How Well Can We Learn With Standard BCI Training Approaches? A Pilot Study.

Similar documents
Designing Autonomous Robot Systems - Evaluation of the R3-COP Decision Support System Approach

Teachers response to unexplained answers

Smart Grids Simulation with MECSYCO

Students concept images of inverse functions

Towards a MWE-driven A* parsing with LTAGs [WG2,WG3]

Process Assessment Issues in a Bachelor Capstone Project

Specification of a multilevel model for an individualized didactic planning: case of learning to read

A Novel Approach for the Recognition of a wide Arabic Handwritten Word Lexicon

Detection and Classification of Mu Rhythm using Phase Synchronization for a Brain Computer Interface

User Profile Modelling for Digital Resource Management Systems

On Human Computer Interaction, HCI. Dr. Saif al Zahir Electrical and Computer Engineering Department UBC

SOFTWARE EVALUATION TOOL

AGENDA LEARNING THEORIES LEARNING THEORIES. Advanced Learning Theories 2/22/2016

Speech Recognition at ICSI: Broadcast News and beyond

Measurement. When Smaller Is Better. Activity:

Does Linguistic Communication Rest on Inference?

Different Requirements Gathering Techniques and Issues. Javaria Mushtaq

Lecture 2: Quantifiers and Approximation

Robot Learning Simultaneously a Task and How to Interpret Human Instructions

Higher education is becoming a major driver of economic competitiveness

The Impact of Neuroscience on Foreign Languages in School

The Effect of Discourse Markers on the Speaking Production of EFL Students. Iman Moradimanesh

How to Judge the Quality of an Objective Classroom Test

Human Emotion Recognition From Speech

Build on students informal understanding of sharing and proportionality to develop initial fraction concepts.

Running head: DELAY AND PROSPECTIVE MEMORY 1

Paper Reference. Edexcel GCSE Mathematics (Linear) 1380 Paper 1 (Non-Calculator) Foundation Tier. Monday 6 June 2011 Afternoon Time: 1 hour 30 minutes

Medical Complexity: A Pragmatic Theory

Rule Learning With Negation: Issues Regarding Effectiveness

AGS THE GREAT REVIEW GAME FOR PRE-ALGEBRA (CD) CORRELATED TO CALIFORNIA CONTENT STANDARDS

Using EEG to Improve Massive Open Online Courses Feedback Interaction

CONCEPT MAPS AS A DEVICE FOR LEARNING DATABASE CONCEPTS

Australian Journal of Basic and Applied Sciences

OPTIMIZATINON OF TRAINING SETS FOR HEBBIAN-LEARNING- BASED CLASSIFIERS

Maeha a Nui: A Multilingual Primary School Project in French Polynesia

5 th Grade Language Arts Curriculum Map

Raising awareness on Archaeology: A Multiplayer Game-Based Approach with Mixed Reality

Interpreting ACER Test Results

Rule Learning with Negation: Issues Regarding Effectiveness

Language specific preferences in anaphor resolution: Exposure or gricean maxims?

OCR for Arabic using SIFT Descriptors With Online Failure Prediction

Degeneracy results in canalisation of language structure: A computational model of word learning

P. Belsis, C. Sgouropoulou, K. Sfikas, G. Pantziou, C. Skourlas, J. Varnas

Ph.D. in Behavior Analysis Ph.d. i atferdsanalyse

A GENERIC SPLIT PROCESS MODEL FOR ASSET MANAGEMENT DECISION-MAKING

Lip reading: Japanese vowel recognition by tracking temporal changes of lip shape

Understanding and Supporting Dyslexia Godstone Village School. January 2017

GCSE English Language 2012 An investigation into the outcomes for candidates in Wales

LEGO MINDSTORMS Education EV3 Coding Activities

Introduction to Questionnaire Design

PDAs and Handhelds: ICT at your side and not in your face

Technology-mediated realistic mathematics education and the bridge21 model: A teaching experiment

Evolution of Symbolisation in Chimpanzees and Neural Nets

Individual Differences & Item Effects: How to test them, & how to test them well

CROSS COUNTRY CERTIFICATION STANDARDS

Robot manipulations and development of spatial imagery

Degree Qualification Profiles Intellectual Skills

Module 12. Machine Learning. Version 2 CSE IIT, Kharagpur

MOODLE 2.0 GLOSSARY TUTORIALS

Multi-sensory Language Teaching. Seamless Intervention with Quality First Teaching for Phonics, Reading and Spelling

WHEN THERE IS A mismatch between the acoustic

FACULTY OF PSYCHOLOGY

Characteristics of Collaborative Network Models. ed. by Line Gry Knudsen

TIMSS ADVANCED 2015 USER GUIDE FOR THE INTERNATIONAL DATABASE. Pierre Foy

CLASSIFICATION OF PROGRAM Critical Elements Analysis 1. High Priority Items Phonemic Awareness Instruction

SAT MATH PREP:

QuickStroke: An Incremental On-line Chinese Handwriting Recognition System

Communities of Practice: Going One Step Too Far?.

GCSE. Mathematics A. Mark Scheme for January General Certificate of Secondary Education Unit A503/01: Mathematics C (Foundation Tier)

Modeling user preferences and norms in context-aware systems

A Study of Synthetic Oversampling for Twitter Imbalanced Sentiment Analysis

Artificial Neural Networks written examination

Pre-AP Geometry Course Syllabus Page 1

Grade 6: Correlated to AGS Basic Math Skills

Evaluation of Usage Patterns for Web-based Educational Systems using Web Mining

Evaluation of Usage Patterns for Web-based Educational Systems using Web Mining

DYNAMIC ADAPTIVE HYPERMEDIA SYSTEMS FOR E-LEARNING

9.85 Cognition in Infancy and Early Childhood. Lecture 7: Number

Summary results (year 1-3)

An Evaluation of E-Resources in Academic Libraries in Tamil Nadu

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

Introduction to Ensemble Learning Featuring Successes in the Netflix Prize Competition

DegreeWorks Training Guide

STT 231 Test 1. Fill in the Letter of Your Choice to Each Question in the Scantron. Each question is worth 2 point.

GRADUATE STUDENT HANDBOOK Master of Science Programs in Biostatistics

Mathematics process categories

Genevieve L. Hartman, Ph.D.

MADERA SCIENCE FAIR 2013 Grades 4 th 6 th Project due date: Tuesday, April 9, 8:15 am Parent Night: Tuesday, April 16, 6:00 8:00 pm

GCSE Mathematics B (Linear) Mark Scheme for November Component J567/04: Mathematics Paper 4 (Higher) General Certificate of Secondary Education

Circuit Simulators: A Revolutionary E-Learning Platform

1. REFLEXES: Ask questions about coughing, swallowing, of water as fast as possible (note! Not suitable for all

OPAC and User Perception in Law University Libraries in the Karnataka: A Study

Exact Equality and Successor Function : Two Keys Concepts on the Path towards Understanding Exact Numbers

CS Machine Learning

Non-Secure Information Only

First Grade Standards

5. UPPER INTERMEDIATE

The Perception of Nasalized Vowels in American English: An Investigation of On-line Use of Vowel Nasalization in Lexical Access

USER ADAPTATION IN E-LEARNING ENVIRONMENTS

Transcription:

How Well Can We Learn With Standard BCI Training Approaches? A Pilot Study. Camille Jeunet, Alison Cellard, Sriram Subramanian, Martin Hachet, Bernard N Kaoua, Fabien Lotte To cite this version: Camille Jeunet, Alison Cellard, Sriram Subramanian, Martin Hachet, Bernard N Kaoua, et al.. How Well Can We Learn With Standard BCI Training Approaches? A Pilot Study.. 6th International Brain-Computer Interface Conference, Sep 2014, Graz, Austria. 2014. <hal- 01052692> HAL Id: hal-01052692 https://hal.inria.fr/hal-01052692 Submitted on 28 Jul 2014 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access archive for the deposit and dissemination of scientific research documents, whether they are published or not. The documents may come from teaching and research institutions in France or abroad, or from public or private research centers. L archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, émanant des établissements d enseignement et de recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires publics ou privés.

How Well Can We Learn With Standard BCI Training Approaches? A Pilot Study. Camille Jeunet 1, Alison Cellard 2, Sriram Subramanian 3, Martin Hachet 2, Bernard N Kaoua 1 and Fabien Lotte 2 1 University of Bordeaux, Bordeaux, France 2 Inria Bordeaux Sud-Ouest, Talence, France 3 University of Bristol, Bristol, England camille.jeunet@inria.fr Abstract While being very promising, brain-computer interfaces (BCI) remain barely used outside laboratories because they are not reliable enough. It has been suggested that current training approaches may be partly responsible for the poor reliability of BCIs as they do not satisfy recommendations from psychology and are thus inadequate [3]. To determine to which extent such BCI training approaches (i.e., feedback and training tasks) are suitable to learn a skill, we used them in another context (without a BCI) to train 20 users to perform simple motor tasks. While such approaches enabled learning for most subjects, results also showed that 15% of them were unable to learn these simple motor tasks, which is close to the BCI illiteracy rate [1]. This further suggests that current BCI training approaches may be an important factor of illiteracy, thus deserving more attention. 1 Introduction Brain-computer interfaces (BCIs) are communication systems allowing users to interact with the environment, using only their brain activity [6]. BCIs, although very promising, remain barely used outside laboratories because they are not reliable enough [6]. Two main reasons have been identified. The first one, extensively investigated, concerns brain signal processing, with current classification algorithms being still imperfect [1]. The second one concerns the users themselves. Indeed, many users seem unable to acquire good BCI skills (i.e. the capacity to generate specific and stable brain activity patterns): around 20% cannot control a BCI at all (the so-called BCI illiteracy ), while most of the remaining 80% have relatively modest performances [1]. An appropriate training is needed to acquire these skills, especially for Mental Imagery-based BCI (MI-BCI). It has been suggested that currently used training and feedback protocols, which do not take into account recommendations from psychology to optimise human learning, might be partly responsible for BCI illiteracy and poor user performance [3]. For instance, it has been shown that, for efficient learning, training protocols have to fit the user learning style and propose an increasing and adaptive difficulty [3]. Yet standard BCI training protocols are the same for all users [3]. While instructive, these studies only provide theoretical considerations about training approaches. It is therefore necessary to concretely assess whether training approaches used in BCI are appropriate to train a skill. Moreover, it is necessary to perform this evaluation independently of BCI, to rule out possible biases due to BCI complexity, non-stationarity and poor signal-to-noise ratio. Thus in this work, we propose to study these BCI training approaches without using a BCI: participants were asked to learn specific and simple motor tasks using the same feedback and training tasks used for MI-BCI. We then studied how well they could learn such motor tasks to assess the quality of the training approaches, independently of BCI use. We studied here two different approaches: 1) the training approach 1

used in standard MI-BCI [5] and 2) a variant of it which provides some autonomy to the user. Indeed, with the standard approach, no autonomy is given to the user, who always has to perform the tasks required by the protocol. Yet, autonomy is known to increase motivation and learning efficiency in general [3]. Interestingly enough, the study described in [4] obtained promising results when providing more autonomy to a single BCI user. 2 Methods Participants were asked to learn to perform two motor tasks: drawing triangles and circles with a pen on a graphic tablet (see Figure 1(b)), using standard MI-BCI training approaches [5]. Indeed, as with MI-BCI, in which users have to learn a suitable movement imagination strategy, the participants here had to learn the strategy which allows the computer to correctly recognise their drawing, e.g., they had to identify the suitable shape size, angles or speed of drawing. (a) An 8 second-long trial (b) Experimental Setup Figure 1: (a) Outline of a trial from a standard run; (b) Experimental setup. 2.1 Experimental protocol Participants had to learn to draw circles and triangles that can be recognised by the computer during different runs, which were either standard (s) or self-paced (sp). S-runs were composed of 20 trials per task. As shown in Figure 1(a)), at the beginning of each trial a green cross was displayed. After 2s, an auditory cue (a beep) announced the beginning of the task. Then, after 3s, a red arrow was displayed, indicating which task the participant had to perform: continuously drawing circles or triangles upon appearance of a left or right arrow, respectively. After 4.25s, a blue feedback bar appeared and was updated continuously for 4s. Its direction indicated the shape recognized by the classifier (left: circle, right: triangle) and its length was proportional to the classifier output (i.e., the distance to the classifier separating hyperplane), as with MI-BCI. During sp-runs, no instructions were given: the participants were asked to do the motor tasks in an autonomous and free way. Half of the participants were asked to learn using a Standard (S) training approach: they did 4 seven-minute-long s-runs. The other half learned using a training approach with increased autonomy, denoted Partially Self-Paced (PSP) approach: the 1 st and 4 th runs were s-runs, while the 2 nd run was replaced by a 3.5 minute long sp-run followed by a shortened s-run (10 trials per task, 3.5 minutes), and the 3rd run was replaced by a shortened s-run followed by a 3.5 minute long sp-run. The training duration was the same in both conditions. We studied the impact of the condition, S vs. PSP, on the recognition accuracy of triangles and circles over runs (i.e., learning effects) and on subjective experience (using a questionnaire). 20 participants (10 per group) took part in our experiment.

2.2 Signal Processing In order to discriminate triangle from circle pen gestures, we used a pattern recognition approach as in BCI. From the past 1s-long time window (in a sliding window scheme, 937.5ms overlap) of the 2D pen position (16Hz sampling rate), a histogram of angles was computed. More precisely, the angles between each consecutive segment of the time window were first computed. Then the number of angles falling in the ranges 0-30 o, 30-75 o, 75-105 o, 105-150 o and 150-180 o were counted, and these 5 count values were used as input features for a Linear Discriminant Analysis (LDA) classifier. The (subject-independent) LDA classifier was trained on 60 trials from each gesture, from 2 persons (1 left-handed, 1 right-handed). The resulting classifier could discriminate triangles from circles with 73.8% classification accuracy (10-fold cross-validation on the training set), which is an accuracy equivalent to the average accuracy of a MI-BCI [2]. Classification accuracy was measured as the average number of 1s-long time windows correctly classified during the feedback period from each trial (see Figure 1(a)). 2.3 Analyses In order to analyse the interaction between the Condition (2 modalities: S and PSP; independent measures) and the performance obtained at each Run (4 modalities: run1, run2, run3 and run4; repeated measures), we performed a 2-way ANOVA. Morevoer, we asked the participants to complete a Usability Questionnaire (UQ) which measured 4 dimensions: learnability/memorability (LM), efficiency/effectiveness (EE), safety (Saf.) and satisfaction (Sat.). Thus, we did a two-way ANOVA to analyse the interaction between the Condition and the Evaluated Dimension (4 modalities: LM, EE, saf. and sat.; repeated measures). 3 Results & Discussion (a) Standard Condition (b) Partially Self-Paced Condition (c) Questionnaire Figure 2: (a) (a) Performance (pen gesture classification accuracy) over runs in the S Condition; (b) Performance over runs in the PSP Condition; (c) Average UQ scores. Irrespectively of the condition, classification accuracy results (see Figures 2(a) and 2(b)) showed that 17 out of 20 participants managed to learn the task (best run accuracy 82.6%), with accuracies increasing with the number of runs on average, as can be observed in BCI. However, 3 participants did not manage to learn the tasks (best run accuracy < 70%). This is particularly interesting considering the simplicity of the motor tasks which ensured the users could technically perform them. Such training approaches thus seem suboptimal. Moreover, this rate of 15% of people who did not manage to learn is close to the BCI-illiteracy rate (20% [1]). Overall, this suggests that BCI illiteracy may not be due to the user only, but also substantially to the training protocol. Then, results showed neither a main effect of the

Condition [F(1,18)=2.33; p=0.15] nor a Condition X Run interaction [F(3,45)=1.35;p=0.27] when considering all the participants. However, when the 3 illiterates are excluded, the 2-way ANOVA showed a main effect of the Condition [F(1,15)=7.48;p=0.01]: the PSP group seemed to perform better than the S group (meanpsp=91.00 ± 3.82, means=83.29 ± 6.95). However, random sampling of the participants led by chance to a PSP group with classification accuracies for the first run that are higher than that of the S group, which prevents us from drawing any relevant conclusion on comparative learning effects. UQ results (see Figure 2(c)) showed no main effect of the Condition [F(1,18)=0.98; p=0.33]. However, they showed a trend towards a Condition X Evaluated Dimension interaction [F(3,54)=2.40; p=0.077], which is due to the better evaluation of LM, EE and Saf. in the PSP condition than in the S condition, which is not the case for the Sat. dimension. These results suggested that while the PSP approach is not more pleasant to learn with than the S approach, it is easier. Interestingly enough, 8 subjects reported in an open-question of the questionnaire that the feedback was very uninformative, which made learning the tasks difficult. 4 Conclusion This study aimed to concretely assess how well one could learn a given skill with BCI training approaches. To do so, we proposed to study BCI training approaches without using BCI, i.e., we used feedback and training tasks from MI-BCI to train participants to draw triangles and circles (i.e., simple motor tasks) so these can be recognized by the computer. Half of the participants did so using a S training approach while the other half used a PSP one. In terms of learning effects, results unfortunately showed no relevant differences between conditions (S vs. PSP), due to initial performances that differed between conditions, by chance. However, irrespectively of the condition, 15% of the participants (3 out of 20) seemed unable to learn the motor tasks, despite their simplicity. This suggests that such training approaches are not optimal for learning and thus that they may be an important factor of BCI illiteracy. Concerning user experience, UQ showed a tendancy towards a better feeling of learnability/memorability, efficiency/effectiveness and safety with the PSP than with the S approach, while the satisfaction appeared similar for both. Overall, this study confirmed in practice the theoretical analyses from [3] suggesting that current BCI training approaches were suboptimal and need to be changed. In the future, we will increase the number of participants, explore the PSP approach with actual BCIs, and propose new training approaches that consider the user s cognitive style and motivational states to improve both the learning experience and performance. References [1] B.Z. Allison and C. Neuper. Could anyone use a BCI? In Desney S. Tan and Anton Nijholt, editors, Brain-Computer Interfaces, pages 35 54. Springer London, 2010. [2] B Blankertz, C Sannelli, S Halder, EM Hammer, A Kübler, K-R Müller, G Curio, and T Dickhaus. Neurophysiological predictor of SMR-based BCI performance. NeuroImage, 51(4):1303 1309, 2010. [3] F. Lotte, F. Larrue, and C. Mühl. Flaws in current human training protocols for spontaneous BCI: lessons learned from instructional design. Frontiers in Human Neurosciences, 7:568, 2013. [4] C. Neuper, G. Müller, A. Kübler, N. Birbaumer, and G. Pfurtscheller. Clinical application of an EEG-based BCI. Clinical Neurophysiology, 114:399409, 2003. [5] G. Pfurtscheller and C. Neuper. Motor imagery and direct brain-computer communication. proceedings of the IEEE, 89(7):1123 1134, 2001. [6] J. R. Wolpaw and E. W. Wolpaw. BCI: Principles and Practice. Oxford University Press, 2012.