Analyticity in the nominal domain supplement: DP Structure in Japanese

Similar documents
Korean ECM Constructions and Cyclic Linearization

In Udmurt (Uralic, Russia) possessors bear genitive case except in accusative DPs where they receive ablative case.

Case government vs Case agreement: modelling Modern Greek case attraction phenomena in LFG

SOME MINIMAL NOTES ON MINIMALISM *

Approaches to control phenomena handout Obligatory control and morphological case: Icelandic and Basque

Remarks on Classifiers and Nominal Structure in East Asian

EPP Parameter and No A-Scrambling

Words come in categories

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

A Computational Evaluation of Case-Assignment Algorithms

CAS LX 522 Syntax I. Long-distance wh-movement. Long distance wh-movement. Islands. Islands. Locality. NP Sea. NP Sea

Second Language Acquisition of Korean Case by Learners with. Different First Languages

Introduction to HPSG. Introduction. Historical Overview. The HPSG architecture. Signature. Linguistic Objects. Descriptions.

Chapter 3: Semi-lexical categories. nor truly functional. As Corver and van Riemsdijk rightly point out, There is more

LNGT0101 Introduction to Linguistics

LING 329 : MORPHOLOGY

Implementing the Syntax of Japanese Numeral Classifiers

On the Notion Determiner

Derivational and Inflectional Morphemes in Pak-Pak Language

AN EXPERIMENTAL APPROACH TO NEW AND OLD INFORMATION IN TURKISH LOCATIVES AND EXISTENTIALS

An Introduction to the Minimalist Program

Hindi Aspectual Verb Complexes

Basic Syntax. Doug Arnold We review some basic grammatical ideas and terminology, and look at some common constructions in English.

Direct and Indirect Passives in East Asian. C.-T. James Huang Harvard University

Pseudo-Passives as Adjectival Passives

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Underlying and Surface Grammatical Relations in Greek consider

Prepositional Elements in a DM/DRT-based Syntax-Semantics-Interface

THE FU CTIO OF ACCUSATIVE CASE I MO GOLIA *

Constraining X-Bar: Theta Theory

Citation for published version (APA): Veenstra, M. J. A. (1998). Formalizing the minimalist program Groningen: s.n.

Target Language Preposition Selection an Experiment with Transformation-Based Learning and Aligned Bilingual Data

When a Complement PP Goes Missing: A Study on the Licensing Condition of Swiping

THE ACQUISITION OF ARGUMENT ELLIPSIS IN JAPANESE: A PRELIMINARY STUDY* Koji Sugisaki Mie University

Multiple case assignment and the English pseudo-passive *

The Structure of Relative Clauses in Maay Maay By Elly Zimmer

FOCUS MARKING IN GREEK: SYNTAX OR PHONOLOGY? Michalis Georgiafentis University of Athens

1/20 idea. We ll spend an extra hour on 1/21. based on assigned readings. so you ll be ready to discuss them in class

LFG Semantics via Constraints

What Can Neural Networks Teach us about Language? Graham Neubig a2-dlearn 11/18/2017

Compositional Semantics

Heads and history NIGEL VINCENT & KERSTI BÖRJARS The University of Manchester

Derivational: Inflectional: In a fit of rage the soldiers attacked them both that week, but lost the fight.

Tibor Kiss Reconstituting Grammar: Hagit Borer's Exoskeletal Syntax 1

Program in Linguistics. Academic Year Assessment Report

15 The syntax of overmarking and kes in child Korean

Indeterminacy by Underspecification Mary Dalrymple (Oxford), Tracy Holloway King (PARC) and Louisa Sadler (Essex) (9) was: ( case) = nom ( case) = acc

Chapter 4: Valence & Agreement CSLI Publications

Proof Theory for Syntacticians

Pobrane z czasopisma New Horizons in English Studies Data: 18/11/ :52:20. New Horizons in English Studies 1/2016

Hindi-Urdu Phrase Structure Annotation

Linguistic Inquiry, Volume 35, Number 1, Winter 2004, pp (Article)

Language acquisition: acquiring some aspects of syntax.

THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF HUMANITIES & SOCIAL STUDIES

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

Part I. Figuring out how English works

The Inclusiveness Condition in Survive-minimalism

Concept Acquisition Without Representation William Dylan Sabo

Acoustic correlates of stress and their use in diagnosing syllable fusion in Tongan. James White & Marc Garellek UCLA

Construction Grammar. University of Jena.

Tutorial on Paradigms

Lecture 1: Machine Learning Basics

Feature-Based Grammar

Interfacing Phonology with LFG

The analysis starts with the phonetic vowel and consonant charts based on the dataset:

Update on Soar-based language processing

Intension, Attitude, and Tense Annotation in a High-Fidelity Semantic Representation

Passamaquoddy as a Split Ergative Language and Its Consequences for Marantz s Ergative Case Generalization

GRADUATE PROGRAM Department of Materials Science and Engineering, Drexel University Graduate Advisor: Prof. Caroline Schauer, Ph.D.

English Language and Applied Linguistics. Module Descriptions 2017/18

Grammars & Parsing, Part 1:

DOWNSTEP IN SUPYIRE* Robert Carlson Societe Internationale de Linguistique, Mali

Universal Grammar 2. Universal Grammar 1. Forms and functions 1. Universal Grammar 3. Conceptual and surface structure of complex clauses

An Interface between Prosodic Phonology and Syntax in Kurdish

The presence of interpretable but ungrammatical sentences corresponds to mismatches between interpretive and productive parsing.

Theoretical Syntax Winter Answers to practice problems

Minding the Absent: Arguments for the Full Competence Hypothesis 1. Abstract

Argument structure and theta roles

Introduction to Causal Inference. Problem Set 1. Required Problems

Lexical phonology. Marc van Oostendorp. December 6, Until now, we have presented phonological theory as if it is a monolithic

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

ON THE SYNTAX AND SEMANTICS

The Syntax of Case and Agreement: its Relationship to Morphology and. Argument Structure

A is an inde nite nominal pro-form that takes antecedents. ere have

Phenomena of gender attraction in Polish *

UC Berkeley Dissertations, Department of Linguistics

cambridge occasional papers in linguistics Volume 8, Article 3: 41 55, 2015 ISSN

Classification. Universals

Noun incorporation in Sora: A case for incorporation as morphological merger TLS: 19 February Introduction.

Control and Boundedness

Constructions with Lexical Integrity *

The ADDIE Model. Michael Molenda Indiana University DRAFT

How to analyze visual narratives: A tutorial in Visual Narrative Grammar

cmp-lg/ Jul 1995

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

Laporan Penelitian Unggulan Prodi

VERB MEANINGS AND THEIR EFFECTS ON SYNTACTIC BEHAVIORS: A STUDY WITH SPECIAL REFERENCE TO ENGLISH AND JAPANESE ERGATIVE PAIRS

The Pennsylvania State University. The Graduate School. College of the Liberal Arts THE TEACHABILITY HYPOTHESIS AND CONCEPT-BASED INSTRUCTION

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

Intervention in Tough Constructions * Jeremy Hartman. Massachusetts Institute of Technology

Transcription:

Andrew Simpson LSA Syntactic Analyticity MIT July 26th July 2005 Analyticity in the nominal domain supplement: DP Structure in Japanese Challenges relating to analyticity in the analysis of DPs in Japanese Watanabe, Akira. 2005. Functional Projections of Nominals in Japanese: Syntax of Classifiers. Ms. University of Tokyo. To appear in Natural Language and Linguistic Theory. Point of departure/primary goal: to give a unified account of the structure and derivation of 4 different nominal patterns in Japanese: (1) a. John-wa hon san-satsu-o katta John-TOP book 3-CL-ACC bought 'John bought three books.' b. John-wa san-satsu-no hon-o katta John-TOP 3-CL-GEN book -ACC bought c. John-wa hon-o san-satsu katta John-TOP book-acc 3-CL bought d. John-wa san-satsu hon-o katta John-TOP 3-CL book -ACC bought Patterning in (1): (2) a. N(P) Num CL ACC b. Num CL GEN N(P) ACC c. N(P) ACC Num CL d. Num CL N(P) ACC Important assumption: (1/2a-d) should be related to a common base structure, and all comprise single, nominal constituents. Area of possible dispute: patterns (a), (b) and (d) may be easily accepted as constituting unitary, nominal phrases, but it may be claimed that pattern (c) contains TWO separate constituents [N(P) + ACC] and [Num + CL], and that the post-nominal [Num + CL] is not part of the same nominal phrase as the [N(P) + ACC].

Argument for [N(P) + ACC] and [Num + CL] as a single constituent: possible occurrence in clefting: (3) John-ga katta-no-wa hon-o san-satsu desu. John-NOM bought-c-top book-acc 3-CL be 'It is three books that John bought.' Argument against the above: multiple constituents may seem to be able to occur in clefts, so occurrence of [N(P) + ACC] and [Num + CL] in clefts does not guarantee this to be a single constituent: (4) Mary-ga ageta-no-wa John-ni ringo-o mit-tsu desu. Mary-NOM gave-c-top John-DAT apple 3-CL be 'It is three apples to John that Mary gave.' Important analytic aspects of the analysis: I. The CASE morphemes -o and -ga are treated as instantiations of the head of a Case Phrase. II. Number and Classifier elements are treated as instantiations of different positions, and are not assumed to constitute a single head (as in Muromatsu 1998, Kitahara 1993). Compared to Kitahara 1993 on patterns (a) and (c): (5) common base DP case can be base-generated either on NP structure either on NP (pattern (c)) NCP D or on NC (pattern (a)) NP NC N san-satsu (-o) 3.CL (-ACC) hon (-o) book (-ACC) For Kitahara, ACC case is checked against D 0 by moving NCP to SpecDP if case is basegenerated on NC (pattern (a)), and by moving NP to SpecDP if case is base-generated on N (pattern (c)).

Common base structure assumed by Watanabe 2005 (6) DP QP D CaseP Q #P Case Nom/Acc case morphemes always occur in Case 0 Classifiers occur in # 0 It is suggested that numerals occur in Spec#P: (7) #P san 3 #' hon book satsu CL Because the linear sequence [numeral NP classifier] is never attested, NP always moves to a higher position, SpecCaseP. Movement is triggered by an EPP feature in Case 0. This derives pattern (a): (8) hon san satsu o Pattern (a) book 3 CL ACC CaseP NP Case' hon #P Case san #' o t satsu

When Q is merged, there is optional raising of #P to SpecQP: (9) QP #P Q' san satsu CaseP Q hon Case' t Case PF attachment of a genitive case-morpheme -no to #P then results in pattern (b): (10) san satsu-no hon-o 3 CL GEN book ACC Pattern (c) results from further movement applying to the structure in (9). When D is merged, CaseP is optionally attracted to SpecDP: (11) DP CaseP D' hon-o QP D o #P Q' san satsu CaseP Q (12) hon o san satsu book ACC 3 CL Finally, for pattern (d), [san satsu hon-o], it is suggested that there may be two options: Option I. CaseP in (11) is moved out of DP, and then DP is scrambled further forward: (i) [ CaseP hon-o] i [ DP san satsu t i ] (ii) [ DP san satsu t i ] k [ CaseP hon-o] i t k

Option II. The numeral + classifier are base-generated as a unit outside of, and preceding the DP as a secondary predicate or adverb. Such options are intended to capture the conclusion reached by other researchers that the numeral + classifier do not form a constituent with the NP + case-marker in pattern (d). Instantiations of D and SpecQP D 0 is suggested to (sometimes) be instantiated by the morphemes -mo and -ka in (13). The accompanying, quantificational element dono is suggested to occupy SpecQP. (13) a.dono hon-mo which book MO 'every book' b. dono hon-ka which book KA 'some book' (14) DP QP D dono Q' ka/mo which CaseP Q hon i book Case' #P Case t i Other quantifiers such as takusan 'many' are assumed to occupy SpecQP when they occur. Challenges for a fully 'analytic' analysis of Japanese DPs The basic problem: deciding which overt morphemes provide reliable indications of underlying structure. Issue I. The position of numerals. It is suggested that numerals occur in a specifier position (Spec#P) because they appear to allow modification, and therefore might be concluded to be phrasal constituents:

(15) a. at least three books b. more than three books 1 (16) John-wa sukunakutomo san-satsu-no-hon-o katta John-TOP at least 3-CL-GEN-book-ACC bought 'John bought at least three books.' (17) #P underlying structure [sukunakutomo san] at least 3 #' hon book satsu CL Complication: the Japanese equivalent of 'more than' occurs following the classifier, in a position that is unexpected if it is base-generated in Spec#P: (18) John-wa san-satsu-ijou-no-hon-o katta John-TOP 3-CL-more.than-GEN-book-ACC bought 'John bought more than three books.' It is suggested that ijou is a suffix which is relocated from SpecQP to a position following the classifier after Spell-Out. This aspect of the overt linear ordering of elements is/has to be treated as deceptive and does not reveal the underlying syntactic position of a functional head. Issue II. The genitive case morpheme no It is suggested that: 'insertion of no is a matter of morphology and is not represented structurally' (p.16). Genitive no is simply added to part of the linear string produced by syntax in the post-spell-out part of the derivation. Reasoning: no is iterable and so might not be thought to have a unique structural position: (19) tsugi-no suugaku-no mondai next-gen maths-gen problem 'the next maths problem' 1 Note that phrasal modifiers such as 'at least' and 'more than' could also be analysed as being the (phrasal) specifiers of a Number Phrase headed by numerals. The occurrence of such modifiers with numerals consequently does not force an analysis of the latter as occurring in a specifier position. To the extent that elements in specifier positions are assumed to 'specify' more precisely the properties of the head of a phrase (as in 'very angry at John', 'right over in the corner' etc), 'at least' and 'more than' might seem to be natural candidates for classification as specifiers of numerals. Note also that even if 'at least' and 'three' are assumed to occur together in a specifier position, they must be combined to form some kind of phrase with internal structure in Spec#P, hence numerals will be the heads of a numeral phrase/nump modified by 'at least/more than' even if this NumP is assumed to be located in the specifier of some other phrase.

Another overt morpheme which is assumed not to correspond to a position in syntactic structure. Case morphemes do not uniformly identify the underlying presence of a Case Phrase and a Case head position. Accusative, Dative and Nominative do. Genitive does not. Related issue: Nominative -ga is iterable. How should this affect its structural analysis? GENERAL: There are different views/positions on the potential correspondence of overt morphemes to underlying syntactic head positions. The strong analytic view: Julien, Marit. 2002. Syntactic Heads and Word Formation. Oxford: Oxford University Press. There is no morphological component separate from syntax. Syntax performs the task of assembling all morphemes into words. Syntax + independent morphology. Some overt morphemes correspond to syntactic heads, others may be introduced by post-spell-out morphology and not correspond to any syntactic head positions. The analysis of a syntactic structure one arrives at may depend on (at least) two important factors: (a) Is the strongly analytic view held, that overt morphemes invariably correspond to syntactic head positions? (b) Which morphemes does one 'start' one's analysis from? E.g. if one assumed that Japanese ijou in (18) was in a head position, this might lead to a rather different analysis of DP-internal structure here. If genitive no was assumed to be a syntactic head, this might also affect the analysis in significant ways. Issue III. The confounding power of morphology. An additional complication introduced into the probing of underlying syntactic structure from the linear position of overt morphemes and words comes from the apparent power of post-spell-out morphology to reverse orders created in syntax. An example case: in Japanese DPs, when dono 'which' and -mo/-ka occur together with a case morpheme, the linear order is the opposite to what is expected from the analysis built up from other careful argumentation, represented in (14):

(14) DP QP D dono Q' ka/mo which CaseP Q hon i book Case' #P Case t i ga/o As shown in (20), the case morpheme FOLLOWS -mo/-ka rather than PRECEDES them: (20) a. dono hon-ka-o b. dono hon-ka-ga which book-ka-acc which book-ka-nom 'some book' 'some book' b. dono hon-mo-ga which book-mo-nom 'every book' Here it has to be suggested that the syntactically created order of the case particle and the quantificational particle is 'flipped' under adjacency in the morphological component. Flipping is assumed to result from morphological merger (Halle and Marantz 1993, Distributed Morphology). Such flipping seems to be independently necessary to account for the linear ordering of postpositions and the quantificational particles. -mo has to occur outside P, whereas the syntactic structure should result in the order [ PP [ DP dono gakusei-mo] P ]: (21) PP DP dono gakusei -mo/-ka which student -MO/-KA P kara from (22) a.*dono gakusei-mo-kara b. dono gakusei-kara-mo which student-mo from which student from-mo 'from every student' Concerning the position of -ka, it is possible for it to occur EITHER following P, OR preceding it:

(23) a. dono gakusei-ka-kara b. dono gakusei-kara-ka which student-ka from which student from-ka 'from some student' 'from some student' The optionality in linear positioning seen with -ka relative to P seems to provide good support for the assumption that certain linear orders built by syntax can be converted into other orders (under adjacency) by post spell-out morphology. For the purposes of identifying the underlying syntactic structure of constituents from the surface linear ordering of overt morphemes, such morphological reversal of syntactically created orders is clearly a difficult factor to control for, and can be very deceptive. Conclusions Phrasal movement seems likely to be involved in the derivation of DPs in Japanese too, as in Southeast Asian languages. In a head-final language such as Japanese, head-movement within DP does not provide clear evidence for functional projections above NP, as this will be string-vacuous movement and undetectable. Phrasal movement and the landing-sites it requires does provide information about functional structure above NP. The existence of functional projections above NP is confirmed by (a) DP-internal patterns which require analyses of phrasal movement, and (b) the occurrence of overt heads of DP-internal functional categories (e.g. -mo, -ka, case-markers, classifiers). Interesting challenges arise when the attempt is made to map all overt DP-internal morphemes to functional heads. Certain overt morphemes may perhaps have to be concluded NOT to provide reliable information about the existence and location of functional heads. References Halle, Morris and Alec Marantz. 1993. Distributed morphology and the pieces of inflection. In Kenneth Hale and Samuel Jay Keyser (eds.), The View from Building 20: essays in honor of Sylvain Bromberger, Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, pp.111-176. Kitahara, Hisatsugu. 1993. Numeral Classifier Phrases inside DP and the Specificity Effect. In S. Choi (ed.) Japanese/Korean Linguistics 3, CSLI Publications, Stanford, pp.171-186. Muromatsu, Keiko. 1998. On the Syntax of Classifiers. Doctoral dissertation, University of Maryland.