Working Memory, Long-Term Memory, and Language Processing: Issues and Future Directions

Similar documents
Short-term memory in Down syndrome: Applying the working memory model

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

Fribourg, Fribourg, Switzerland b LEAD CNRS UMR 5022, Université de Bourgogne, Dijon, France

Beeson, P. M. (1999). Treating acquired writing impairment. Aphasiology, 13,

Longitudinal family-risk studies of dyslexia: why. develop dyslexia and others don t.

Acta Psychologica 138 (2011) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Acta Psychologica. journal homepage:

2,1 .,,, , %, ,,,,,,. . %., Butterworth,)?.(1989; Levelt, 1989; Levelt et al., 1991; Levelt, Roelofs & Meyer, 1999

AGENDA LEARNING THEORIES LEARNING THEORIES. Advanced Learning Theories 2/22/2016

Concept Acquisition Without Representation William Dylan Sabo

Strategy Abandonment Effects in Cued Recall

A Comparison of the Effects of Two Practice Session Distribution Types on Acquisition and Retention of Discrete and Continuous Skills

Phonological encoding in speech production

A redintegration account of the effects of speech rate, lexicality, and word frequency in immediate serial recall

Curriculum Vitae. Sara C. Steele, Ph.D, CCC-SLP 253 McGannon Hall 3750 Lindell Blvd., St. Louis, MO Tel:

Intervening to alleviate word-finding difficulties in children: case series data and a computational modelling foundation

Aging and the Use of Context in Ambiguity Resolution: Complex Changes From Simple Slowing

Presentation Format Effects in a Levels-of-Processing Task

AP PSYCHOLOGY VACATION WORK PACKET UNIT 7A: MEMORY

Learning Disability Functional Capacity Evaluation. Dear Doctor,

Tip-of-the-tongue states as metacognition

Encoding. Retrieval. Forgetting. Physiology of Memory. Systems and Types of Memory

Maximizing Learning Through Course Alignment and Experience with Different Types of Knowledge

Testing protects against proactive interference in face name learning

Exploring Dyslexics Phonological Deficit I: Lexical vs Sub-lexical and Input vs Output Processes

English Language and Applied Linguistics. Module Descriptions 2017/18

Linguistics. Undergraduate. Departmental Honors. Graduate. Faculty. Linguistics 1

Comparison Between Three Memory Tests: Cued Recall, Priming and Saving Closed-Head Injured Patients and Controls

30 Days. Memory. to a More POWERFUL. Gini Graham Scott, Ph.D. American Management Association

Clinical Review Criteria Related to Speech Therapy 1

Effects of Vocabulary and Phonotactic Probability on 2-Year-Olds Nonword Repetition

Levels of processing: Qualitative differences or task-demand differences?

Inclusion in Music Education

Ling/Span/Fren/Ger/Educ 466: SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION. Spring 2011 (Tuesdays 4-6:30; Psychology 251)

Learning and Retaining New Vocabularies: The Case of Monolingual and Bilingual Dictionaries

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Dyslexia/dyslexic, 3, 9, 24, 97, 187, 189, 206, 217, , , 367, , , 397,

STAFF DEVELOPMENT in SPECIAL EDUCATION

Special Education Services Program/Service Descriptions

V Congress of Russian Psychological Society. Alexander I. Statnikov*, Tatiana V. Akhutina

Unraveling symbolic number processing and the implications for its association with mathematics. Delphine Sasanguie

Characteristics of the Text Genre Informational Text Text Structure

Conceptual and Procedural Knowledge of a Mathematics Problem: Their Measurement and Their Causal Interrelations

raıs Factors affecting word learning in adults: A comparison of L2 versus L1 acquisition /r/ /aı/ /s/ /r/ /aı/ /s/ = individual sound

L.E.A.P. Learning Enrichment & Achievement Program

Investigating the Effectiveness of the Uses of Electronic and Paper-Based Dictionaries in Promoting Incidental Word Learning

Understanding the Relationship between Comprehension and Production

Stages of Literacy Ros Lugg

Reading Horizons. Organizing Reading Material into Thought Units to Enhance Comprehension. Kathleen C. Stevens APRIL 1983

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

The Effects of Strategic Planning and Topic Familiarity on Iranian Intermediate EFL Learners Written Performance in TBLT

GEOMETRY, WORKING MEMORY AND INTELLIGENCE

Afsaneh Rahimi Tehrani University of Isfahan, Isfahan, Iran. Hossein Barati English Department, University of Isfahan, Isfahan, Iran

Describing Motion Events in Adult L2 Spanish Narratives

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 141 ( 2014 ) WCLTA Using Corpus Linguistics in the Development of Writing

Abstractions and the Brain

CEFR Overall Illustrative English Proficiency Scales

STUDENT NUMBER Letter Figures Words PSYCHOLOGY. Written examination 2. Thursday 3 November 2005

D Road Maps 6. A Guide to Learning System Dynamics. System Dynamics in Education Project

Sentence comprehension is a necessary skill for successful

BIOH : Principles of Medical Physiology

ASSISTIVE COMMUNICATION

The phonological grammar is probabilistic: New evidence pitting abstract representation against analogy

Organizing Comprehensive Literacy Assessment: How to Get Started

Analyzing Linguistically Appropriate IEP Goals in Dual Language Programs

Psychology of Speech Production and Speech Perception

Characteristics of the Text Genre Informational Text Text Structure

SCHEMA ACTIVATION IN MEMORY FOR PROSE 1. Michael A. R. Townsend State University of New York at Albany

Characteristics of the Text Genre Realistic fi ction Text Structure

Second Language Acquisition in Adults: From Research to Practice


SOME MINIMAL NOTES ON MINIMALISM *

The Journey to Vowelerria VOWEL ERRORS: THE LOST WORLD OF SPEECH INTERVENTION. Preparation: Education. Preparation: Education. Preparation: Education

Recommended Guidelines for the Diagnosis of Children with Learning Disabilities

The Complete Brain Exercise Book: Train Your Brain - Improve Memory, Language, Motor Skills And More By Fraser Smith

Source-monitoring judgments about anagrams and their solutions: Evidence for the role of cognitive operations information in memory

ANNUAL REPORT SCHOOL OF COMMUNICATION SCIENCES & DISORDERS FACULTY OF MEDICINE

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 154 ( 2014 )

CALIFORNIA STATE UNIVERSITY, SAN MARCOS SCHOOL OF EDUCATION

Think A F R I C A when assessing speaking. C.E.F.R. Oral Assessment Criteria. Think A F R I C A - 1 -

Retrieval in cued recall

An Asset-Based Approach to Linguistic Diversity

An extended dual search space model of scientific discovery learning

On Human Computer Interaction, HCI. Dr. Saif al Zahir Electrical and Computer Engineering Department UBC

Title: Language Impairment in Bilingual children: State of the art 2017

SPEECH LANGAUGE PATHOLOGHY HANDBOOK

An argument from speech pathology

Effect of time of day on language in healthy ageing and Alzheimer s disease

A Game-based Assessment of Children s Choices to Seek Feedback and to Revise

Translational Display of. in Communication Sciences and Disorders

Accelerated Learning Course Outline

Development of the Grammar and Phonology Screening (GAPS) test to assess key markers of specific language and literacy difficulties in young children

The present study investigated whether subjects were sensitive to negative

Which verb classes and why? Research questions: Semantic Basis Hypothesis (SBH) What verb classes? Why the truth of the SBH matters

The Perception of Nasalized Vowels in American English: An Investigation of On-line Use of Vowel Nasalization in Lexical Access

Reading Horizons. A Look At Linguistic Readers. Nicholas P. Criscuolo APRIL Volume 10, Issue Article 5

Adults with traumatic brain injury (TBI) often have word retrieval problems (Barrow, et al., 2003; 2006; King, et al., 2006a; 2006b; Levin et al.

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 146 ( 2014 )

ERP measures of auditory word repetition and translation priming in bilinguals

Teacher: Mlle PERCHE Maeva High School: Lycée Charles Poncet, Cluses (74) Level: Seconde i.e year old students

LEXICAL COHESION ANALYSIS OF THE ARTICLE WHAT IS A GOOD RESEARCH PROJECT? BY BRIAN PALTRIDGE A JOURNAL ARTICLE

Transcription:

Brain and Language 71, 46 51 (2000) doi:10.1006/brln.1999.2209, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Working Memory, Long-Term Memory, and Language Processing: Issues and Future Directions Fabienne Collette, Martial Van der Linden, and Martine Poncelet Neuropsychology Unit, University of Liège, Liège, Belgium Working memory refers to a limited capacity system responsible for the temporary storage and processing of information while cognitive tasks are performed. The multi component model proposed by Baddeley and Hitch (Baddeley & Hitch, 1974; Baddeley, 1986) represents the most extensively investigated and the best articulated theoretical account of working memory. It consists of a modality-free controlling central executive which is aided by two slave systems ensuring temporary maintenance of verbal and visuospatial information: the phonological loop (composed of a phonological store and an articulatory rehearsal system) and the visuospatial sketchpad. This model has unquestionably contributed to a better understanding of the part played by working memory in various domains of cognition (for a review, see Logie, 1993). In particular, numerous findings suggest that working memory (especially the phonological loop and the central executive components) makes significant contributions to some aspects of language processing, namely, sentence comprehension, speech production, vocabulary acquisition, and reading (see Baddeley, Gathercole, & Papagno, 1998; Van der Linden, & Poncelet, 1998; Van der Linden, Hupet, Feyereisen, et al., 1999). However, some aspects of Baddeley s working memory model have recently been questioned, especially the relationships between working memory and long-term memory (see Van der Linden, 1998). According to Baddeley (1996), working memory is viewed as a gateway between sensory input and long-term memory. In particular, working memory is considered to be closely involved in the learning of novel information. In this perspective, a F. Collette is Chargé de Recherche at the Belgian Fund for Scientific Research (FNRS). Address correspondence and reprint requests to F. Collette, Service de Neuropsychologie, Boulevard du Rectorat 3 (B33), 4000 Liège, Belgium. E-mail: f.collette@ulg.ac.be. 0093-934X/00 $35.00 Copyright 2000 by Academic Press All rights of reproduction in any form reserved. 46

MILLENNIUM ISSUE 47 vast amount of data have suggested that the long-term acquisition of phonological forms of new words requires the integrity of the phonological store (e.g., Barisnikov, Van der Linden, & Poncelet, 1996). Several studies have led to question this gateway view, especially by demonstrating the existence of long-term memory effects in working memory (span) tasks. For example, there exist some results suggesting that memory span performance is better for words than for nonwords (Hulme et al., 1995), for high-frequency and high-imageability words than for low-frequency and low-imageability items (Watkins & Watkins, 1977; Bourassa & Besner, 1994), and also for words belonging to the same semantic category than for words from different semantic categories (Poirier & Saint-Aubin, 1995). These data indicate that lexical or semantic information (or both) may contribute to memory span. In the same vein, several studies have shown that nonword repetition was better for the more wordlike nonwords (Gathercole, 1995). In order to accommodate these empirical findings (in particular the contribution of phonological long-term memory to span performance) and the role of the phonological storage component of working memory in the long-term acquisition of new phonological information, Baddeley et al. (1998) postulates the existence of two separate but interrelated (short- and long-term) phonological stores. In this view, the visually or auditorily presented verbal information is maintained in a phonological short-term store. The storage of information in this component depends upon the temporary activation of units in a phonological network. Otherwise, the long-term phonological representations constitute a stable pattern corresponding to phonological structures which are frequently activated. Since the activation in the short-term store spreads into the long-term store, the repeated presentations of a phonological information will modify the representations in the long-term phonological system (which explains the learning of novel phonological forms). Moreover, the long-term phonological information also influences the functioning of the short-term store, by reconstructing partially decayed representations in this store. Logie (1996) proposed a rather different interpretation of the long-term memory contribution to working memory performance. He suggests that working memory operates not as a gateway between sensory input and longterm memory but as a workspace. In this view, the storage components of working memory (the phonological loop and the visuospatial sketchpad) are not input buffers but rather they serve as temporary buffers for the information that has yet to be processed or is about to be rehearsed overtly. Thus, information that has been recently presented to the senses will activate the whole corresponding traces in long-term memory (visual, phonological, semantic, etc.), which then become available for temporary activation in the different components of working memory. This model furnished an explanation of the intervention of long-term memory in span tasks by suggesting

48 MILLENNIUM ISSUE that the performance depending on the phonological loop will be increased if semantic and visual information are simultaneously available for the other components of working memory. Logie s proposition constitutes an intermediate conception between Baddeley s multiple component working memory model and the view which considers that working memory is nothing else than a temporary activation of representations and procedures in long-term memory (e.g., La Pointe & Engle, 1990). This interpretation is also quite consistent with the view that the phonological store plays a role in the long-term acquisition of new phonological information. Indeed, it could be suggested, as Gathercole and Martin (1996) did, that the incoming to-be-acquired new words activate phonological elements in a phonological network and these activated phonological elements are available for temporary retention in the phonological loop. Moreover, the more discriminating and durable is the temporary trace in the phonological loop, the more readily a stable long-term phonological representation can be constructed in the lexical memory system. From this point of view, working memory still plays a role in long-term memory by processing the information it receives and returning it to long-term memory. However, it should be noted that neither Logie s nor Baddeley s view provides a precise description of the mechanisms by which the semantic information contributes to working memory performance. Contrary to Baddeley s view (as well as Logie s adaptation), Martin and Romani (1994) suggested that verbal working memory is not a specialized subsystem dedicated to short-term memory storage and separate from the language system, but rather, it draws on the operation and storage capacities of a subset of components involved in language processing. Martin and Romani (1994) explored the short-term abilities and sentence comprehension of two brain-damaged patients (AB, EA). Although they showed similar span reduction, one patient (AB) demonstrated a better performance on span tasks for phonological rather than for semantic information, while the other patient (EA) showed the reverse pattern. Moreover, the performance of both patients on the sentence processing tasks was consistent with these contrasting working memory deficits: the patient with a phonological retention deficit was more impaired on a sentence repetition task whereas the patient with the semantic retention deficit was more impaired on a sentence comprehension task. Another patient (MW) was also described: he showed normal span performance but had a deficit affecting specifically the short-term retention of syntactic structures. From these results, the authors concluded that the different levels of representation involved in memory span and language processing draw on specific resources, which may be conceptualized either as buffers specialized for particular types of representations, or in terms of rate of decay that may differ for different levels of representation (see also, Martin & Saffran, 1997). In a similar vein, Waters and Caplan (1996) proposed to distinguish gen-

MILLENNIUM ISSUE 49 eral-purpose working memory resources from specific working memory resources specifically dedicated to on-line syntactic processing in sentence comprehension. In that perspective, the performance on general verbal working-memory tasks will not predict language-processing efficiency, which is consistent with the existence of patients with a limited verbal span and who do not demonstrate difficulties in processing a wide range of syntactic structures, and also of patients with normal span performance who showed difficulties in sentence comprehension (the patient MW; Martin & Romani, 1994). These views clearly differ from Baddeley s conception in that they consider that verbal short-term memory is an integral part of the language system, whereas for Baddeley, the working memory components are not strictly tied to any particular cognitive system. Within the framework of Baddeley s model, the temporary storage of semantic (or even syntactic) information might plausibly be viewed as residing in the central executive system or as resulting from a temporary activation of long-term memory information (a long-term working memory; Ericsson & Kintsch, 1995). In conclusion, there exist different contrasted conceptions of the relationships between working memory and long-term memory, as well as between working memory and language processing. One of the objectives of cognitive neuropsychology in the next Millennium will be to decide between these different views. In this perspective, along with the exploration of brain-damaged patients, functional imagery studies should contribute to achieve this objective. Indeed, specific predictions about patterns of cerebral activation can be made in the debate concerning the existence of separate short- and long-term phonological stores (Baddeley et al., 1998) or the existence of one only store responsible for short- and long-term phonological processes (Gathercole & Martin, 1996). So, the existence of only one phonological store for short- and long-term representations would lead to a similar pattern of activation when subjects have to perform span tasks with items having, or not, long-term phonological representations (for example, words and nonwords). On the contrary, the existence of two separate phonological stores would be expressed by supplementary activation when words (in comparison to nonwords) have to be maintained in working memory. In a recent PET study (Collette et al., 1999), we compared the cerebral areas activated in span tasks for words and nonwords. Results showed increased activity in the left middle temporal gyrus and temporo-parietal region in the word span task only. These areas were previously related respectively to a phonological word lexicon (Howard et al., 1992) and to lexico-semantic processes (Price et al., 1997). These data appear to be in agreement with the existence of two separate phonological stores, as postulated by Baddeley et al. (1998). More generally, they confirm that the use of functional imagery should help to confront theoretical conceptions about the relationships between workingmemory, long-term memory, and language.

50 MILLENNIUM ISSUE REFERENCES Baddeley, A. D. 1986. Working memory. Oxford: Clarendon Press. Baddeley, A. D. 1996. The fractionation of working memory. Proceedings of the National Academic of Sciences, USA, 93, 13468 13472. Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. 1974. Working memory. In G. Bower (Ed.), Recent advances in learning and motivation, Vol. 8. (pp. 47 90). New York: Academic Press. Baddeley, A. D., Gathercole, S., & Papagno, C. 1998. The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105, 158 173. Barisnikov, K., Van der Linden, M., & Poncelet, M. 1996. Acquisition of new words and phonological working memory in Williams syndrome: A case study. Neurocase, 2, 395 404. Bourassa, D. C., & Besner, D. 1994. Beyond the articulatory loop: A semantic contribution to serial order recall of subspan lists. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 1, 122 125. Collette, F., Van der Linden, M., Salmon, E., Dabe, P., Delfiore, G., Degueldre, C., & Luxen, A. 1999. Contribution of long-term memory to working memory tasks. A PET activation study. Neuroimage, 9, S943. Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. 1995. Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 102, 211 245. Gathercole, S. E. 1995. Is nonword repetition a test of phonological memory or long-term knowledge? It all depends on the nonwords. Memory and Cognition, 23, 83 94. Gathercole, S. E., & Martin, A. J. 1996. Interactive processes in phonological memory. In S. E. Gathercole (Ed.), Models of short-term memory (pp. 73 100). Hove: Psychology Press. Howard, D., Patterson, K., Wise, R., Brown, W. D., Friston, K. J., Weiller, C., & Frackowiak, R. S. J. 1992. The cortical localization of the lexicons. Brain, 115, 1769 1782. Hulme, C., Roodenrys, S., & Mercer, R. 1995. The role of long-term memory mechanisms in memory span. British Journal of Psychology, 86, 527 536. La Pointe, L. B., & Engle, R. W. 1990. Simple and complex word spans as measures of working memory capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 16, 1118 1133. Logie, R. H. 1993. Working memory in everyday cognition. In G. M. Davies & R. H. Logie (Eds.), Memory in everyday life (pp. 173 218). Amsterdam: Elsevier Publishers B. V. Logie, R. H. 1996. The seven ages of working memory. In M. Richards, R. W. Engle, L. Hasher, R. H. Logie, E. R. Stoltzfus, & R. T. Zacks (Eds.), Working memory and human cognition (pp. 31 65). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Martin, N., & Saffran, E. M. 1997. Language and auditory verbal short-term memory impairments: Evidence for common underlying processes. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14, 641 682. Martin, R. C., & Romani, C. 1994. Verbal working memory and sentence comprehension: A multiple-components view. Neuropsychology, 8, 506 523. Poirier, M., & Saint-Aubin, J. 1995. Memory for related and unrelated words: Further evidence on the influence of semantic factors in immediate serial recall. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48A, 384 404. Price, C. J., Moore, C. J., Humphreys, G. W., & Wise, R. J. S. 1997. Segregating semantic from phonological processes during reading. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 727 733. Van der Linden, M. 1998. The relationships between working memory and long-term memory. Académie des Sciences, Paris, Sciences de la vie, 321, 175 177.

MILLENNIUM ISSUE 51 Van der Linden, M., & Poncelet, M. 1998. The role of working memory in language and communication disorders. In B. Stemmer & H. A. Whitaker (Eds.), Handbook of neurolinguistics (pp. 289 300). New York: Academic Press. Van der Linden, M., Hupet, M., El Ahmadi, A., Feyereisen, P., Schelstraete, M. A., Bestgen, Y., Bruyer, R., Lories, G., & Seron, X. 1999. Cognitive mediators of age-related differences in langage comprehension and verbal memory performance. Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 6, 32 55. Waters, G. S., & Caplan, D. 1996. The capacity theory of sentence comprehension: Critique of Just and Carpenter (1992). Psychological Review, 103, 761 772. Watkins, O. C., & Watkins, M. J. 1977. Serial recall and the modality effect: Effects of word frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 3,712 718.