When reading is readn or somthn. Distinctness of phonological representations of lexical items in normal and disabled readers

Similar documents
Longitudinal family-risk studies of dyslexia: why. develop dyslexia and others don t.

Stages of Literacy Ros Lugg

ABSTRACT. Some children with speech sound disorders (SSD) have difficulty with literacyrelated

Dyslexia/dyslexic, 3, 9, 24, 97, 187, 189, 206, 217, , , 367, , , 397,

Developing phonological awareness: Is there a bilingual advantage?

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

Computerized training of the correspondences between phonological and orthographic units

Speech Perception in Dyslexic Children. With and Without Language Impairments. Franklin R. Manis. University of Southern California.

Florida Reading Endorsement Alignment Matrix Competency 1

CLASSIFICATION OF PROGRAM Critical Elements Analysis 1. High Priority Items Phonemic Awareness Instruction

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

SLINGERLAND: A Multisensory Structured Language Instructional Approach

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

Enhancing Phonological Awareness, Print Awareness, and Oral Language Skills in Preschool Children

1 st Quarter (September, October, November) August/September Strand Topic Standard Notes Reading for Literature

The Impact of Morphological Awareness on Iranian University Students Listening Comprehension Ability

Age Effects on Syntactic Control in. Second Language Learning

Tier 2 Literacy: Matching Instruction & Intervention to Student Needs

Correspondence between the DRDP (2015) and the California Preschool Learning Foundations. Foundations (PLF) in Language and Literacy

First Grade Curriculum Highlights: In alignment with the Common Core Standards

A Note on Structuring Employability Skills for Accounting Students

The influence of orthographic transparency on word recognition. by dyslexic and normal readers

Houghton Mifflin Reading Correlation to the Common Core Standards for English Language Arts (Grade1)

Phonological encoding in speech production

How to Judge the Quality of an Objective Classroom Test

The Perception of Nasalized Vowels in American English: An Investigation of On-line Use of Vowel Nasalization in Lexical Access

ELA/ELD Standards Correlation Matrix for ELD Materials Grade 1 Reading

Understanding and Supporting Dyslexia Godstone Village School. January 2017

Exploring Dyslexics Phonological Deficit I: Lexical vs Sub-lexical and Input vs Output Processes

To appear in The TESOL encyclopedia of ELT (Wiley-Blackwell) 1 RECASTING. Kazuya Saito. Birkbeck, University of London

A Critique of Running Records

Technical Report #1. Summary of Decision Rules for Intensive, Strategic, and Benchmark Instructional

DIBELS Next BENCHMARK ASSESSMENTS

Unraveling symbolic number processing and the implications for its association with mathematics. Delphine Sasanguie

Taught Throughout the Year Foundational Skills Reading Writing Language RF.1.2 Demonstrate understanding of spoken words,

The Journey to Vowelerria VOWEL ERRORS: THE LOST WORLD OF SPEECH INTERVENTION. Preparation: Education. Preparation: Education. Preparation: Education

Books Effective Literacy Y5-8 Learning Through Talk Y4-8 Switch onto Spelling Spelling Under Scrutiny

UCLA Issues in Applied Linguistics

Running Head: PASS theory of intelligence in Greek 1. PASS theory of intelligence in Greek: A review

Cognitive bases of reading and writing in a second/foreign language. DIALUKI (

Investigating speech perception in children with dyslexia: is there evidence of a. consistent deficit in individuals? Abstract

Beeson, P. M. (1999). Treating acquired writing impairment. Aphasiology, 13,

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

ORTHOGRAPHIC ANALOGY TRAINING WITH KINDERGARTEN CHILDREN: EFFECTS ON ANALOGY USE, PHONEMIC SEGMENTATION, AND LETTER-SOUND KNOWLEDGE

English Language and Applied Linguistics. Module Descriptions 2017/18

Primary English Curriculum Framework

TEKS Comments Louisiana GLE

Phonology Revisited: Sor3ng Out the PH Factors in Reading and Spelling Development. Indiana, November, 2015

Get Your Hands On These Multisensory Reading Strategies

Ekapeli (in Finnish), GraphoGame (internationally)

Identifying the training needs of EFL teachers in teaching children with dyslexia

Wonderland Charter School 2112 Sandy Drive State College, PA 16803

Development of the Grammar and Phonology Screening (GAPS) test to assess key markers of specific language and literacy difficulties in young children

The Efficacy of PCI s Reading Program - Level One: A Report of a Randomized Experiment in Brevard Public Schools and Miami-Dade County Public Schools

Merbouh Zouaoui. Melouk Mohamed. Journal of Educational and Social Research MCSER Publishing, Rome-Italy. 1. Introduction

Candidates must achieve a grade of at least C2 level in each examination in order to achieve the overall qualification at C2 Level.

Linguistics. Undergraduate. Departmental Honors. Graduate. Faculty. Linguistics 1

THE INFLUENCE OF TASK DEMANDS ON FAMILIARITY EFFECTS IN VISUAL WORD RECOGNITION: A COHORT MODEL PERSPECTIVE DISSERTATION

Curriculum Vitae. Sara C. Steele, Ph.D, CCC-SLP 253 McGannon Hall 3750 Lindell Blvd., St. Louis, MO Tel:

Language Acquisition by Identical vs. Fraternal SLI Twins * Karin Stromswold & Jay I. Rifkin

Word Stress and Intonation: Introduction

A Study of Metacognitive Awareness of Non-English Majors in L2 Listening

Algebra 1, Quarter 3, Unit 3.1. Line of Best Fit. Overview

STAFF DEVELOPMENT in SPECIAL EDUCATION

Large Kindergarten Centers Icons

Recommended Guidelines for the Diagnosis of Children with Learning Disabilities

Reading Horizons. A Look At Linguistic Readers. Nicholas P. Criscuolo APRIL Volume 10, Issue Article 5

Orthographic analysis of words during fluency training promotes reading of new similar words

1. REFLEXES: Ask questions about coughing, swallowing, of water as fast as possible (note! Not suitable for all

Criterion Met? Primary Supporting Y N Reading Street Comprehensive. Publisher Citations

The process of acquiring reading skills. John Munro 1

Phonemic Awareness. Jennifer Gondek Instructional Specialist for Inclusive Education TST BOCES

have to be modeled) or isolated words. Output of the system is a grapheme-tophoneme conversion system which takes as its input the spelling of words,

Test Blueprint. Grade 3 Reading English Standards of Learning

The Acquisition of Person and Number Morphology Within the Verbal Domain in Early Greek

Sector Differences in Student Learning: Differences in Achievement Gains Across School Years and During the Summer

OVERVIEW OF CURRICULUM-BASED MEASUREMENT AS A GENERAL OUTCOME MEASURE

5. UPPER INTERMEDIATE

Rachel E. Baker, Ann R. Bradlow. Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA

Is rapid automatized naming automatic?

Arizona s English Language Arts Standards th Grade ARIZONA DEPARTMENT OF EDUCATION HIGH ACADEMIC STANDARDS FOR STUDENTS

BENCHMARK TREND COMPARISON REPORT:

Chapter 5. The Components of Language and Reading Instruction

Multisensory Teaching Approach for Reading, Spelling, and Handwriting, Orton-Gillingham Based Curriculum, in a Public School Setting

Does training in syllable recognition improve reading speed? A computer-based trial with poor

THE PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIVERSITY SCHREYER HONORS COLLEGE DEPARTMENT OF MATHEMATICS ASSESSING THE EFFECTIVENESS OF MULTIPLE CHOICE MATH TESTS

Speech Recognition at ICSI: Broadcast News and beyond

Understanding the Relationship between Comprehension and Production

WHO ARE SCHOOL PSYCHOLOGISTS? HOW CAN THEY HELP THOSE OUTSIDE THE CLASSROOM? Christine Mitchell-Endsley, Ph.D. School Psychology

Miami-Dade County Public Schools

Conditioned spellings in Danish

L2 studies demonstrate the importance of word recognition skills in reading (Baker,

Levels of processing: Qualitative differences or task-demand differences?

South Carolina English Language Arts

Effects of Vocabulary and Phonotactic Probability on 2-Year-Olds Nonword Repetition

Effect of Word Complexity on L2 Vocabulary Learning

A cautionary note is research still caught up in an implementer approach to the teacher?

GCSE English Language 2012 An investigation into the outcomes for candidates in Wales

SARDNET: A Self-Organizing Feature Map for Sequences

Student Morningness-Eveningness Type and Performance: Does Class Timing Matter?

Transcription:

Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 1998, 39, 149 153 When reading is readn or somthn. Distinctness of phonological representations of lexical items in normal and disabled readers CARSTEN ELBRO Department of General and Applied Linguistics, Uni ersity of Copenhagen, Denmark Elbro, C. (1998). When reading is readn or somthn. Distinctness of phonological representations of lexical items in normal and disabled readers. Scandina ian Journal of Psychology, 39, 149 153. This paper specifies nothing less than an underlying cause of dyslexia. It is a cause which may be responsible for lacking responsiveness to both the teaching of phoneme awareness and to initial reading instruction. Can a single phonological factor explain many of the phonological deficits related to dyslexia? In this paper it is suggested that the answer may be affirmative and that indistinct phonological representations of lexical items in long term memory may be such a unifying factor. Results are summarised from a correlational study of adults and from a longitudinal and intervention study of at-risk children. Key words: Reading disabilities, dyslexia, linguistic bases, phonological representation, prediction, prevention. Carsten Elbro, Department of General and Applied Linguistics, Uni ersity of Copenhagen, Njalsgade 86, DK-2300 Copenhagen S, Denmark. Email: ce@cphling.dk. DISTINCTNESS OF PHONOLOGICAL REPRESENTATIONS Problems in establishing complete, full, clear, or precise phonological representations in long-term memory have frequently been mentioned as a possible unifying cause of the diverse phonological difficulties characteristic of dyslexia (Constable et al., in press; de Gelder & Vroomen, 1991; Fowler, 1991; Katz, 1986; 1996 Snowling et al., 1988; Swan & Goswami, submitted). Arguably, poor phonological representations may be a cuase of both difficulties with phonological discrimination, picture naming, phonological STM, and phonological awareness (Elbro, 1996; Fowler, 1991). However, supportive evidence has been scattered and suggestive. Neither has the notion of imprecise phonological representations been made clear at all. Distinctness of phonological representations is brought forward here as a useful working concept (see Elbro, 1996, for more details). Distinctness refers to the magnitude of the difference between a representation and its neighbours. For example and pronounced [ænd] (full form, as in the most frequent word is and ) is more distinct than [ n] (reduced form, as in Peter and Lynette ), because [ n] is indistinguishable from the pronunciation of unstressed an. In terms of a structural theory of phonological representation, distinctness may be measured by the number of features that distinguish a representation from its neighbours. Phonological representations with many distinctive features are, on average, more distinct than representations with fewer distinctive features. A central assumption of the distinctness hypothesis (Elbro, 1996; Elbro et al., submitted) links distinctness to completeness (and accuracy) of phonological representations. The distinctness of a lexical representation is a determinant, among others, of the completeness and accuracy of the representation, the ease of access to the representation and to sublexical phonological units within the representation. While words represented at very low levels of distinctness are likely to be mispronounced, words represented at higher levels of distinctness may be pronounced conventionally (correctly), but not necessarily in their most distinct forms. For instance, platnum (platinum) is less distinct than platenum even though both pronunciations are conventional (correct). The distinctness hypothesis concerning dyslexia proposes that children who experience severe difficulties in learning the alphabetic principle in reading possess relatively indistinct phonological representations, or have other difficulties in accessing distinct phonological representations. Evidence to support the distinctness hypothesis is summarised below from three studies of dyslexic and normal readers. Notice here that distinctness is an aspect of the quality of phonological material. It should thus be distinguished from phonological processes, such as phonological discrimination, phonological working memory, phonological retrieval, phonological awareness and articulation. A central assumption of the distinctness hypothesis is that distinctness of phonological representations influences the speed and accuracy of different phonological processes in various ways and to varying degrees. In particular, it is hypothesised that low levels of distinctness is a serious impediment to the access to sublexical segments (such as units of phoneme size). Hence, the distinctness hypothesis suggests that reduced distinctness levels may be a cause of the well-known phonological processing difficulties in dyslexia. Published by Blackwell Publishers, 108 Cowley Road, Oxford OX4 1JF, UK and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA. ISSN 0036-5564.

150 C. Elbro Scand J Psychol 39 (1998) First, preschool children who later develop reading difficulties have shown to possess relatively low levels of phonological and phonemic awareness (e.g. Catts, 1991; Lundberg et al., 1980; Mann & Liberman, 1984). Such difficulties with awareness of speech sounds may be caused by indistinct phonological representations. While a phonological representation may contain sufficient information to produce and identify a particular word, the same representation may be too indistinct for easy access to sublexical units (Elbro, 1996). Second, even access to whole word phonology may be impeded by indistinct phonological representations. It seems reasonable to assume that it is easier to get access to a phonological representation that is well specified and clearly separated from its neighbours than to a phonological representation that is incompletely specified and less well separated from its neighbours. In this way, the distinctness hypothesis may also account for the naming speed deficits observed in association with dyslexia (e.g. Katz, 1986, 1996; Bowers & Wolf, 1993). Third, low phonological distinctness may also be linked to phonological processing in early reading in a more direct manner. During the initial phases of reading development, the child must internalise writing-to-sound correspondences only a small proportion of which are taught directly. The child has to learn implicitly many letter patterns, such as -ight or -tion, i.e. letter strings which are pronounced in the same way in several words but which are pronounced rather unpredictably given the single letter sounds. If such implicit learning is to take place, the child must have access to the segments of spoken words which correspond to the letter strings that form the letter pattern. And this is where distinctness of phonological representations comes in. A child with indistinct phonological representations will have a smaller chance than other children of automatically setting up such complex letter-sound-correspondences, simply because the sublexical sound patterns are not easily accessible. The following studies were set up to explore the relationships between, in particular, phonological distinctness, phonemic awareness, and reading development in normal and dyslexic persons. STUDY 1. DISTINCTNESS OF PHONOLOGICAL REPRESENTATIONS IN DYSLEXIC ADULTS Distinctness of phonological representations was studied in dyslexic adults by means of measures of both receptive and productive language abilities. The study has previously been reported by Elbro et al. (1994) with an additional discussion of the definition of dyslexia in adults by Elbro (in press). The present summary focuses only on the tasks which were tapping distinctness. It is based on the data from the 1994-study with the addition of some adults who have been interviewed at a later point. The participants were 121 adults with a history of difficulties in learning to read and with present problems in phonological coding in reading (non-word reading). They were compared to 62 normal controls (spouses of the dyslexic adults). The average age was 36 years (range 24 to 56 years). The educational level was approximately two years higher in the control group than in the dyslexic group, but the average educational level across both groups was close to the population mean. Two ocabulary tests were used: one taxing semantic word knowledge and one taxing phonological word knowledge. In the semantic test subjects heard a spoken word and were asked to point to the correct one out of three pictured objects from the same semantic field (e.g. kayak, canoe, and dinghy ). In the phonological test subjects heard a spoken word or a phrase and were asked to select a synonym from a choice of three similarly sounding spoken words (e.g. capital punishment, is that the same as excursion, execution, or excavation?). No printed words were shown. In another task, the adults were asked to pronounce printed words as they would do if reading to their children. The words were embedded in real sentences, they were polysyllabic and each contained a syllable which could be pronounced correctly at different levels of distinctness, e.g. with a full vowel [plæt n m] (an over-distinct pronunciation of platinum), with a long consonant [plætn: m], or entirely without the syllable [plætn m]. Details of the tasks, their administration and scoring are given in Elbro et al., 1994. The dyslexic adults were outperformed by the normal readers in all tasks (Table 1). The differences between groups were still significant even when covarying for differences in education and daily reading practices. So even though the dyslexic adults had some difficulties in reading the words in the pronunciation tasks, they tended to pronounce the target words at a slightly lower level of distinctness. Furthermore, a very robust interaction was found between the two word knowledge tests and the two groups (F(1,181)=32.9, p 0.001). This interaction was still significant even after covarying for educational level, daily Table 1. Linguistic abilities in adults with both self-reported reading difficulties and non-word reading difficulties, compared with normally reading adults with no history of reading difficulties. Mean scores are presented with standard de iations in parentheses. Reading status Dyslexia Normal Significant (N=121) (N=62) F(1,181) Word knowledge, 17.6 (2.1) 18.5 (1.6) 9.0** semantic (max 21) Word knowledge, 12.4 (2.6) 15.6 (1.8) 74.1*** phonetic (max 19) Distinctness of 2.30 (2.04) 3.82 (2.56) 9.4** pronunciation (max 8) N=86, **p 0.01, ***p 0.001.

Scand J Psychol 39 (1998) reading practices, and phonological awareness. This interaction and the simple group comparisons suggest that the dyslexic adults were specifically impaired as to the distinctness of phonological representations. Since the study did not include any longitudinal data, it is not possible to make inferences about cause and effect. Indistinct phonological representations may be a cause of reading difficulties, but the opposite may just as well be the case. In order to rule out the possibility that differences in reading were the sole cause of differences in phonological distinctness, a prospective longitudinal study was conducted. STUDY 2. POOR PHONOLOGICAL REPRESENTATIONS PREDICT DYSLEXIA IN CHILDREN The development of language and reading abilities were studied in children of the families with dyslexia (see Study 1) and in control children from families without dyslexia. Presently, we have followed a total of 159 children (87 at-risk children and 72 controls) from the onset of the kindergarten grade, when children are on average six years old in Denmark, until the beginning of the second grade (two years later). Study 2 was concerned with the early prediction of reading difficulties in these children. A report based on the older half of these children is published by Elbro et al., (1998) and some of the results below are from this report. At the onset of the kindergarten grade, several tests of language abilities, pre-reading abilities, and two general tests of cognitive abilities were administered to the children. Among the language tests were tests of phoneme awareness, syllable awareness, and morpheme awareness in addition to tests of phoneme discrimination, verbal short term memory (digit span from WISC-R), lexical access (picture naming), and articulatory fluency (repeat ba-ga-da as quickly as possible). In an attempt to tap the quality of phonological representations as directly as possible, a new task was developed for this study. In this task the child was invited to take the role of a speech therapist. The child was shown a hand-held puppet and asked to help the puppet pronounce words correctly. On behalf of the puppet the experimenter named pictured objects at a very low level of distinctness, e.g. codi for crocodile. The puppet was even said to be hard of hearing so that the child had to be careful to pronounce the words very clearly for the puppet. The children s responses were scored for accuracy (percent conventional pronunciations), distinctness (based on selected vowel segments, e.g. the unstressed vowels in crocodile) and proximity to orthography (see Elbro et al., 1998, for details of items, procedure and scoring). At the beginning of the second grade, the children were given a number of reading tests, among which were two tests of phonological recoding (reading of pronounceable Phonological distinctness 151 nonwords). Similarly to the definition of dyslexia in adults (see Elbro et al., 1994; Lyon, 1995) a cutoff definition of dyslexia was employed for the children based on averaged z-scores across the two reading measures. Children were considered possibly dyslexic if they scored below the tenth percentile in the not-at-risk group. Analyses of variance indicated that all language measures in kindergarten were significant predictors of possible dyslexia, whereas non-verbal IQ (Raven s progressive matrices) did not predict dyslexia. A series of logistic regression analyses were conducted in order to isolate measures that contributed independently to the prediction of dyslexia. Three measures were found: letter knowledge (naming letters), phoneme awareness, and distinctness of phonological representations. This result suggests that distinctness of phonological representations contributes independent variance to the prediction of dyslexia over and above the contribution of the standard predictors. And it is noteworthy that the inclusion of articulatory fluency and lexical access (measured in a picture naming task) did not explain away the contribution of the distinctness measure to the early prediction of dyslexia. In sum, it appears that variation in distinctness of phonological representations does in fact contribute to the prediction of early reading development. STUDY 3. UNDER SPECIFIED PHONOLOGICAL REPRESENTATIONS PREDICT DEVELOPMENT OF PHONEME AWARENESS IN THE KINDERGARTEN GRADE A number of studies have indicated that dyslexia may to some extent be prevented in kindergarten through activities that require phoneme awareness (e.g. Borstro m & Elbro, 1997; Brady et al., 1994; Byrne et al., 1997; Gillam & van Kleeck et al., 1996; Lundberg 1994; Torgesen et al., 1994). Children who would otherwise develop initial reading skills only very slowly appear to get a more smooth transition into literacy when they are made aware of some of the speech segments of phoneme size before the first grade. Consequently, a next goal for research in prevention of dyslexia would be to predict which children will be resistant to early training of phoneme awareness. Such a goal also fit well with the search for the roots of the development of phoneme awareness. In pursuit of these goals we conducted a training study with a randomly selected subgroup of the children of dyslexic families (from studies 1 and 2). Thirty-six of the at-risk children (from 27 different classes) participated in an intensive 17 week program in their regular kindergarten classes designed to help them improve in phoneme awareness. Two control groups consisted of 52 untrained at-risk children (from 44 different classes) and 48 untrained not-atrisk children (from 7 classes). Training effects were found in phoneme awareness and in letter knowledge with positive

152 C. Elbro Scand J Psychol 39 (1998) long term effects on reading of both words and nonwords (Borstro m & Elbro, 1997). Individual gains in phoneme awareness over the kindergarten year were computed as a simple sum of scores in the phoneme identification and phoneme deletion tasks. And correlation analyses indicated that the individual accuracy of phonological representations of lexical items was a significant predictor of gains in phoneme awareness in both trained at-risk children and in normal controls (r=0.55 and r=0.44, respectively, p 0.001 for both). The gains in phoneme awareness in the untrained at-risk children were too small to provide reliable individual differences. The correlations in the two other groups remained significant even when other significant predictors were controlled for (Elbro et al., submitted). Awareness of larger sublexical units (syllables and morphemes) did not predict gains in phoneme awareness. These results suggest that the quality of phonological representations may indeed set the stage for the development of phoneme awareness that is so important for learning to read. While accuracy of phonological representations was found to be a significant predictor in this study, distinctness of phonological representations seemed to be the more important predictor of dyslexia (Study 2). While the two measures of phonological quality are certainly not one and the same, we suggest that they are strongly related. DISCUSSION: THE PHONOLOGICAL DISTINCTNESS HYPOTHESIS As mentioned in the introduction, accuracy and distinctness may be seen as two ends of the same scale. Words represented at very low levels of distinctness are under specified which means that they are likely to be mispronounced and confused with other, similar sounding words. Conversely, variation at high levels of distinctness will typically result in pronunications within the normal range of variation, yet not all at the most distinct level. The distinctness hypothesis (Elbro, 1996; Elbro et al., submitted), proposes that the distinctness of a lexical representation is a determinant, among others, of the completeness and accuracy of the representation, and of the ease of access to sublexical phonological units of the representation. With respect to individual variability, the distinctness model suggests that the level of distinctness of phonological representations of lexical items vary within individuals and between individuals. As a consequence, some children may possess an age-appropriate vocabulary and be able to pronounce and perceive words fairly normally, but still have problems with tasks that require access to sublexical units. These children will also be unlikely to be able to pronounce the same words at varying levels of distinctness to meet the demands of changing conditions of communication and changing listener characteristics. So far however, studies of the quality of phonological representations are scarce. In-depth case studies might be one way to proceed in order to map the detailed relations between words represented at varying degrees of distinctness and the ease of access to their sublexical units. Training studies would also be needed for a more direct test of the claim that low distinctness is an underlying cause of difficulties in the development of phoneme awareness. Finally, and most ambitiously, converging evidence should be sought across languages and educational systems. The research reported here was supported by grants from The Danish Research Council (5-25-98-85) and from the Rebekka foundation. REFERENCES Borstro m, I. & Elbro, C. (1997). Prevention of dyslexia in kindergarten: Effects of phoneme awareness training with children of dyslexic parents. In C. Hulme & M. Snowling (Eds.), Dyslexia: Biology, Cognition and Inter ention (pp. 235 253). London: Whurr. Bowers, P. G. & Wolf, M. (1993). Theoretical links among naming speed, precise timing mechanisms and orthographic skills in dyslexia. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 5, 69 85. Brady, S., Fowler, A., Stone, B. & Winbury, N. (1994). Training phonological awareness: A study with inner-city kindergarten children. Annals of Dyslexia, 44, 26 59. Byrne, B., Fielding-Barnsley, R., Ashley, L. & Larsen, K. (1997). Assessing the child s and the environment s contribution to reading acquisition: What we know and what we don t know. In B. Blachman (ed), Foundations of reading acquisition and dyslexia: Implications for early inter ention. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Catts, H. W. (1991). Early identification of dyslexia: Evidence from a follow-up study of speech-language impaired children. Annals of Dyslexia, 41, 163 177. Constable, A., Stackhouse, J. & Wells, B. (in press). Developmental word finding difficulties and phonological processing: The case of the missing handcuffs. Applied Psycholinguistics. de Gelder, B. & Vroomen, J. (1991). Phonological deficits: Beneath the surface of reading-acquisition problems. Psychological Research, 53, 88 97. Elbro, C. (1996). Early linguisitic abilities and reading development: A review and a hypothesis. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 8, 453 485. Elbro, C. (in press). Reading-listening discrepancy definitions of dyslexia. In P. Reitsma & L. Verhoeven (eds.), Problems and inter entions in literacy de elopment. Dordrecht: Kluwer. Elbro, C., Borstro m, I. & Petersen, D. K. (1998). Predicting dyslexia from kindergarten. The importance of distinctness of phonological representations of lexical items. Reading Research Quarterly, 33 (1), 36 60. Elbro, C., Borstro m, I., & Petersen, D. K. (submitted). Causes and consequences of the development of phoneme awareness: A longitudinal and intervention study of normal and at-risk children. Copenhagen: University of Copenhagen. Elbro, C., Nielsen, I. & Petersen, D. K. (1994). Dyslexia in adults: Evidence for deficits in nonword reading and in the phonological representation of lexical items. Annals of Dyslexia, 44, 205 226.

Scand J Psychol 39 (1998) Fowler, A. E. (1991). How early phonological development might set the stage for phoneme awareness. In S. Brady & D. Shankweiler (Eds), Phonological processes in literacy: A tribute to Isabelle Y. Liberman (pp. 97 118). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Gillam, R. B. & van Kleeck, A. (1996). Phonological awareness training and short-term working memory: Clinical implications. Topics in Language Disorders, 17, 72 81. Katz, R.B. (1986). Phonological deficiencies in children with reading disability: Evidence from an object-naming task. Cognition, 22, 225 257. Katz, R. B. (1996). Phonological and semantic factors in the object-naming errors of skilled and less skilled readers. Annals of Dyslexia, 46, 189 208. Lundberg, I. (1994). Reading difficulties can be predicted and prevented: A Scandinavian perspective on phonological awareness and reading. In C. Hulme & M. Snowling (Eds.), Reading de elopment and dyslexia (pp. 180 199). London: Whurr Publishers. Phonological distinctness 153 Lundberg I., Olofsson, Å. & Wall, S. (1980). Reading and spelling skills in the first school years predicted from phonemic awareness skills in kindergarten. Scandina ian Journal of Psychology, 21, 159 173. Lyon, G. R. (1995). Towards a definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 45, 3 27. Mann, V. A. & Liberman, I. Y. (1984). Phonological awareness and verbal short-term memory: can they presage early reading problems? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 17, 592 599. Snowling, M., Wagtendonk, B. & Stafford, C. (1988). Object-naming deficits in developmental dyslexia. Journal of Research in Reading, 11, 67 85. Swan, D., & Goswami, U. (submitted). Phonological awareness deficits in developmental dsylexia and the phonological representations hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K. & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). Longitudinal studies of phonological processing and reading. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27, 276 286. Accepted 20 January 1998.