Déjà Vu All Over Again: A Response to Saito, Horwitz, and Garza

Similar documents
Roya Movahed 1. Correspondence: Roya Movahed, English Department, University of Zabol, Zabol, Iran.

ROSETTA STONE PRODUCT OVERVIEW

The Impact of Morphological Awareness on Iranian University Students Listening Comprehension Ability

L2 studies demonstrate the importance of word recognition skills in reading (Baker,

Developing phonological awareness: Is there a bilingual advantage?

Cognitive bases of reading and writing in a second/foreign language. DIALUKI (

SLINGERLAND: A Multisensory Structured Language Instructional Approach

Florida Reading Endorsement Alignment Matrix Competency 1

A Study of Metacognitive Awareness of Non-English Majors in L2 Listening

Age Effects on Syntactic Control in. Second Language Learning

ORTHOGRAPHIC ANALOGY TRAINING WITH KINDERGARTEN CHILDREN: EFFECTS ON ANALOGY USE, PHONEMIC SEGMENTATION, AND LETTER-SOUND KNOWLEDGE

Learning and Retaining New Vocabularies: The Case of Monolingual and Bilingual Dictionaries

Multisensory Teaching Approach for Reading, Spelling, and Handwriting, Orton-Gillingham Based Curriculum, in a Public School Setting

Listening and Speaking Skills of English Language of Adolescents of Government and Private Schools

The Effect of Extensive Reading on Developing the Grammatical. Accuracy of the EFL Freshmen at Al Al-Bayt University

CLASSIFICATION OF PROGRAM Critical Elements Analysis 1. High Priority Items Phonemic Awareness Instruction

A Critique of Running Records

Second Language Acquisition in Adults: From Research to Practice

University of New Orleans

Stages of Literacy Ros Lugg

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

To appear in The TESOL encyclopedia of ELT (Wiley-Blackwell) 1 RECASTING. Kazuya Saito. Birkbeck, University of London

Longitudinal family-risk studies of dyslexia: why. develop dyslexia and others don t.

The Ohio State University. Colleges of the Arts and Sciences. Bachelor of Science Degree Requirements. The Aim of the Arts and Sciences

Busuu The Mobile App. Review by Musa Nushi & Homa Jenabzadeh, Introduction. 30 TESL Reporter 49 (2), pp

Effect of Word Complexity on L2 Vocabulary Learning

SCHEMA ACTIVATION IN MEMORY FOR PROSE 1. Michael A. R. Townsend State University of New York at Albany

ELP in whole-school use. Case study Norway. Anita Nyberg

Assessment and Evaluation

Miriam Muñiz-Swicegood Arizona State University West. Abstract

The role of the first language in foreign language learning. Paul Nation. The role of the first language in foreign language learning

The Effect of Personality Factors on Learners' View about Translation

Linguistics. Undergraduate. Departmental Honors. Graduate. Faculty. Linguistics 1

Lower and Upper Secondary

The Effect of Discourse Markers on the Speaking Production of EFL Students. Iman Moradimanesh

Exams: Accommodations Guidelines. English Language Learners

Syntactic and Lexical Simplification: The Impact on EFL Listening Comprehension at Low and High Language Proficiency Levels

What is PDE? Research Report. Paul Nichols

English Language and Applied Linguistics. Module Descriptions 2017/18

Running head: METACOGNITIVE STRATEGIES FOR ACADEMIC LISTENING 1. The Relationship between Metacognitive Strategies Awareness

STAFF DEVELOPMENT in SPECIAL EDUCATION

UCLA Issues in Applied Linguistics

Language Center. Course Catalog

Roadmap to College: Highly Selective Schools

ABSTRACT. Some children with speech sound disorders (SSD) have difficulty with literacyrelated

The Singapore Copyright Act applies to the use of this document.

The influence of orthographic transparency on word recognition. by dyslexic and normal readers

5. UPPER INTERMEDIATE

Correspondence between the DRDP (2015) and the California Preschool Learning Foundations. Foundations (PLF) in Language and Literacy

Sheila M. Smith is Assistant Professor, Department of Business Information Technology, College of Business, Ball State University, Muncie, Indiana.

Aviation English Training: How long Does it Take?

Positive turning points for girls in mathematics classrooms: Do they stand the test of time?

Reading Horizons. A Look At Linguistic Readers. Nicholas P. Criscuolo APRIL Volume 10, Issue Article 5

Wonderland Charter School 2112 Sandy Drive State College, PA 16803

Identifying the training needs of EFL teachers in teaching children with dyslexia

Improving Advanced Learners' Communication Skills Through Paragraph Reading and Writing. Mika MIYASONE

Observing Teachers: The Mathematics Pedagogy of Quebec Francophone and Anglophone Teachers

THE ACQUISITION OF INFLECTIONAL MORPHEMES: THE PRIORITY OF PLURAL S

First Grade Curriculum Highlights: In alignment with the Common Core Standards

Handbook for Graduate Students in TESL and Applied Linguistics Programs

Language Anxiety, Acculturation, and L2 Self: A Relational Analysis in the Taiwanese Cultural Context

MFL SPECIFICATION FOR JUNIOR CYCLE SHORT COURSE

Running head: LISTENING COMPREHENSION OF UNIVERSITY REGISTERS 1

Automatization and orthographic development in second language visual word recognition

The Effects of Super Speed 100 on Reading Fluency. Jennifer Thorne. University of New England

Dyslexia/dyslexic, 3, 9, 24, 97, 187, 189, 206, 217, , , 367, , , 397,

A Decent Proposal for Bilingual Education at International Standard Schools/SBI in Indonesia

Text and task authenticity in the EFL classroom

Tour. English Discoveries Online

Modern Languages. Introduction. Degrees Offered

Maximizing Learning Through Course Alignment and Experience with Different Types of Knowledge

Technical Report #1. Summary of Decision Rules for Intensive, Strategic, and Benchmark Instructional

Section V Reclassification of English Learners to Fluent English Proficient

Effects of connecting reading and writing and a checklist to guide the reading process on EFL learners learning about English writing

International Conference on Education and Educational Psychology (ICEEPSY 2012)

Ling/Span/Fren/Ger/Educ 466: SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION. Spring 2011 (Tuesdays 4-6:30; Psychology 251)

Ekapeli (in Finnish), GraphoGame (internationally)

English-German Medical Dictionary And Phrasebook By A.H. Zemback

Special Educational Needs and Disabilities Policy Taverham and Drayton Cluster

Organizing Comprehensive Literacy Assessment: How to Get Started

Creating Travel Advice

Recommended Guidelines for the Diagnosis of Children with Learning Disabilities

DEPARTMENT OF JAPANESE LANGUAGE AND STUDIES

The Impact of Formative Assessment and Remedial Teaching on EFL Learners Listening Comprehension N A H I D Z A R E I N A S TA R A N YA S A M I

Let's Learn English Lesson Plan

prehending general textbooks, but are unable to compensate these problems on the micro level in comprehending mathematical texts.

Applying ADDIE Model for Research and Development: An Analysis Phase of Communicative Language of 9 Grad Students

Basic German: CD/Book Package (LL(R) Complete Basic Courses) By Living Language

Criterion Met? Primary Supporting Y N Reading Street Comprehensive. Publisher Citations

Match or Mismatch Between Learning Styles of Prep-Class EFL Students and EFL Teachers

Afsaneh Rahimi Tehrani University of Isfahan, Isfahan, Iran. Hossein Barati English Department, University of Isfahan, Isfahan, Iran

Understanding the Relationship between Comprehension and Production

Kelli Allen. Vicki Nieter. Jeanna Scheve. Foreword by Gregory J. Kaiser

The EDI contains five core domains which are described in Table 1. These domains are further divided into sub-domains.

1 st Quarter (September, October, November) August/September Strand Topic Standard Notes Reading for Literature

The impact of E-dictionary strategy training on EFL class

Linguistics. The School of Humanities

Abdul Rahman Chik a*, Tg. Ainul Farha Tg. Abdul Rahman b

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Enhancing Phonological Awareness, Print Awareness, and Oral Language Skills in Preschool Children

Transcription:

MLJ Readers Forum Déjà Vu All Over Again: A Response to Saito, Horwitz, and Garza RICHARD L. SPARKS College of Mount St. Joseph Education Department 5701 Delhi Road Cincinnati, OH 45233 1670 Email: richard_sparks@mail.msj.edu LEONORE GANSCHOW Miami University Department of Educational Psychology Oxford, OH 45056 Email: lganschow@aol.com JAMES JAVORSKY Oakland University Department of Special Education Rochester, MI 48309 Email: javorsky@oakland.edu SEVERAL YEARS AGO, TWO OFUS PUBLISHED a manuscript in this journal entitled Foreign Language Learning Differences: Affective or Native Language Aptitude Differences? (Sparks & Ganschow, 1991). In that manuscript, we introduced to the foreign language (FL) field the Linguistic Coding Differences Hypothesis (LCDH) and responded to a manuscript about anxiety and FL learning by Horwitz, Horwitz, and Cope (1986). In the LCDH, FL learning is based primarily on one s native language learning ability (i.e., language aptitude), students anxiety about FL learning is likely to be a consequence of their FL learning difficulties, and students language learning ability is a confounding variable when studying the impact of affective differences (e.g., anxiety, motivation, attitude) on FL learning. We suggested that any hypothesis that views affective variables as causal factors in learning a FL must be approached with caution. Since that time, we have conducted numerous empirical studies that have supported the basic premises of the LCDH (for a review of these studies, see Ganschow & Sparks, in press; Ganschow, Sparks, & Javorsky, 1998; and Sparks, Ganschow, Artzer, Siebenhar, & Plageman, The Modern Language Journal, 84, ii, (2000) 0026-7902/00/251 259 $1.50/0 2000 The Modern Language Journal 1998). Other researchers have conducted studies that support the tenets of the LCDH (e.g., Humes- Bartlo, 1989; Olshtain, Shohamy, Kemp, & Chatow, 1990; Papagno, Valentine, & Baddeley, 1991; Service, 1992; Service & Kohonen, 1995). In a recent article published in this journal, Saito, Horwitz, and Garza (MLJ, 83, 1999, pp. 202 218) introduced the concept of FL reading anxiety and reported the results of a preliminary study of reading anxiety. In that study, they found that reading a FL was anxiety-provoking for some students, that reading anxiety varied by target language, that FL reading anxiety increased with students perceptions of difficulty in reading the FL, and that students FL grades decreased in conjunction with their levels of FL reading anxiety and general FL anxiety. Some of Saito et al. s findings (e.g., reading a FL was anxiety-provoking for some students) are not surprising. However, their explanation for these findings, for example, that anxiety is responsible for a decrease in FL grades, is misguided in our view. In fact, Saito et al. s hypotheses about FL reading anxiety are almost identical to Horwitz et al. s (1986) hypotheses about general FL anxiety. For several years, we have studied the issues of FL learning problems and also affective differences, particularly anxiety, in FL learners. Based on our research findings, we have taken a position contrary to Saito et al. s on

252 The Modern Language Journal 84 (2000) the issue of anxiety and FL learning. Our response focuses on four points: evidence from recent research in reading acquisition, problems inherent in the authors Foreign Language Reading Anxiety Scale (FLRAS), problems in research design, and failure to control for students level of language ability in research studies on affective variables. RESEARCH IN READING ACQUISITION There is a considerable body of research on reading acquisition in both English and other languages. Findings show clearly that reading words and reading comprehension are based on students skill in processing language. It is well known, for example, that reading acquisition in English is best predicted by phonological processing skills, in particular phonemic awareness, and that failure to develop phonemic awareness leads directly to significant problems with word decoding and indirectly to reading comprehension problems. (For a review of this literature, see Gough, Ehri, & Treiman, 1992; Rieben & Perfetti, 1991; and Stanovich, 1988, 1998.) The relationship between reading difficulty and poor phonological processing skills is also apparent in other languages, such as Danish, French, German, Greek, Hebrew, Italian, Portuguese, Spanish, Turkish, and Swedish (e.g., Alegria, Pignot, & Morais, 1982; Caravolas & Bruck, 1993; Cossu, Shankweiler, Liberman, Katz, & Tola, 1988; Leong & Joshi, 1997; Olofsson & Niedersoe, 1999; Oney & Durgunoglu, 1997; Porpodas, 1999). Even in nonalphabetic languages (e.g., Chinese), research has shown that phonological processing skills are important for learning to read (Hu & Catts, 1998; Suk-Han-Ho & Bryant, 1997). Furthermore, findings indicate that the development of word decoding and reading comprehension are similar in first language (L1) and second language (L2; Geva, 1999) and that FL word recognition (i.e., word decoding) plays a significant role in FL reading comprehension (Koda, 1992, 1996). Thus, Saito et al. s (1999) findings that reading is anxiety-provoking to some students and that anxiety increases with students perceptions of their reading difficulties are not unexpected. However, their suggestion that FL reading problems might be due to anxiety is premature because they did not determine their participants level of reading skill. Our research has shown consistently that students with significantly lower levels of native language skills (i.e., reading, spelling, vocabulary) and FL proficiency have higher levels of anxiety about FL learning (Ganschow et al., 1994; Ganschow & Sparks, 1996; Sparks & Ganschow, 1996; Sparks, Ganschow, Artzer, Siebenhar, & Plageman, 1997). FOREIGN LANGUAGE READING ANXIETY SCALE An examination of questions on the FLRAS indicates a possible confounding interaction between language skills and anxiety. This finding is similar to our earlier criticism of Horwitz s Foreign Language Classroom Anxiety Scale (FLCAS; Horwitz et al., 1986), where we suggested that there may be a confounding interaction between an affective variable (e.g., anxiety) and a student s expressive/receptive language skills (i.e., listening, speaking, verbal memory). 1 All 20 questions on the FLRAS involve comfort level with either the decoding (i.e., reading words) or comprehension aspects of reading. For example, item 2, When reading (the FL), I often understand the words but still can t quite understand what the author is saying, indicates potential difficulty with reading comprehension. Item 9, I usually end up translating word by word when I m reading (the FL), indicates potential difficulty with decoding skill. Like Horwitz s earlier and more general anxiety scale, 10% of the questions appear to involve comfort level with decoding only; 35%, comfort level with comprehension only; and 55%, comfort level with both decoding and comprehension. The Appendix presents items on the FLRAS with a breakdown of the reading components we observed. Thus, given the potential for interaction between reading skill and anxiety, the authors of the FLRAS cannot be certain whether their scale is measuring FL reading anxiety, FL reading skill, or both. PROBLEMS IN RESEARCH DESIGN Saito et al. (1999) investigated whether levels of reading anxiety vary according to target language. They concluded that whereas general FL anxiety did not differ by target language, FL reading anxiety did. Although this research question has much merit, the study s design does not allow the authors to answer the question conclusively because the students were not randomly placed in the three FL classes. Thus, the study s internal validity is weakened because group membership was biased; that is, students selected a FL in which to enroll in relation to preference, interest, graduation requirement, and so forth (Campbell & Stanley, 1963; Krathwohl, 1993). Without randomization, the authors cannot assume that the groups were equivalent in language skills, motiva-

Readers Forum 253 tion to enroll in a FL, or level of FL reading anxiety. Any specific conclusion that a given FL is associated with higher levels of FL reading anxiety cannot be supported in this study. MEASURING VARIANCE IN LANGUAGE SKILLS Perhaps the primary problem in Saito et al. s (1999) study is their failure to measure and control for their participants level of native language reading or FL reading skill (i.e., decoding, comprehension). Students level of reading skill could have been measured in several ways, for example, by administering a standardized measure of reading or by using ACT Reading scores. To be fair, Saito et al. did suggest that it is difficult to be sure whether anxiety is the cause or effect of the difficulties observed (p. 215). They also stated that their participants experienced anxiety as a result of actual difficulties in text processing (p. 215). However, tests of reading skills were not administered to the participants; thus, Saito et al. did not determine whether their participants had difficulty with text processing. They also stated that the large number of students who reported anxiety over reading causes us to believe that many of the anxious students have normal language processing capabilities (p. 215). However, this statement is a hypothesis. Saito et al. do not know whether the students in their study had normal language processing capabilities because they did not measure those capabilities. Au (1988) maintained that failure to control for FL proficiency in affective studies is a serious methodological weakness. For several years, we have suggested that FL researchers who study affective variables must measure and control for their participants level of native language skill and FL proficiency. Only by including language variables in their studies will researchers who investigate affective variables be able to discern the contribution made by affective differences in FL learning. 2 WHAT S THE HARM? Saito et al. (1999) reported that they were unable to identify any material on first language reading anxiety even though first language reading difficulties are well-known (p. 203). We suggest that they were unable to locate studies on anxiety and L1 reading skill because anxiety as a causal factor in L1 reading acquisition is not a viable hypothesis. Voluminous evidence has implicated cognitive processes, specifically language skills, as the primary causal factor in L1 reading acquisition and reading problems. At this time, there is also no reason to expect that the relationship between word reading and reading comprehension is different in L1 and L2 (Geva, 1999). Developing effective reading comprehension skills in L2 requires efficient word recognition (decoding) skills and some measure of proficiency in the L2. If anxiety is accepted by FL educators as a primary causal factor in FL reading differences, our concern is that researchers will not develop or seek out new (or different) teaching methodologies that have the potential to teach students with language skill differences. For several years, we have suggested that the largest part of the variance in FL learning is accounted for by language skills because learning a FL is the learning of language (e.g., Sparks & Ganschow, 1995). In our view, FL researchers should include the contribution of language skills when investigating students affective differences in reading and comprehending a FL. NOTES 1 For example, item 1 on the FLCAS is I never feel sure when speaking in class. We suggested that this item could be related to a student s level of oral expressive language skill. We examined each of the 33 questions on the FLCAS and found the following: 60% of the questions involved comfort level with expressive language, receptive language, or both; 15% involved verbal memory skills; and 12% involved speed of processing verbal stimuli. 2 We have suggested that FL researchers who propose that affective variables play a causal role in FL learning could invalidate our theory (the LCDH) by finding that (a) there are no native language differences between students who have affective differences; (b) an unsuccessful FL learner s affective differences do not routinely affect his or her performance in other academic areas, especially language-based subjects (e.g., English, social studies); and (c) students with significantly weaker native language skills and higher levels of anxiety can have their anxiety reduced and still become as proficient in a FL as students with significantly stronger native language skills. REFERENCES Alegria, J., Pignot, E., & Morais, A. (1982). Phonetic analysis of speech and memory codes in beginning readers. Memory and Cognition, 10, 451 456. Au, S. (1988). A critical appraisal of Gardner s socialpsychological theory of second language learning. Language Learning, 38, 75 100.

254 The Modern Language Journal 84 (2000) Campbell, D., & Stanley, J. (1963). Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for research. Chicago: Rand McNally. Caravolas, M., & Bruck, M. (1993). The effect of oral and written language input on children s phonological awareness: A cross-linguistic study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 55, 1 30. Cossu, G., Shankweiler, D., Liberman, I., Katz, L., & Tola, T. (1988). Awareness of phonological segments and reading ability in Italian children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 1 16. Ganschow, L., & Sparks, R. (1996). Anxiety about foreign language learning among high school women. Modern Language Journal, 80, 199 212. Ganschow, L., & Sparks, R. (in press). Reflections on foreign language study for students with language learning problems: Research, issues, and challenges. Dyslexia. Ganschow, L., Sparks, R., Anderson, R., Javorsky, J., Skinner, S., & Patton, J. (1994). Differences in language performance among high-, average-, and low-anxious college foreign language learners. Modern Language Journal, 78, 41 55. Ganschow, L., Sparks, R., & Javorsky, J. (1998). Foreign language learning problems: An historical perspective. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 31, 248 258. Geva, E. (1999, June). Issues in the assessment of reading disabilities in L2 children Beliefs and research evidence. Paper presented at the Multilingualism and Dyslexia Conference, Manchester, England. Gough, P., Ehri, R., & Treiman, R. (Eds.) (1992). Reading acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Horwitz, E., Horwitz, M., & Cope, J. (1986). Foreign language classroom anxiety. Modern Language Journal, 70, 125 132. Hu, C., & Catts, H. (1998). The role of phonological processing in early reading ability: What we can learn from Chinese? Scientific Studies of Reading, 2, 55 80. Humes-Bartlo, M. (1989). Variation in children s ability to learn second languages. In K. Hyltenstam & L. Obler (Eds.), Bilingualism across the lifespan (pp. 41 54). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. Koda, K. (1992). The effects of lower-level processing skills on FL reading performance: Implications for instruction. Modern Language Journal, 76, 502 512. Koda, K. (1996). L2 word recognition research: A critical review. Modern Language Journal, 80, 450 460. Krathwohl, D. (1993). Methods of educational and social science research: An integrated approach. White Plains, NY: Longman. Leong, C. K., & Joshi, R. (1997). Cross language studies of learning to read and spell. Boston, MA: Kluwer. Olofsson, A., & Niedersoe, J. (1999). Early language development and kindergarten phonological awareness as predictors of reading problems: From 3 to 11 years. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 464 472. Olshtain, E., Shohamy, E., Kemp, J., & Chatow, R. (1990). Factors predicting success in EFL children among culturally different learners. Language Learning, 40, 23 44. Oney, B., & Durgunoglu, A. (1997). Beginning to read in Turkish: A phonologically transparent orthography. Applied Psychologuistics, 18, 1 15. Papagno, C., Valentine, T., & Baddeley, A. (1991). Phonological short-term memory and foreign language vocabulary learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 331 347. Porpodas, C. (1999). Patterns of phonological and memory processing in beginning readers and spellers of Greek. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 406 416. Rieben, L., & Perfetti, C. (1991). Learning to read: Basic research and its implications. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Saito, Y., Horwitz, E., & Garza, T. (1999). Foreign language reading anxiety. Modern Language Journal, 83, 202 218. Service, E. (1992). Phonology, working memory, and foreign language learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45A, 21 50. Service, E., & Kohonen, V. (1995). Is the relation between phonological memory and foreign language learning accounted for by vocabulary acquisition? Applied Psycholinguistics, 16, 155 172. Sparks, R., & Ganschow, L. (1991). Foreign language learning differences: Affective or native language aptitude differences? Modern Language Journal, 75, 3 16. Sparks, R., & Ganschow, L. (1995). A strong inference approach to causal factors in foreign language learning: A response to MacIntyre. Modern Language Journal, 79, 235 244. Sparks, R., & Ganschow, L. (1996). Teachers perceptions of students foreign language skills and affective characteristics. Journal of Educational Research, 89, 172 185. Sparks, R., Ganschow, L., Artzer, M., Siebenhar, D., & Plageman, M. (1997). Anxiety and proficiency in a foreign language. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 85, 559 562. Sparks, R., Ganschow, L., Artzer, M., Siebenhar, D., & Plageman, M. (1998). Differences in native language skills, foreign language aptitude, and foreign language grades among high, average, and low proficiency foreign language learners: Two studies. Language Testing, 15, 181 216. Stanovich, K. (1988). The right and wrong places to look for the cognitive locus of reading disability. Annals of Dyslexia, 38, 154 177. Stanovich, K. (1998). Twenty five years of research on the reading process: The grand synthesis and what it means for our field. In T. Shanahan & F. Rodriguez-Brown (Eds.), Forty-seventh yearbook of the National Reading Conference (pp. 44 58). Chicago: National Reading Conference. Suk-Han-Ho, C., & Bryant, P. (1997). Phonological skills are important in learning to read Chinese. Developmental Psychology, 33, 946 951.

Readers Forum 255 APPENDIX FLRAS Items with Reading Components Item Decoding Comprehension 1. I get upset when I m not sure whether I understand X what I am reading in (French, Russian, Japanese). 2. When reading (French, Russian, Japanese), I often X understand the words but still can t quite understand what the author is saying. 3. When I m reading (French, Russian, Japanese), I get so X X confused I can t remember what I m reading. 4. I feel intimidated whenever I see a whole page of X X (French, Russian, Japanese) in front of me. 5. I am nervous when I am reading a passage in (French, X Russian, Japanese) when I am not familiar with the topic. 6. I get upset whenever I encounter unknown grammar X when reading (French, Russian, Japanese). 7. When reading (French, Russian, Japanese), I get nervous X and confused when I don t understand every word. 8. It bothers me to encounter words I can t pronounce X while reading (French, Russian, Japanese). 9. I usually end up translating word by word when X I m reading (French, Russian, Japanese). 10. By the time you get past the funny letters and symbols in X X (French, Russian, Japanese), it s hard to remember what you re reading about. 11. I am worried about all the new symbols you have to learn X in order to read (French, Russian, Japanese). 12. I enjoy reading (French, Russian, Japanese). X X 13. I feel confident when I am reading in (French, X X Russian, Japanese). 14. Once you get used to it, reading (French, Russian, X X Japanese) is not so difficult. 15. The hardest part of learning (French, Russian, X X Japanese) is learning to read. 16. I would be happy just to learn to speak (French, Russian, X X Japanese) rather than having to learn to read as well. 17. I don t mind reading to myself, but I feel very X uncomfortable when I have to read (French, Russian, Japanese) aloud. 18. I am satisfied with the level of reading ability in (French, X X Russian, Japanese) that I have achieved so far. 19. (French, Russian, Japanese) culture and ideas seem very X foreign to me. 20. You have to know so much about (French, Russian, X Japanese) history and culture in order to read (French, Russian, Japanese).