The Syllable in RCVP: Structure and Licensing. CUNY Phonology Forum / Conference on the Syllable University of Connecticut January 17/19, 2008

Similar documents
Radical CV Phonology: the locational gesture *

The analysis starts with the phonetic vowel and consonant charts based on the dataset:

Consonants: articulation and transcription

Consonant-Vowel Unity in Element Theory*

Pobrane z czasopisma New Horizons in English Studies Data: 18/11/ :52:20. New Horizons in English Studies 1/2016

Phonological Processing for Urdu Text to Speech System

ADDIS ABABA UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF GRADUATE STUDIES MODELING IMPROVED AMHARIC SYLLBIFICATION ALGORITHM

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

SOUND STRUCTURE REPRESENTATION, REPAIR AND WELL-FORMEDNESS: GRAMMAR IN SPOKEN LANGUAGE PRODUCTION. Adam B. Buchwald

Speech Recognition using Acoustic Landmarks and Binary Phonetic Feature Classifiers

Manner assimilation in Uyghur

Phonetics. The Sound of Language

DEVELOPMENT OF LINGUAL MOTOR CONTROL IN CHILDREN AND ADOLESCENTS

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

To appear in the Proceedings of the 35th Meetings of the Chicago Linguistics Society. Post-vocalic spirantization: Typology and phonetic motivations

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

Phonological encoding in speech production

Sounds of Infant-Directed Vocabulary: Learned from Infants Speech or Part of Linguistic Knowledge?

Speech Segmentation Using Probabilistic Phonetic Feature Hierarchy and Support Vector Machines

1. REFLEXES: Ask questions about coughing, swallowing, of water as fast as possible (note! Not suitable for all

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. VCV-sequencies in a preliminary text-to-speech system for female speech

A Fact in Historical Phonology from the Viewpoint of Generative Phonology: The Underlying Schwa in Old English

A Cross-language Corpus for Studying the Phonetics and Phonology of Prominence

Similarity Avoidance in the Proto-Indo-European Root

Parallel Evaluation in Stratal OT * Adam Baker University of Arizona

Stages of Literacy Ros Lugg

Rhythm-typology revisited.

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Voiced-voiceless distinction in alaryngeal speech - acoustic and articula

Universal contrastive analysis as a learning principle in CAPT

Word Stress and Intonation: Introduction

SEGMENTAL FEATURES IN SPONTANEOUS AND READ-ALOUD FINNISH

On the nature of voicing assimilation(s)

Lexical phonology. Marc van Oostendorp. December 6, Until now, we have presented phonological theory as if it is a monolithic

The Perception of Nasalized Vowels in American English: An Investigation of On-line Use of Vowel Nasalization in Lexical Access

On the Formation of Phoneme Categories in DNN Acoustic Models

Partial Class Behavior and Nasal Place Assimilation*

An argument from speech pathology

**Note: this is slightly different from the original (mainly in format). I would be happy to send you a hard copy.**

Language Acquisition by Identical vs. Fraternal SLI Twins * Karin Stromswold & Jay I. Rifkin

age, Speech and Hearii

Listener-oriented phonology

source or where they are needed to distinguish two forms of a language. 4. Geographical Location. I have attempted to provide a geographical

Infants learn phonotactic regularities from brief auditory experience

A simpler view of Danish stød

THE PHONOLOGICAL WORD IN STANDARD MALA Y

Markedness and Complex Stops: Evidence from Simplification Processes 1. Nick Danis Rutgers University

have to be modeled) or isolated words. Output of the system is a grapheme-tophoneme conversion system which takes as its input the spelling of words,

Joan Bybee, Phonology and Language Use. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2001,

Unvoiced Landmark Detection for Segment-based Mandarin Continuous Speech Recognition

Florida Reading Endorsement Alignment Matrix Competency 1

NCU IISR English-Korean and English-Chinese Named Entity Transliteration Using Different Grapheme Segmentation Approaches

The Indian English of Tibeto-Burman language speakers*

Linguistics 220 Phonology: distributions and the concept of the phoneme. John Alderete, Simon Fraser University

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

Dyslexia/dyslexic, 3, 9, 24, 97, 187, 189, 206, 217, , , 367, , , 397,

5. Margi (Chadic, Nigeria): H, L, R (Williams 1973, Hoffmann 1963)

2,1 .,,, , %, ,,,,,,. . %., Butterworth,)?.(1989; Levelt, 1989; Levelt et al., 1991; Levelt, Roelofs & Meyer, 1999

A Level Playing-Field: Perceptibility and Inflection in English Compounds. Robert Kirchner and Elena Nicoladis (U. Alberta)

A Neural Network GUI Tested on Text-To-Phoneme Mapping

Books Effective Literacy Y5-8 Learning Through Talk Y4-8 Switch onto Spelling Spelling Under Scrutiny

Phonology Revisited: Sor3ng Out the PH Factors in Reading and Spelling Development. Indiana, November, 2015

Module 12. Machine Learning. Version 2 CSE IIT, Kharagpur

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

Underlying Representations

Bare Root Nodes in Basaa

Phonological Encoding in Sentence Production

Proof Theory for Syntacticians

Christine Mooshammer, IPDS Kiel, Philip Hoole, IPSK München, Anja Geumann, Dublin

Contrastiveness and diachronic variation in Chinese nasal codas. Tsz-Him Tsui The Ohio State University

Journal of Phonetics

Revisiting the role of prosody in early language acquisition. Megha Sundara UCLA Phonetics Lab

Different Task Type and the Perception of the English Interdental Fricatives

The Prosodic (Re)organization of Determiners

Precedence Constraints and Opacity

Speech Recognition at ICSI: Broadcast News and beyond

Rachel E. Baker, Ann R. Bradlow. Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA

SARDNET: A Self-Organizing Feature Map for Sequences

Clinical Application of the Mean Babbling Level and Syllable Structure Level

The Future of Consortia among Indian Libraries - FORSA Consortium as Forerunner?

Phonetic Knowledge in Tonal Adaptation: Mandarin and English. Loanwords in Lhasa Tibetan*

Truncation to Subminimal Words

Perceived speech rate: the effects of. articulation rate and speaking style in spontaneous speech. Jacques Koreman. Saarland University

Acoustic correlates of stress and their use in diagnosing syllable fusion in Tongan. James White & Marc Garellek UCLA

Derivations (MP) and Evaluations (OT) *

DOWNSTEP IN SUPYIRE* Robert Carlson Societe Internationale de Linguistique, Mali

ABSTRACT. Some children with speech sound disorders (SSD) have difficulty with literacyrelated

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Sound symbolism in deictic words

Salience in Sociolinguistics

Journal of Phonetics

Stochastic Phonology Janet B. Pierrehumbert Department of Linguistics Northwestern University Evanston, IL Introduction

A Bayesian Model of Stress Assignment in Reading

UKLO Round Advanced solutions and marking schemes. 6 The long and short of English verbs [15 marks]

REPRESENTATIONAL HANDLING OF POZNAŃ-CRACOW VOICING IN GOVERNMENT PHONOLOGY

MARK 12 Reading II (Adaptive Remediation)

A survey of intonation systems

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Segregation of Unvoiced Speech from Nonspeech Interference

Classification. Universals

Get Your Hands On These Multisensory Reading Strategies

STUDENT LEARNING ASSESSMENT REPORT

Transcription:

: Structure and Licensing Harry van der Hulst CUNY Phonology Forum / Conference on the Syllable University of Connecticut January 17/19, 2008 (1) Syllable structure in RCVP The syllable ( built-in major class distinctions ) (van der Hulst 2005) a. Universal syllable: σ C CONSONANT ( onset ) V SONORANT ( rhyme) { } { } { } = set of permissible segments in relevant position (cf. below) b. σ C CONSONANT ( onset ) V SONORANT ( rhyme) c. Parameter: null option permitted (Y/N) σ C CONSONANT ( onset ) V SONORANT ( rhyme) C OBSTRUENT V SONORANT V VOWEL C CONSONANT Parameter: double filling ( branching ) permitted (Y/N)

(2) Segmental Content I - Each syllabic position allows a set of segments. - A segment is a combination of PHONATION, MANNER and LOCATION (called gestures aka class nodes ). Only manner (the head gesture) is obligatory. - Each gesture allows a choice from two elements: a. syllabic (skeletal) position Phonation gestures (cf. class nodes) {H,L} Manner {C,V} Location {I,U} elements (cf. features) - Each element has multiple (related) phonetic interpretations (see below). - Permissible combinations display the following maximal structure: b. gesture adjunct (specifier, sister-dependent) s-dependent head d-dependent basic structures (head + complement ) C / V C / V C / V complement (daughter-dependent) - The occurrence of s- and d-dependent tends to be mutually exclusive. c. Possible gesture structures per gesture Basic structures: C C V V C V D-dependent (post-subscript) Adjunction structures: S-dependent (pre-superscript) C X V X - As shown, an adjunct element tends to occur only with a simple basic structure. d. Enhancement: Bare C and V are interpreted as if they have a C and V dependent: C = C C and V = V V 2

e. Manner in onset head position (obstruents) manner gesture s-dependent head d-dependent C NASAL / V PHAR C STOP / V FRIC C MELLOW / V STRIDENT (extra cavities) (basic stricture) (modification of basic stricture) f. C a plain stop (i.e. C C : NON-stridency enhances stops) C V a strident stop (i.e. affricate) V C a non-strident fricative V a strident fricative (i.e. V V : stridency enhances fricativeness) C X V X (pre) nasalized obstruent (or lateralized obstruents) phargyngealized obstruent g. Manner in onset dependent position (sonorant consonants) manner gesture head d-dependent C CONTACT / V APPROXIMATION C BLOCK / V FLOW (basic stricture) (modification of basic stricture) h. C a nasal C V a lateral V C a rhotic V an approximant - In onset dependent position only the basic structures occur. - It is to be expected that head positions allow greater complexity than dependent positions (the fundamental head dependent asymmetry). - Note how occurrences of C and V correspond to traditional features labels in most, but not all cases. 3

i. Manner in rhyme head position (vowels) manner gesture s-dependent head d-dependent C NASAL / V ATR C HIGH / V LOW C HIGH / V LOW (extra cavities) (basic stricture) (modification of basic stricture) j. C a high vowel C V a high-mid vowel V C a low-mid vowel V a low vowel C X V X nasalized vowel advanced vowel (ATR) k. Manner in rhyme dependent position (consonantal sonorants) As (2g) - The same elements and elemental structures are used for consonants and vowels or more specifically for onset heads, rhyme heads and dependent syllabic positions. - Hence, we have a unified set of manner structure. - There also are unified structures for Phonation and Location distinctions. (3) Segment content II a. Three gestures Syllabic position Phonation (incl. Tone) Articulation {h,l} {H, Hl,Lh,L} adjunct basic structures Manner Location {c,v} {C,cv,Vc,V} {i,u} {I, Iu,Ui,U} 4

b. Phonation and Location only apply to syllabic heads which we can (but do not have to) graphically represents by linking these gestures to syllabic head nodes: Phon Phon Loc Loc V (syllable) C (onset) V (rhyme) (skeleton ) C V V C man man man man - The fact that only syllabic heads have phonation and location is another (expected) head dependent asymmetry. c. The phonetic interpretation of elements is determined by: i. Syllabic position (four options) onset head onset dependent rhyme head rhyme dependent ii. Subgestural role (three options) primary subgesture (head) secondary subgesture (d-dependent) adjunct (s-dependent) - Note: the interpretation for both onset and rhyme dependents are the same, except that the rhyme dependent interpretations are phonetically weaker. - For example: all sonorant consonants are stronger (i.e. articulated with more stricture) in the onset (a C-constituent) than in the rhymes (a V-constituent). - This is why I call a sonorant in onset dependent position a sonorant consonant and a rhyme dependent (coda) sonorant a consonantal sonorant. 5

d. The phonetic interpretation of the elements MANNER Syllabic position C (onset head) OBSTRUENT Cv (onset dependent) SONORANT V (rhyme head) VOWEL Vc (rhyme dependent) CONSONANT Adjunct V V C C V C strident fricative plain fricative affricate (strident stop) plain stop approximant rhotic lateral nasal low vowel low-mid vowel high-mid vowel approximant rhotic lateral nasal onset: pharyngealized rhyme: ATR high vowel onset: nasal rhyme: nasal PHONATION Syllabic L (V) Lh (V C ) Hl (C V ) H (C) position C (onset head) voiced Creaky Breathy voiceless V (rhyme head) low tone low mid tone high mid tone high tone Adjunct aspirated glottalized LOCATION Syllabic position C (onset head) U (V) Ui (V C ) Iu (C V ) I (C) peripheral & labial, e.g. [p] peripheral & coronal, e.g. [k] coronal & peripheral, e.g. [c] coronal, e.g. [t] V (rhyme head) peripheral & round, e.g. [u] peripheral & front, e.g. [ ] front & peripheral e.g. [ü] front, e.g. [i] Adjunct labialized palatalized 6

e. RAA ( There are only two elements ) i. Syllabic position Phonation (incl. Tone) Articulation {c,v} {C, Cv,Vc,V} adjunct basic structures Manner Location {c,v} {C,cv,Vc,V} {C,V} {C, Cv,Vc,V} ii. Cross-gestural equivalences Manner Location Phonation C C I L V V U H f. The phonetic interpretation of elements is determined by ( revised ): i. Syllabic position (four options) onset head onset dependent rhyme head rhyme dependent ii. Subgestural role (three options) primary division (head) secondary division (d-dependent) adjunct division (s-dependent) iii. Gesture (three options): manner location phonation 7

g. Illustration: Interpretations of C ( Traditional binary features are articulatory short hands for phonetic interpretation ) a. Syllabic position: onset head b. Role: head [ continuant] c. Gesture: head (manner) a. Syllabic position: rhyme head b. Role: head [+high] c. Gesture: head (manner) a. Syllabic position: onset head b. Role: head [ voice] c. Gesture: head (phonation) a. Syllabic position: rhyme head b. Role: head [+high (tone)] c. Gesture: head (phonation) a. Syllabic position: onset head b. Role: head [+coronal] c. Gesture: head (location) a. Syllabic position: rhyme head b. Role: head [-back] c. Gesture: head (location) - In addition C has an interpretation as adjunct which is similar/the same for onset heads and rhyme heads: Phonation: glottalized Manner: nasalized Location: palatalized 8

(4) The Syllable Segment Connection: Preferences a. The structure of non-branching onsets: C CONSONANT ( onset ) C CONSONANT ( onset ) C OBSTRUENT V SONORANT BIAS: C > V (in onset head position obstruents are preferred over sonorants). b. Within each if these two sets, simple (enhanced) structure are preferred over complex structures (ENHANCEMENT >> BIAS). Thus for head positions BIAS does not reach into the manner gesture. ENHANCEMENT creates perceptual distance, a force which we expect to affect head positions because head positions carry the contrastive load: C V C V V C Stop strident fricative affricate mellow fricative C V C V V C Nasal approximant lateral rhotic (When two paths are available both are followed, rather then one first and then the other. Hence stops and nasals will precede fricatives and approximants.) c. Branching onsets C CONSONANT ( onset ) C OBSTRUENT V SONORANT C a plain stop V approximant V a strident fricative V C rhotic C V a strident stop (i.e. affricate) C V lateral V C a non-strident fricative C nasal C X V X (pre) nasalized obstruent phargyngealized obstruent 9

- In complex onsets, the head position tends to be a simple structure (C or V), but note that affricates can have onset dependents (German). - As for the dependent position ENHANCEMENT plays no role here (or BIAS >> ENHANCEMENT) and we would expect the order of preference to be dictated by BIAS alone: V > V C > C V > C. Nasals indeed seem the least preferred, but are approximants preferred over liquids? Perhaps a syntagmatic force (the upcoming rhyme) militates against the approximant. Liquids, then, offer the perfect compromise. d. The structure of non-branching rhymes: V SONORANT ( onset ) V SONORANT ( onset ) V VOWEL C CONSONANT syllabic consonants BIAS: V > C (vowels are preferred over consonants) b. Again, within each if these two sets simple (enhanced) structure are preferred over complex structures (ENHANCEMENT >> BIAS). V C V C C V low vowel high vowel low-mid high-mid V C V C C V approximant nasal rhotic lateral c. Branching rhymes: V ( rhyme ) V VOWEL C CONSONANT V a low vowel C nasal C a high vowel C V lateral V C a low-mid vowel V C rhotic C V a high-mid vowel V approximant C X V X nasalized vowel advanced vowel 10

- In complex onset the head position tends to be a simple structure (C or V), but mid vowels can occur in closed syllables. - But adjunct structures (nasalized and advanced vowels) are less likely in closed syllables. - As for the dependent coda position ENHANCEMENT (or BIAS >> ENHANCEMENT) plays no role here and we would expect the order of preference to be dictated by BIAS alone: C > C V > V C > V. Nasals indeed seem the most preferred. - The approximant coda option is a long vowel. - The intrasyllabic syntagmatic force causes onsets following a coda to be obstruents, rather than also sonorants ( Syllable Contact Law ). - Note that codas can only be sonorants, not obstruents. Alleged coda obstruents must be onsets. - Geminates: an empty coda position? Can dependent positions be empty? (5) Phonological Licensing in RcvP a. Central Thesis of Government Phonology (van der Hulst 2006) Marked syllabic structures require phonological licensing ( A marked syllabic constituent [empty of branching] must be followed, and as such licensed, by an unmarked syllabic constituent ) b. Two types of licensing a. Homogeneous: between Rs or between Os b. Heterogeneous: between O and R or between R and O c. Emptiness (X = empty R or empty O; X = contentful R or O) Empty rhyme (i) Homogeneous (R R ): an empty rhyme must be followed by a contentful rhyme (= Proper Government) (ii) Heterogeneous (R O ): an empty rhyme must be followed by a contentful onset (includes Resolution : C + V = CV) Empty onset (iii) Homogeneous (O O ): an empty onset most be followed by a contentful onset, i.e. no double hiatus (is questionable at best and perhaps only needed to exclude double occurrence of ghost consonant onsets) (iv) Heterogeneous (O R ): an empty onset must followed by a contentful rhyme (i.e. a syllable must have content) 11

d. Branchingness Branching rhyme (i) Homogeneous (R^R ): a branching rhyme must be followed by a contentful rhyme: * VC. Cv (parvenir) (ii) Heterogeneous (R^O ): a branching rhyme must be followed by a contentful rhyme: * VC. {V, #} ( coda licensing ) Branching onset (iii) Homogeneous (O^O ): a branching onset must be followed by a contentful onset: * CC V. cv (not empirically supported) (iv) Heterogeneous (O^R ): a branching onset must be followed by a contentful rhyme: * CC v (librement) e. Licensing between Os is not supported. Explanation: there is no onset projection level. f. A marked rhyme (empty or branching) most be followed by an unmarked rhyme and an unmarked (or at least non-empty) onset: A: R M O R g. A marked onset (empty or branching) must be followed by an unmarked rhyme: B: O M R h. A and B are the basic laws of phonotactic organization, in addition to the law that phonological expressions are constituted by an alternation of onsets and rhyme which are maximally binary branching. (6) Conclusions This enterprise ( RCVP ) maintains that it is worthwhile to express phonotactic regularities, i.e. regularities in phonological representations that are stored in memory. Furthermore, it explores the hypothesis that phonotactic wellformedness is based on limited (binary, headed) structural relations between two elements, C and V. The elements are not devoid of phonetic content in the sense that each structural occurrence of each element corresponds to certain phonetic properties which exist at the phonetic utterance level. Thirdly, whereas the syllabic constituents define constraints on the occurrence and combinations of segments, lateral licensing constraints on combinations of syllabic constituents prohibit local accumulations of marked constituents. 12

(7) Tentative Answers to the Questions in the Call for Papers Do syllables exist? Yes, twice. We have syllables at the level of memory representations (phonotactic phonology) and at the level of production/perception (utterance phonology). At the phonotactic level it might be argued that in addition to onsets and rhyme no further structure is necessary. As such a phonotactic syllable would not exist. Are syllables derived? Is syllable structure lexically distinctive? Syllable structure is part and parcel of lexical representations encoding major class distinctions. As such this structure is distinctive. What is the internal structure of the syllable? See (1) in this hand out which deals with the phonotactic syllable. Essentially, an onset rhyme structure. Moras are simply rhyme positions. The utterance syllable could have a different, perhaps much flatter structure. Are syllables hierarchically dominated by other prosodic categories? Again, that depends on whether we talk about phonotactic or utterance syllables. Phonotactic rhymes may be organized into feet. Utterance syllables are organized into a metrical grid structure. What principles guide the syllabification of a string of phonemes? None, if syllable structure is part of the memory representations. What aspects of syllables are referred to by morphological and phonological rules/constraints? The primary role of phonotactic syllables is being part of the definition of the notion of phonotactic wellformedness. How do phonetic syllables relate to phonological syllables (and vice versa)? Phonetic syllables (here called utterance syllables) and phonological (here: phonotactic) syllables exist at different levels. Levels are not derivationally related; they represent different cognitive analyses of linguistic expressions. We can formulate systematic correspondence relations between the units at both levels. What is the role of sonority for syllables? At the phonotactic level sonority is the degree of V-ness (or the C-lessness). References Hulst, H. van der (2005). The molecular structure of phonological segments. In: P. Carr, J. Durand & C. Ewen (eds.). Headhood, elements, specification and contrastivity. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, 193-234. Hulst, H. van der (2006). Licensing in phonology. The Linguistic Review 23/4, 383-428. 13