Introduction to Phonetics and Phonology 1

Similar documents
Consonants: articulation and transcription

Phonetics. The Sound of Language

Linguistics 220 Phonology: distributions and the concept of the phoneme. John Alderete, Simon Fraser University

Universal contrastive analysis as a learning principle in CAPT

Affricates. Affricates, nasals, laterals and continuants. Affricates. Affricates. Study questions

Phonology Revisited: Sor3ng Out the PH Factors in Reading and Spelling Development. Indiana, November, 2015

source or where they are needed to distinguish two forms of a language. 4. Geographical Location. I have attempted to provide a geographical

SEGMENTAL FEATURES IN SPONTANEOUS AND READ-ALOUD FINNISH

Contrasting English Phonology and Nigerian English Phonology

Word Stress and Intonation: Introduction

The Journey to Vowelerria VOWEL ERRORS: THE LOST WORLD OF SPEECH INTERVENTION. Preparation: Education. Preparation: Education. Preparation: Education

1. REFLEXES: Ask questions about coughing, swallowing, of water as fast as possible (note! Not suitable for all

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

Affricates. Affricates, nasals, laterals and continuants. Affricates. Affricates. Affricates. Affricates 11/20/2015. Phonetics of English 1

The analysis starts with the phonetic vowel and consonant charts based on the dataset:

MASTERY OF PHONEMIC SYMBOLS AND STUDENT EXPERIENCES IN PRONUNCIATION TEACHING. Master s thesis Aino Saarelainen

Rhythm-typology revisited.

Speech Recognition using Acoustic Landmarks and Binary Phonetic Feature Classifiers

Language Acquisition by Identical vs. Fraternal SLI Twins * Karin Stromswold & Jay I. Rifkin

Phonological Processing for Urdu Text to Speech System

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Voiced-voiceless distinction in alaryngeal speech - acoustic and articula

The Perception of Nasalized Vowels in American English: An Investigation of On-line Use of Vowel Nasalization in Lexical Access

Consonant-Vowel Unity in Element Theory*

A Cross-language Corpus for Studying the Phonetics and Phonology of Prominence

On Developing Acoustic Models Using HTK. M.A. Spaans BSc.

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. VCV-sequencies in a preliminary text-to-speech system for female speech

Unvoiced Landmark Detection for Segment-based Mandarin Continuous Speech Recognition

Demonstration of problems of lexical stress on the pronunciation Turkish English teachers and teacher trainees by computer

Language Change: Progress or Decay?

**Note: this is slightly different from the original (mainly in format). I would be happy to send you a hard copy.**

To appear in the Proceedings of the 35th Meetings of the Chicago Linguistics Society. Post-vocalic spirantization: Typology and phonetic motivations

Pobrane z czasopisma New Horizons in English Studies Data: 18/11/ :52:20. New Horizons in English Studies 1/2016

Florida Reading Endorsement Alignment Matrix Competency 1

English Language and Applied Linguistics. Module Descriptions 2017/18

An Acoustic Phonetic Account of the Production of Word-Final /z/s in Central Minnesota English

The Indian English of Tibeto-Burman language speakers*

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

Radical CV Phonology: the locational gesture *

Rachel E. Baker, Ann R. Bradlow. Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA

Correspondence between the DRDP (2015) and the California Preschool Learning Foundations. Foundations (PLF) in Language and Literacy

A Neural Network GUI Tested on Text-To-Phoneme Mapping

A Fact in Historical Phonology from the Viewpoint of Generative Phonology: The Underlying Schwa in Old English

Parallel Evaluation in Stratal OT * Adam Baker University of Arizona

Christine Mooshammer, IPDS Kiel, Philip Hoole, IPSK München, Anja Geumann, Dublin

What the National Curriculum requires in reading at Y5 and Y6

Considerations for Aligning Early Grades Curriculum with the Common Core

age, Speech and Hearii

Using a Native Language Reference Grammar as a Language Learning Tool

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

RP ENGLISH AND CASTILIAN SPANISH DIPHTHONGS REVISITED FROM THE BEATS-AND-BINDING PERSPECTIVE

1 st Quarter (September, October, November) August/September Strand Topic Standard Notes Reading for Literature

The influence of orthographic transparency on word recognition. by dyslexic and normal readers

DEVELOPMENT OF LINGUAL MOTOR CONTROL IN CHILDREN AND ADOLESCENTS

SOUND STRUCTURE REPRESENTATION, REPAIR AND WELL-FORMEDNESS: GRAMMAR IN SPOKEN LANGUAGE PRODUCTION. Adam B. Buchwald

Journal of Phonetics

English for Life. B e g i n n e r. Lessons 1 4 Checklist Getting Started. Student s Book 3 Date. Workbook. MultiROM. Test 1 4

Primary English Curriculum Framework

Fix Your Vowels: Computer-assisted training by Dutch learners of Spanish

Speaker Recognition. Speaker Diarization and Identification

CLASSIFICATION OF PROGRAM Critical Elements Analysis 1. High Priority Items Phonemic Awareness Instruction

DIBELS Next BENCHMARK ASSESSMENTS

Contrastiveness and diachronic variation in Chinese nasal codas. Tsz-Him Tsui The Ohio State University

WiggleWorks Software Manual PDF0049 (PDF) Houghton Mifflin Harcourt Publishing Company

ABSTRACT. Some children with speech sound disorders (SSD) have difficulty with literacyrelated

Books Effective Literacy Y5-8 Learning Through Talk Y4-8 Switch onto Spelling Spelling Under Scrutiny

Acoustic correlates of stress and their use in diagnosing syllable fusion in Tongan. James White & Marc Garellek UCLA

GOLD Objectives for Development & Learning: Birth Through Third Grade

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

ELA/ELD Standards Correlation Matrix for ELD Materials Grade 1 Reading

CEFR Overall Illustrative English Proficiency Scales

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Perceived speech rate: the effects of. articulation rate and speaking style in spontaneous speech. Jacques Koreman. Saarland University

A survey of intonation systems

Stages of Literacy Ros Lugg

NCU IISR English-Korean and English-Chinese Named Entity Transliteration Using Different Grapheme Segmentation Approaches

ADDIS ABABA UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF GRADUATE STUDIES MODELING IMPROVED AMHARIC SYLLBIFICATION ALGORITHM

A Believable Accent: The Phonology of the Pink Panther

Underlying Representations

First Grade Curriculum Highlights: In alignment with the Common Core Standards

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Sound symbolism in deictic words

South Carolina English Language Arts

Learning to Read and Spell Words:

Lexical phonology. Marc van Oostendorp. December 6, Until now, we have presented phonological theory as if it is a monolithic

On the Formation of Phoneme Categories in DNN Acoustic Models

have to be modeled) or isolated words. Output of the system is a grapheme-tophoneme conversion system which takes as its input the spelling of words,

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Lang Speech. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 January 1.

A Trio of Phonetic Details in Homalco

Opportunities for Writing Title Key Stage 1 Key Stage 2 Narrative

Clinical Application of the Mean Babbling Level and Syllable Structure Level

THE PHONOLOGICAL WORD IN STANDARD MALA Y

Taught Throughout the Year Foundational Skills Reading Writing Language RF.1.2 Demonstrate understanding of spoken words,

Markedness and Complex Stops: Evidence from Simplification Processes 1. Nick Danis Rutgers University

Manner assimilation in Uyghur

AGS THE GREAT REVIEW GAME FOR PRE-ALGEBRA (CD) CORRELATED TO CALIFORNIA CONTENT STANDARDS

SARDNET: A Self-Organizing Feature Map for Sequences

Adding Japanese language synthesis support to the espeak system

Taking into Account the Oral-Written Dichotomy of the Chinese language :

KEY 2: PRONOUNCE WORDS CLEARLY

9 Sound recordings: acoustic and articulatory data

Transcription:

Introduction to Phonetics and Phonology 1 Some of the symbols and terms in Baker (2007) and Horobin and Smith (2002) may be unfamiliar to students who have limited experience of phonetics, i.e. THE SCIENTIFIC STUDY OF SPEECH SOUNDS IN LANGUAGES, and phonology, i.e. THE SCIENTIFIC STUDY OF SOUND SYSTEMS OF LANGUAGES. This compendium is intended to help such students to make sense of the account in the textbooks. I first introduce the descriptive apparatus and the symbols used to discuss speech sounds in writing, and then address the basic concepts of notation, phonemes, allophones, syllables, and stress. The account focuses on matters that are relevant to the production of English speech sounds. 1 The Description of Speech Sounds In order to describe variation and change in speech sounds clearly in writing, it is necessary to be able to represent sounds using written symbols that mean the same thing to all readers. This section outlines the use of such phonetic symbols, and the different characteristics of speech sounds that these symbols represent. Speech sounds fall into two major categories: vowels and consonants. I will describe these in turn, starting with vowels. It is important to remember that, when vowels and consonants are discussed phonetically, it is the sound that matters, not the spelling; for instance, in a word like through, the two vowel letters <o> and <u> as well as the two consonant letters <g> and <h> represent the single vowel sound [uː]. As is common practice in linguistics, linguistic forms will appear in italics in this compendium, and <angle brackets> will be used to draw attention specifically to written forms, when necessary. [Square brackets] will be used around exact descriptions of speech sounds, and /slashes/ will be used around the sound units known as phonemes (see section 2.1). 1.1 Vowels All vowel sounds meet two criteria: 1. When they are produced, THE AIR FLOWING THROUGH THE ORAL CAVITY IS RELATIVELY UNIMPEDED. 2. THEY CAN FORM THE NUCLEUS OF A SYLLABLE (see section 2.2.1). The first criterion is the more important one for our purposes. It means that there is no closure anywhere that is tight enough to produce audible friction; instead, which vowel is produced depends on the shape of the articulatory organs. In English, the tongue and the lips are of particular importance. There are two main types of vowel in English. The first kind of vowel is simple or pure in that the tongue, lips, and other articulatory organs stay in the same place from the moment the vowel begins to be produced until the next speech sound starts. Such simple vowels are called monophthongs. English monophthongs can basically be described using four features: 1. Openness. This feature concerns THE VERTICAL POSITION OF THE TONGUE IN THE ORAL CAVITY: close (the tongue almost makes contact with the roof of the mouth), mid-close, mid, 1 I am grateful to Gregory Garretson for his comments on a draft version of this compendium. 1

mid-open, or open (the tongue is at the bottom of the oral cavity). 2 In some sources, this feature is called height, and the vowels are then referred to as being high, midhigh, mid, mid-low, and low instead. 2. Backness. This feature concerns THE HORIZONTAL POSITION OF THE TONGUE IN THE ORAL CAVITY: front (the highest part of the tongue is close to the teeth), central, back (the highest part of the tongue is close to the pharynx). 3. Rounding. This feature has to do with WHETHER OR NOT THE LIPS ARE ROUNDED WHEN THE VOWEL IS PRODUCED: rounded or unrounded. 4. Quantity. This feature concerns THE LENGTH OF THE VOWEL SOUND: long or short. Long vowels are indicated by the phonetic colon sign <ː>. This feature is called duration (and occasionally length) in Baker (2007); Horobin and Smith (2002) use the term quantity. In some languages, e.g. French, nasality whether or not the air passes through the nasal cavity is also an important criterion. But this feature is of lesser importance in English. The other major type of vowel is called a diphthong. In a diphthong, the tongue as well as the lips, in some cases moves from one position to another while the vowel is being produced. Diphthongs are given a double phonetic symbol that combines the symbol for the starting position of the articulatory organs and the symbol for the final position of the articulatory organs; for instance, the symbol for the vowel sound in Present-day English my is [aɪ], which indicates that the articulatory organs start in the position for [a] and move into position for [ɪ] as the vowel is being produced. In Old English, diphthongs could be either long (i.e. equivalent to a long monophthong in length) or short (i.e. equivalent to a short monophthong in length). To distinguish these two categories, long diphthongs are given a [ː] after the first vowel symbol (because the first sound was more prominent in Old English diphthongs), e.g. long [eːo] vs. short [eo]. In Middle English, in contrast, all diphthongs are equivalent to a long monophthong in length, so no length marks are used for Middle English diphthongs. 3 1.2 Consonants In contrast to vowels, English consonants meet at least one of the two following criteria: 1. THEY ARE PRODUCED BY BLOCKING OR RESTRICTING THE AIRSTREAM THROUGH THE VOCAL TRACT. 2. THEY CANNOT FORM THE NUCLEUS OF SYLLABLES. As with vowels, the first criterion is the more important one for our purposes. Consonants in English can be described using three features: 1. Voicing, i.e. WHETHER THE VOCAL FOLDS ARE PULLED APART OR BROUGHT TOGETHER. A consonant sound is thus either unvoiced (the vocal folds are pulled apart) or voiced (the vocal folds are brought together). (In contrast, all vowels are typically voiced.) Some sources use the term voiceless instead of unvoiced. 2. Place of articulation, i.e. WHERE THE AIRSTREAM IS MODIFIED. We chiefly need to include the following categories: a. Bilabial. The upper and lower lip are involved. b. Labiodental. The upper teeth and lower lip are involved. 2 Note that it is not merely the position of the tip of the tongue that is relevant to the distinction between close and open vowels; for instance, to produce the close vowel [u], it is the body of the tongue that is raised towards the roof of the mouth. 3 This is a slightly inconsistent notation, in that a symbol like Old English [eo] indicates a diphthong equivalent to a short monophthong in length, while a symbol like Middle English [ɔɪ] indicates a diphthong equivalent to a long monophthong in length. However, these notations are used in virtually every book on the history of English, so it is necessary to get used to the inconsistency. 2

c. Dental. The tip of the tongue and the front teeth are involved. d. Alveolar. The tip of the tongue and the alveolum the ridge behind the front teeth are involved. e. Postalveolar. The front of the tongue and the sharply rising surface between the alveolum and the hard palate are involved. f. Palatal. The front of the tongue and the hard palate are involved. g. Velar. The back of the tongue and the velum the soft palate are involved. h. Glottal. The vocal folds in the glottis are involved. 3. Manner of articulation, i.e. how the airstream is modified. In English, the most important categories are as follows: a. Stops. Stops are produced by completely closing off the airflow through the mouth, and then releasing it. b. Fricatives. Fricatives are produced by continuous airflow through a narrow opening in the vocal tract, which produces audible friction. These sounds are called spirants in Baker (2007); I follow Horobin and Smith (2002) and use the more frequent term fricatives in this compendium. c. Affricates. Affricates represent a combination of a stop and a following fricative, but count as one single sound. d. Nasals. Like stops, nasals are produced by sealing off the oral cavity at a specific place, which modifies the sound; but the velum is lowered so that the air escapes through the nose instead. Nasals are typically voiced in English. e. Approximants. Approximants are produced with little obstruction of the airflow. They can be subdivided into two groups. i. Liquids include sounds like English [l] and (in most varieties) [r]. ii. Semivowels in English comprise the sounds [j] and [w]. Semivowels are produced in the same way as vowels are, i.e. with very little obstruction of the airflow. However, they count as consonants because they cannot form the nucleus of a syllable. 2 Phonological Concepts 2.1 Phonemes and Allophones The descriptions in section 1 concern speech sounds, or phones. But we need only look at how we use our first language to see that it is not enough to merely describe the sounds in the language in order to explain how they function as a system. Most importantly, in any given language, some differences in articulation will be considered important, in the sense that two sounds that differ in those respects are not considered the same sound. Other differences, in contrast, will be regarded as incidental, in the sense that they are systematic and predictable from the sounds that occur before or after the sound in question. To illustrate this, let us look at a common feature in natural languages known as aspiration. If a consonant is aspirated, it is accompanied by a puff of air. This is signalled in phonetic notation with the symbol [ h ]; an aspirated [p], for instance, is given as [p h ]. The only difference between [p] and [p h ] is aspiration; in other respects the sounds are identical. It is easy to check whether a consonant is aspirated: if you hold the palm of your hand in front of your lips while producing the sound, you will feel the puff of air clearly if the consonant is aspirated, while an unaspirated consonant will produce a far weaker puff or no puff at all. Test the difference between aspirated and unaspirated consonants by doing so 3

while saying first pit and then spit in English. You will notice that pit has an aspirated [p h ], while spit has an unaspirated [p]. This is because of a very simple rule that applies to most Germanic languages (it works for Swedish too): an unvoiced stop [p], [t], or [k] is aspirated at the beginning of a stressed syllable, except when it follows [s]. The [s] in spit blocks the aspiration. The difference between aspirated and unaspirated unvoiced stops in most Germanic languages is thus both systematic and predictable. For this reason, we regard [p] and [p h ] as the same sound in languages like English and Swedish indeed, it is likely that you had never considered the fact that there are two different p sounds in English and Swedish before you read this, because we do not think of such differences as significant. In contrast, let us consider a feature that was introduced in section 1.2: voicing. Consonants may be voiced, in which case the vocal folds are brought together and vibrate as the sound is produced (you can feel this vibration by holding a finger on your throat as you produce a voiced sound). Alternatively, they may be unvoiced, in which case the vocal folds are pulled apart. Let us look at the sounds [v] and [f]. Both sounds are labiodental fricatives; the only difference between them is that [v] is voiced and [f] is unvoiced. But [v] and [f] clearly do not count as the same sound in Present-day English. The easiest way to test this empirically is to find two words that are identical in pronunciation with one single exception: in one word the first of the two sounds we are looking at occurs, and in the other word the second of the two sounds we are looking at occurs in exactly the same place. Such pairs are known as minimal pairs. For [v] and [f], one minimal pair is fan [fæn] and van [væn] (note that only pronunciation counts, while spelling is irrelevant; for instance, few [fjuː] and view [vjuː] also form a minimal pair). Native speakers of Present-day English instantly perceive that fan and van are different words even though they only differ in this one respect. This test thus enables us to conclude that the difference between [f] and [v] is enough to distinguish Present-day English words on its own, and that [f] and [v] are not the same sound in Present-day English. In linguistic terms, they belong to separate phonemes. The phoneme is the smallest distinct sound unit in a particular variety of a language. By contrast, it would be impossible to find a minimal pair for the two sounds [p] and [p h ] in Present-day English, because there is always one more feature that distinguishes them: the [s] that comes before [p] but not before [p h ] (see above). This means that [p] and [p h ] must instead be regarded as two variants of the same English phoneme. Such variants are known in linguistics as allophones. 4 Note that one can only decide what sounds belong to different phonemes and what sounds belong to the same phoneme for a single variety of a language. Different languages and dialects may have different systems; for instance, both Present-day English and Khmer (the official language of Cambodia) have the sounds [p] and [p h ]. As we saw above, these sounds are allophones of the same phoneme /p/ in English; in contrast, in Khmer, these sounds belong to different phonemes /p/ and /p h /, since there are minimal pairs like 4 There are some cases where it is difficult or impossible to find minimal pairs, but where we still conclude that two sounds belong to different phonemes. For instance, [h] and [ŋ], as in hang [hæŋ], never form a minimal pair in Present-day English, because [h] only occurs at the start of a syllable and [ŋ] never does. But because they are so different, we still conclude that they belong to different phonemes. It is very difficult to find minimal pairs for [θ] and [ð] (either/ether is one, if you pronounce either with [iː] or [i]), and they are also similar phonetically. Nevertheless, we conclude that they belong to different phonemes, because their distribution is not predictable; that is, we cannot look at their phonetic context and determine which of them will appear, as we would have been able to do if [θ] and [ð] had been allophones of the same phoneme (cf. the account of [p] and [p h ] above, where, in contrast, the distribution is completely predictable). 4

/pɔːn/ to wish vs. /p h ɔːn/ also. Conversely, we saw above that, in Present-day English, the sounds [f] and [v] belong to separate phonemes /f/ and /v/; but as you will see in Baker (2007), the Old English sounds [f] and [v] were allophones of the same phoneme /f/. 5 There are several phonemes in varieties of English that have important allophones. For instance, in many Present-day English accents, there is a clear [l], used before vowels, as in lab [læb], and a dark, more [ʊ]-like [ɫ], used before consonants and at the end of words, as in milk [mɪɫk] and deal [diːɫ]. These are allophones of the same phoneme because, again, which allophone is used is predictable from the surrounding phonetic context. This predictability is also why we never think about allophonic variation as native speakers: the abstract unit of the phoneme is what we focus on subconsciously. The relative unimportance of allophonic variation can also be seen from many alphabetic writing systems, where one symbol typically corresponds to a phoneme, including its allophones, if any; for instance, we use the same <p> in English to represent [p] and [p h ], and the same <l> to represent [l] and [ɫ]. 6 When we use symbols to represent sounds, it is necessary to distinguish between two kinds of notation, depending on whether we wish to capture the exact sound, or merely symbolize the phonemes. When we are interested in describing the exact sound, we surround the phonetic symbols with [square brackets]; this is known as narrow or phonetic transcription. In contrast, if we are only interested in symbolizing the phoneme, we surround the symbols with /slashes/; we are then using broad or phonemic transcription. In phonemic transcription of Present-day English, pit and spit above would thus have the same symbol representing the unvoiced bilabial stop: pit /pɪt/, spit /spɪt/. But in phonetic transcription, the symbol would be different for these two words: pit [p h ɪt] vs. spit [spɪt]. The relationship between a phoneme and its allophones can be described as in Figure 1. Phonemic level /f/ Allophonic level [v] [f] Distribution (Allophone used between voiced sounds within words) (Allophone used word-initially, word-finally, and within words when preceded and/or followed by an unvoiced sound) Figure 1. The allophones of the Old English phoneme /f/ and their distribution As illustrated in Baker (2007: 14 15), the Old English phoneme /f/ can be realized in two distinct ways [f] and [v] and the realization is dependent on the phonetic context and is thus predictable. When the situation is as described in Figure 1, with one allophone [v] 5 In Old English, the same is true of [s] and [z], which were allophones of the same phoneme /s/, and of [θ] and [ð], which were allophones of the same phoneme /θ/. See Baker (2007: 14 15) for the contexts that determined the occurrence of the unvoiced and voiced allophones. 6 Writing systems are rarely perfect representations of the phonemes in a given language, so this statement is not always true; for instance, in English, <a> represents the vowels in trap, bath, face, palm, and start, and these vowels belong to three different phonemes in most varieties of English. In addition, alphabets are usually more conservative and standardized than speech, which means that recent changes in pronunciation, as well as regional differences from the standard pronunciation, are unlikely to be reflected in alphabets. But the basis of alphabetic writing is typically phonemic, not phonetic. 5

occurring in one specific context and the other allophone [f] occurring in all other contexts, scholars frequently give only the contexts where the first allophone occurs and then simply state that the other one occurs elsewhere ; in this case, [f] would be the elsewhere allophone. 2.2 Beyond the Phoneme There are a few phonological features operative above the level of the individual phoneme that need to be addressed. Two matters will be discussed briefly in this section: syllable structure and stress. 2.2.1 The Syllable A syllable is a unit of sound above the phoneme. Every syllable has a nucleus, and the nucleus is almost always a vowel either a monophthong or a diphthong. 7 Any consonants (one or several) that precede the nucleus in the syllable are known collectively as the onset of the syllable. Any consonants (one or several) that follow the nucleus in the syllable are known collectively as the coda of the syllable. The nucleus and the coda together are known as the rhyme (also spelt rime), because two syllables that have identical nuclei and codas rhyme. Let us look at two one-syllable words to illustrate this: print and mint. syllable syllable rhyme rhyme onset nucleus coda onset nucleus coda pr ɪ nt m ɪ nt The syllables print and mint rhyme because both have the rhyme /ɪnt/. Many words consist of more than one syllable (because they contain more than one vowel). The Old English word nama, for example, which contains the phonemes /n/, /ɑ/, /m/, and /ɑ/, has two syllables, one for each occurrence of the vowel /ɑ/. The /n/ must be the onset of the first syllable; but where does the /m/ belong? In such cases, we count the consonant appearing between vowels as belonging together with the following vowel: the syllable structure is thus /nɑ/ (onset /n/, nucleus /ɑ/) + /mɑ/ (onset /m/, nucleus /ɑ/). If there were two consonants between vowels, as in Old English wordum /wordum/ (the dative plural of word word ) and fremman /fremmɑn/ to advance, one consonant ends up in each syllable; thus fremman would have the structure /frem/ (onset /fr/, nucleus /e/, coda /m/) + /mɑn/ (onset /m/, nucleus /ɑ/, coda /n/); wordum would have the structure /wor/ (onset /w/, nucleus /o/, coda /r/) + /dum/ (onset /d/, nucleus /u/, coda /m/). 8 (See Handout I for why fremman is transcribed with a geminated consonant /mm/.) You need to know this much about syllable division in order to understand two features that are important when 7 There are exceptions, and the most important ones for Present-day English are the consonants /l/ and /n/, which occasionally form the nucleus of syllables. Speakers who do not pronounce an /ǝ/ after the /t/ in words such as bottle /ˈbɒtl bɑːtl/ and Britain /ˈbrɪtn/ are still thought to have two syllables in these words, but the nucleus of the second syllable is /l/ and /n/, respectively. Such consonants are sometimes referred to as syllabic consonants. 8 This account of syllable division is an oversimplification, but it is provides you with the information that is necessary to follow this course. 6

studying Old and Middle English: syllable length and the difference between open and closed syllables. The division of syllables into long and short syllables is important in Old English; for instance, a word may take different endings depending on whether it consists of a long or a short syllable. Only the rhyme (nucleus + coda) is relevant to syllable length. In order to decide whether a syllable is long or short, it is helpful to think of the rhyme as composed of length units where each consonant is equivalent to one unit, each short vowel is equivalent to one unit, and each long vowel is equivalent to two units. In linguistics, this unit is called a mora (plural: morae). 9 In the account in Baker (2007: 20), an Old English syllable is long if its rhyme contains at least two morae. This is why fæt /fæt/ is a long syllable: its rhyme contains two morae, one for /æ/ and one for /t/. Similarly, sǣ /sæː/ is long because its rhyme /æː/ contains two morae on its own. But the two syllables in fæte /fæte/ are both short, since the /t/ is considered to be the onset of the second syllable rather than the coda of the first (see above): the first syllable has only one mora in the rhyme (the nucleus /æ/), and the second syllable also has a rhyme consisting only of one mora (the nucleus /e/). There are two main types of syllable: open syllables and closed syllables. A closed syllable is one that has a coda; both print and mint in Present-day English are thus closed syllables because they have the coda /nt/. An open syllable is one that does not have a coda; Present-day English free /friː/, for instance, is an open syllable, because it has only the onset /fr/ and the nucleus /iː/. Since a single consonant between two vowels is considered to belong with the following vowel, the Old English word nama, which we looked at above, consists of two open syllables: /nɑ/ and /mɑ/. The difference between open and closed syllables will become important when we deal with Middle English, since some vowels were lengthened when they occurred in open syllables (see Horobin and Smith 2002: 59 60). In fact, the reason why name, the present-day version of nama, is pronounced /neɪm/ and not /næm/ in Present-day English is that the first syllable was open in Middle English! 2.2.2 Stress (= Accentuation) Syllables can be stressed or unstressed. 10 A stressed syllable is perceived as more prominent than an unstressed syllable. Stress is signalled in different ways in different languages (pitch, length, loudness, etc.); in English, a stressed syllable is primarily louder than an unstressed syllable, but stressed and unstressed syllables often differ in pitch and length as well. When we look at individual words, there is no need to signal stress if the word has only one syllable, since that syllable will carry lexical stress by default. If there are several syllables in the word, the sign /ˈ/ is used to signal primary stress in a word. Note that stress signs are placed before the syllable that takes the stress. Stress is important in English for several reasons. First, unstressed syllables have tended to be reduced to /ǝ/ over the history of the language. As you will learn during this course, this reduction is one of the most important reasons why English now relies on word order and prepositions rather than word endings to signal the relationship between clause elements: after many endings, which were unstressed, merged as /ə/ (and then 9 You do not have to know the term mora (although it is a useful term to be familiar with). The important thing is that you can tell the difference between long and short syllables in Old English. 10 Baker (2007) uses the term accentuation and discusses the difference between accented syllables and unaccented syllables, while Horobin and Smith (2002) use the terms stress, stressed syllables, and unstressed syllables to account for the same features. I follow Horobin and Smith (2002) in this compendium, as their terminology is more widely used in linguistics. 7

disappeared), it was no longer possible to use endings to distinguish between functions such as subject and indirect object. Secondly, lexical stress was once predictable in English. As Baker (2007: 20 21) shows, all Old English words were stressed on the first syllable, with two regular exceptions: 1. If any word began with the prefix ġe-, the stress fell on the syllable after the prefix. 2. If a verb began with a prefix (i.e. not just ġe-, but any prefix), the stress fell on the syllable after the prefix. Since these rules apply to all Old English words including loanwords there is no need to indicate lexical stress in transcriptions of Old English words. In Middle English, however, stress in English becomes variable, mainly as a result of an influx of French loanwords that are stressed on the last syllable. It is thus necessary to indicate stress in transcriptions of Middle English words. 8