principle where syllables begin with low sonority sounds, become more sonorous, such as usually having a vowel nucleus, and then may only end in less sonorous sounds. For example: plant, or trust. What about sixths then? Some have also suggested that syllables may be stored units in motor planning.

Syllables in Phonology Let s consider now syllables from a phonological point of view where they are most abstract units of organization. Do we need to include syllables in this discussion? Chomsky & Halle s Sound Pattern of English (1968) didn t define the syllable. Words were considered to be strings of segments, and one may manage to describe phonological patterns without them.

Syllables in Phonology Without referring to syllables, how might we describe why we can t say knee as [kni] but acne as [ækni] is fine? What about the patterning of clear [l] vs. dark [ɫ] l? Consider how /l/ patterns in the following words: bowl ebola love polka [bɔ(ɫ)] [ibɔlɘ] [lʌv] [pɔ(ɫ)kɘ] Without referring to syllables we would have to give a complex environment: /l/ > [ɫ] (or Ø) / _C OR _# (/l/ surfaces as [ɫ] (or Ø) at the end of a word or before a consonant)

Syllables in Phonology This complex environment--- before a word boundary (#) or a consonant (C) is very common cross-linguistically. But how do we capture the unity of these environments? This can be done by referring to syllables. Both before a C and before a # are both the coda position (final consonant) within syllables. Thus: /bɔl/ CVC /pɔl.kɘ/ CVC.CV vs. /i.bɔ.lɘ/ V.CV.CV /lʌv/ CVC

Syllable Structure Syllables may begin with an onset (ω) which is an initial consonant. All languages optionally allow onsets, some require them. Syllables are usually required to have a nucleus (ν) which is usually what grants a string of sounds syllablehood. This is most commonly a vowel or sonorant segment.

Syllable Structure Final consonants within a syllable but occurring after the nucleus are in the coda (κ). Some languages may not allow any coda segments, and only CV syllables are allowed. The nucleus and coda together are referred to as the rhyme.

Syllabification and the Maximum Onset Principle How do we decide how to analyze certain segments that might be ambiguous with respect to their syllable position? For example, the /l/ s in ebola or polka or the /pl/ in diploma? The Maximum Onset Principle states that intervocalic consonants should be considered onsets first if possible as long there is no violation of the sonority hierarchy. That is, there is a preference for consonants to be onsets rather than codas. Thus: /e.bo.la/ not /e.bol.a/ (or /eb.ol.a/); /pol.ka/ not /po.lka/ (because this would violate the sonority hierarchy) or /polk.a/; /di.plo.ma/ not /dip.lo.ma/ or /dipl.om.a/

Application - Koryak Now let s apply our knowledge of syllables to understand an example from the Koryak language of Siberia. Koryak allows only V, VC, CV, and CVC syllables (no consonant clusters). Underlying word forms may consist of strings of syllables with no vowels. But schwa segments must be inserted to adhere to the allowable syllable types.

Application - Koryak The underlying form then of I ask him is said to be /t-pŋlo-n/. To break up the consonants here, schwa [ə] is inserted. So the output then is: [təp.ŋə.lon]. MOP rules out [ət.pəŋ.lon], and Koryak s syllable structure rules out possibilities like [tpəŋ.lon] or [təp.ŋlon]. /mt-pŋlo-n/ -> [mət.pəŋ.lon] /na-pŋlo-n] -> [nap.ŋə.lon] Again, being able to refer to syllable structure makes for a more elegant description as to why two consonants can occur next to each other word internally, but not at the beginning or end of a word.

Syllable Types and Phonotactics Phonotactics deals with the rules and restrictions languages have for what strings of segments are permissible. Crosslinguistically, different languages have different requirements for what types of syllables are allowed. CV is considered the most basic syllable type and is allowed in every language.

Syllable Types Syllables with no coda are referred to as open syllables. (so V, CV, CCV, CCCV, etc.) Syllables with a coda are referred to as closed syllables. (so VC, CVC, VCC, CCVC, etc.) For processes like stress and tone, these might be sensitive to syllables of different weights. Having a coda or a long vowel usually makes a syllables heavier. Weight implicational hierarchy: CV < CVC < CVV

Syllable Types Crosslinguistically Most languages have constraints on what syllable types are allowed. Some languages may only allow V and CV syllables, such as Hawai ian: u.ke.le.le pa.ho.e.ho.e hu.mu.hu.mu.nu.ku.nu.ku.a:.pu.a.ʔa Some languages can have long consonants clusters in onsets or codas. Itel men (Siberia): tksxqzukitʧen I wanted to eat ktwel j knen he brought it ktimpl j x bring it k anjʧpx teach him Maximum syllable in English: CCCVCC[+ coronals]

Onset and Coda Restrictions In addition to restricting the syllable types, languages may restrict what segments are permitted in different parts of the syllable. Typically only non-syllabic consonants occur in onsets and codas. Languages tend to allow most of their consonant segments in onsets while codas may be quite restricted. This may occur because phonetic cues are harder to hear in cues, especially if the sounds are unreleased. So for example, ancient Greek onset consonants could include: /p t k b d g p h t h k h m n l r s z h/. but in codas, only /s n/ are allowed

Phonotactics in Han Athabascan voiced plain aspirated ejectives voiceless fric. voiced fric. voiced sonorants 1 voiceless sonorants 1 labial dental lateral alve olar <b> <d> /b/ 2 /d/ 3 <w, -ww> /w/ 2 <hw, -wh> /w / <m> /m/ 2 <d> /d/ <ddh> /d ð / <dl> /d ɬ / <t> <tth> <tl> /t/ /t θ / /t ɬ / <t <tth > <tl > > /t θ / /t ɬ / /t / <th> <ł> /θ/ /ɬ/ <dh> <l> /ð/ /l/ 4 <n, -nn (-n)> /n/ 3 /l/ <-n (-nh)> /n / <-l (-lh)> /l / <dz> /d z / <ts> /t s / <ts > /t s / <s> /s/ <z> /z/ retroflex palatal velar glottal <dr> /d r / <tr> /t r / <tr > /t r / <sr> /s r / <zr> /z r / <r, -rr (-r)> /r/ <-r (-rh)> /ɻ / <j> /d ʒ / 3 <j> /ǰ/ <ch> /č/ <ch > /č / <sh> /š/ <zh> /ž/ <y, -yy (- y)> /j/ <-y (-yh)> /j/ <g> /g/ <k> /k/ <k > /k / <kh> /x/ <gh> /ɣ/ <ng> /ŋ/ < > /ʔ/ <h> /h/ Only the brown sounds occur both in onsets and codas; the green ones occur only in codas and the blue/gray ones occur only in onsets (34 to 16)

Nucleus Restrictions Vowels can be nuclei in all languages. Typically most vowels are allowed in open syllables while mergers may occur in close syllables or before certain consonants (somewhat the reverse in English). Some languages allow other sonorants (liquids, glides, nasals) to be syllable nuclei. In a few languages, obstruents such as fricatives and even stops can be syllable nuclei (or are nuclei not required in syllables?) The division of what is allowed as a syllable nucleus typically occurs somewhere on the syllable hierarchy. vowels > (approximants) > liquids > nasals > voiced fricatives > voiceless fricatives > voiced stops > voiceless stops

Rare Nucleus Segments Some languages allow nuclei with less sonorant or more constricted nuclei Mambila language (Nigeria, Bantoid). Fricative vowels, something Matt Faytak studies /bzɨ / ask /kɣɯ / cut True fricatives and stops may even be acceptable nuclei, yielding complex consonant clusters

Nucleus Restrictions Wakashan Languages Bella Coola (British Columbia) [xɬpʼχʷɬtɬpɬɬs] he had in his possession a bunchberry plant Berber (North Africa) tftχtstt you rolled it (fem) More Berber examples on right

Salish Consonant Clusters Long strings of fricatives are permitted. Syllabification not always clear. Looking again at Bella Coola, [xɬpʼχʷɬtɬpɬɬs]--- is this one syllable, lots of syllables with fricative nuclei, or are syllables totally irrelevant for examples like these?

A few other things --- syllables or strings? Some have argued (Blevins 2003) that the syllable may not be a universal unit of segmental organization, and that some languages only consider the segmental string. Recall (such as in Koryak) that it is frequent that languages may only allow consonant clusters intervocalically, but not finally--- CVCCV vs. CVCC. This is because we can analyze the first one as being split into two syllables, CVC.CV where there is only one consonant in each onset and coda, but two in the coda in CVCC. However, some languages like Lenakel allow CC onsets and clusters, but medial CCC clusters are not allowed, even though they might be if syllabified as CVC.CCV. Thus the constraint here might be *CCC (no sequences of three consonants allowed).

A few other things--- ambisyllabicity Recall the Maximum Onset Principle, stating that intervocalic consonants should be analyzed as onsets of the following syllable. Recall also the patterning of /l/ in English which seems to support this. At the same time, the patterning of tense vs. lax vowels may suggest otherwise. Now recall that while tense vowels can occur about anywhere, lax vowels can only occur in closed syllables, thus /pɛt/ or /pɪt/ are fine but not /pɛ/ or /pɪ/.

A few other things--- ambisyllabicity However, medial consonants in English can act like codas in that they license lax vowels: penny pity packing, etc. /pɛni/ /pɪɾi/ /pækɪŋ/ These intervocalic consonants are thus often analyzed as being ambisyllabic where there is a single consonant that is both the coda of the preceding syllable but the onset of the following syllable.

Conclusion Syllables may be rooted in phonetics, in that they generally follow patterns of sonority peaks. But they are more complex that than, and are abstract organizational units. Nevertheless, the phonotactics and other phonological rules of languages are often sensitive to aspects of syllable structure. Thus, syllables are often a useful concept for describing languages despite the unanswered questions we still have about them.