Introduction. Smiles are a prototypical facial expression of joy and positive emotion. A smile is formed when

Similar documents
2 months: Social and Emotional Begins to smile at people Can briefly calm self (may bring hands to mouth and suck on hand) Tries to look at parent

Client Psychology and Motivation for Personal Trainers

2014 Free Spirit Publishing. All rights reserved.

Promoting the Social Emotional Competence of Young Children. Facilitator s Guide. Administration for Children & Families

Contribution of facial and vocal cues in the still-face response of 4-month-old infants

Chapter 1 Notes Wadsworth, Cengage Learning

Elizabeth R. Crais, Ph.D., CCC-SLP

Shared Book Reading between Mother and Infant Facilitates The Frequency of Joint Attention

Bayley scales of Infant and Toddler Development Third edition

Cognitive Development Facilitator s Guide

IMPLEMENTING THE EARLY YEARS LEARNING FRAMEWORK

THE HEAD START CHILD OUTCOMES FRAMEWORK

Genevieve L. Hartman, Ph.D.

Introduction to Psychology

GOLD Objectives for Development & Learning: Birth Through Third Grade

Linking object names and object categories: Words (but not tones) facilitate object categorization in 6- and 12-month-olds

Shyness and Technology Use in High School Students. Lynne Henderson, Ph. D., Visiting Scholar, Stanford

Coping with Crisis Helping Children With Special Needs

Course Law Enforcement II. Unit I Careers in Law Enforcement

Infant Behavior and Development

Planning Theory-Based and Evidence-Based Health Promotion Interventions. An Intervention Mapping Approach

Language Development: The Components of Language. How Children Develop. Chapter 6

5 Early years providers

Speak with Confidence The Art of Developing Presentations & Impromptu Speaking

Behaviors: team learns more about its assigned task and each other; individual roles are not known; guidelines and ground rules are established

CDTL-CELC WORKSHOP: EFFECTIVE INTERPERSONAL SKILLS

Illinois WIC Program Nutrition Practice Standards (NPS) Effective Secondary Education May 2013

Experience Corps. Mentor Toolkit

Paper presented at the ERA-AARE Joint Conference, Singapore, November, 1996.

Cognition 112 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Cognition. journal homepage:

The Learning Tree Workshop: Organizing Actions and Ideas, Pt I

Speech/Language Pathology Plan of Treatment

YMCA SCHOOL AGE CHILD CARE PROGRAM PLAN

Visual processing speed: effects of auditory input on

Updated: December Educational Attainment

Alberta Police Cognitive Ability Test (APCAT) General Information

CDA Renewal 1: Professionalism-Beliefs, Knowledge, Action

Philosophy of Literacy Education. Becoming literate is a complex step by step process that begins at birth. The National

Introduction to the HFLE course

The Development of Altruistic Behavior: Helping in Children and Chimpanzees

Human Development: Life Span Spring 2017 Syllabus Psych 220 (Section 002) M/W 4:00-6:30PM, 120 MARB

BASIC EDUCATION IN GHANA IN THE POST-REFORM PERIOD

Haddonfield Memorial High School

INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY

Behavior List. Ref. No. Behavior. Grade. Std. Domain/Category. Social/ Emotional will notify the teacher when angry (words, signal)

Colorado Academic. Drama & Theatre Arts. Drama & Theatre Arts

AGENDA LEARNING THEORIES LEARNING THEORIES. Advanced Learning Theories 2/22/2016

Assessing and Providing Evidence of Generic Skills 4 May 2016

Indicators Teacher understands the active nature of student learning and attains information about levels of development for groups of students.

Final Teach For America Interim Certification Program

WHY DID THEY STAY. Sense of Belonging and Social Networks in High Ability Students

PERFORMANCE COMPETENCE LIFESPAN FRAMEWORK

Special Education Services Program/Service Descriptions

Critical Thinking in Everyday Life: 9 Strategies

How Does It Feel? Sensory Processing, Brain Functioning and Behavior. Agenda. Acknowledgements. Presented by Gerry Morgan, M.S. Behavior Specialist

Inclusion in Music Education

Dr. Shaheen Pasha Division of Education University of Education, Lahore

PREP S SPEAKER LISTENER TECHNIQUE COACHING MANUAL

Second Step Suite and the Whole School, Whole Community, Whole Child (WSCC) Model

Part I. Figuring out how English works

PEDAGOGY AND PROFESSIONAL RESPONSIBILITIES STANDARDS (EC-GRADE 12)

Lecture 2: Quantifiers and Approximation

Accessing Higher Education in Developing Countries: panel data analysis from India, Peru and Vietnam

Stimulating Techniques in Micro Teaching. Puan Ng Swee Teng Ketua Program Kursus Lanjutan U48 Kolej Sains Kesihatan Bersekutu, SAS, Ulu Kinta

Milton Public Schools Special Education Programs & Supports

Human Factors Engineering Design and Evaluation Checklist

UNESCO Bangkok Asia-Pacific Programme of Education for All. Embracing Diversity: Toolkit for Creating Inclusive Learning-Friendly Environments

CORRELATION FLORIDA DEPARTMENT OF EDUCATION INSTRUCTIONAL MATERIALS CORRELATION COURSE STANDARDS / BENCHMARKS. 1 of 16

International Partnerships in Teacher Education: Experiences from a Comenius 2.1 Project

THE FIELD LEARNING PLAN

Kindergarten Iep Goals And Objectives Bank

Alpha provides an overall measure of the internal reliability of the test. The Coefficient Alphas for the STEP are:

Training Course Toolkit for participants

Conversation Starters: Using Spatial Context to Initiate Dialogue in First Person Perspective Games

GETTING THE MOST OF OUT OF BRAINSTORMING GROUPS

STAFF DEVELOPMENT in SPECIAL EDUCATION

CEEF 6306 Lifespan Development New Orleans Baptist Theological Seminary

Eliciting Language in the Classroom. Presented by: Dionne Ramey, SBCUSD SLP Amanda Drake, SBCUSD Special Ed. Program Specialist

Introduction to Questionnaire Design

Soaring With Strengths

SUPPORTING AND EDUCATING TRAUMATIZED STUDENTS. CSSP Conference 2014 Barb Bieber

MINISTRY OF EDUCATION. This syllabus replaces previous NSSC syllabuses and will be implemented in 2010 in Grade 11

Participant Application & Information

Two heads can be better than one

Sex Differences in Self-Efficacy and Attributions: Influence of Performance Feedback

PROGRAM REQUIREMENTS FOR RESIDENCY EDUCATION IN DEVELOPMENTAL-BEHAVIORAL PEDIATRICS

International Variations in Divergent Creativity and the Impact on Teaching Entrepreneurship

QUEEN ELIZABETH S SCHOOL

Written by Joseph Chilton Pearce Thursday, 01 March :00 - Last Updated Wednesday, 25 February :34

Greek Teachers Attitudes toward the Inclusion of Students with Special Educational Needs

AN INTRODUCTION (2 ND ED.) (LONDON, BLOOMSBURY ACADEMIC PP. VI, 282)

Setting the Scene and Getting Inspired

SSIS SEL Edition Overview Fall 2017

1 Copyright Texas Education Agency, All rights reserved.

Tracy Dudek & Jenifer Russell Trinity Services, Inc. *Copyright 2008, Mark L. Sundberg

Special Educational Needs and Disabilities Policy Taverham and Drayton Cluster

Non-Secure Information Only

UNIVERSITY OF UTAH VETERANS SUPPORT CENTER

Effective practices of peer mentors in an undergraduate writing intensive course

Study Abroad Housing and Cultural Intelligence: Does Housing Influence the Gaining of Cultural Intelligence?

Transcription:

Smiling Introduction Smiles are a prototypical facial expression of joy and positive emotion. A smile is formed when the zygomatic major muscle contracts and pulls the lip corners laterally upward. In addition to communicating happiness, smiles elicit positive engagement in others. This dynamic process of expressing and perceiving positive emotion contributes to the emergence of social competence in the developing child. This entry takes a lifespan approach, considering research on the development and role of smiling in infants, children, and adults. Infancy First smiles Newborn infants tend to smile more frequently during drowsy and sleeping states, but smiles sometimes occur during non-sleep states during sensory stimulation. Over the first two months of life, infant smiling becomes increasingly linked to auditory and visual stimulation during nonsleep states setting the stage for the emergence of social smiling. Interactive smiles Infant smiles create and maintain positively-toned social interactions with caregivers. Social smiling smiling at the parent while gazing at the parent s face develops during the second month of life. Infant s actively engage in interaction with the parent through social smiling. Between two and six months, infants increasingly respond to and initiate smiles with others. This

interactive smiling is linked to gazing at the parent s face and is often accompanied by vocalizations which express positive engagement. Different types of smiles Smiles are recognized as expressions of positive emotion. However, some smiles are more joyful than others. Smiles that involve eye constriction and the raising of the cheeks around the eyes Duchenne smiles are more likely to occur in circumstances that elicit positive emotion and are perceived as more emotionally positive than non-duchenne smiles, which do not involve eye constriction. The same is true of smiles involving mouth opening. Smiles that combine both of these factors involve the strongest smiling action and may be the most emotionally positive (see Figure 1). Figure 1. A 6-month-old infant displays a strong smile, evidenced by both the Duchenne marker (eye constriction) and mouth opening. The form and timing of infant smiles change with development. In the first six months, smiles involving both eye constriction and mouth opening are increasingly likely to occur during highly joyful, social situations such as when the infant is gazing at the smiling mother. During the same period, infants begin to gaze away from the parent while smiling to regulate their own emotional arousal. In fact, overall, infants time their smile onsets and offsets in a fashion that maximizes the time when only the mother, and not the infant, is smiling. Between six and twelve months, smiles become increasingly likely to involve vocalizations such as laughter which convey emotional intensity. Infant smiles with laughter often occur during physical games like tickling

and visual games like peek-a-boo. As infants become more active agents in social games (e.g., moving the parent s hands in peek-a-boo), they become partners in producing their own positive expressions. Smiles in referential communication Between eight and twelve months, infants increasingly use smiles to referentially communicate enjoyment of objects and experiences to their social partners. Smiles begin to occur during coordinated joint attention in which the infant actively shifts attention between a toy and a social partner. Around eight months, infants begin to initiate joint attention by gazing between a toy and an adult and begin to integrate a smile into a gaze at an adult. One example is anticipatory smiling, in which an infant smiles at a toy and then turns to gaze at an adult, which may serve to communicate that the infant wants to share a humorous experience ( that s a funny toy ). Perception of smiles Infants not only produce smiles, they perceive the smile of others. When infant smiles are influenced by the smiles of others, it suggests the infants comprehension of the affective meaning of the smile. By four months, infants are able to discriminate between happy (smiling) faces and fearful expressions. Between twelve and eighteen months, comprehension of smiles during social referencing paradigms provides evidence for the development of social communication between infants and parents. In social referencing paradigms, infants are more likely to interpret an ambiguous stimulus as nonthreatening when an adult smiles. This suggests the infant interprets the adult s smile not as a direct expression of happiness, but as a referential signal.

Childhood Toddler smiles More is known about smiling in infancy than in childhood, as other components of social competence become more salient, and opportunities for close observation of the face become less frequent. However, smiling remains a critical element of play with parents as toddlers become more aware of the social meaning of smiles. Around two years, for example, smiling serves as a marker of pretense. Mothers smile more during pretense activities than during functional activities (e.g., pretending vs. really eating a snack). In response, toddlers participate and smile more during pretend activities when mothers smile more. Smiles in preschoolers peer play By eighteen months, smiling is associated with interaction amongst peers, an increasingly important context for social development. Specific kinds of smiling become increasingly socially patterned with development. Between two and four years of age, Duchenne smiles and openmouth smiles become increasingly used with same-sex peers. Boys use open-mouth smiles with male rather than female peers. These smiles may reflect developing social relationships, such as emerging sex segregation in young children. By four years, preschoolers exhibit stronger smiles, evidenced by mouth opening and eye constriction, during periods of social proximity and success. Sociality of smiling in older children

As children age, social context exerts a continued influence on smiling. Preschoolers exhibit higher levels of Duchenne smiles when playing with an experimenter than when playing alone. By five years, children produce more Duchenne smiles when they succeed in games than when they fail. At six years, children evidence similar levels of smiling after receiving a prize when alone or when accompanied by an experimenter. By eight years, children show fewer strong smiles when alone than when with the experimenter, highlighting the role of sociality in the development of smile production. Adulthood Duchenne smiles There is strong evidence for the sociality of smiles in adults. Duchenne smiles are robustly produced by interaction with others but are not always produced during feelings of intense joy. Nevertheless, adult Duchenne smiles are often perceived to be a spontaneous reflection of positive emotion, while non-duchenne smiles are perceived to be a reflection of false or fake positive emotion. Observers attribute a multitude of positive characteristics to individuals displaying Duchenne smiles. They are perceived as more genuine, generous, and extroverted than individuals producing non-duchenne smiles. Deliberate Duchenne smiles Although Duchenne smiles were initially thought to be spontaneous, a minority of individuals can deliberately produce a Duchenne smile even in the absence of positive emotion. Deliberate Duchenne smiles are perceived as more positive than non-duchenne smiles. Individuals may use a deliberate Duchenne smile in interpersonal settings for utilitarian purposes such as disguising

their intentions. However, spontaneous Duchenne smiles are perceived as more positive than deliberate Duchenne smiles. This suggests the importance of temporal dynamics in naturalistic smile production. Spontaneous smiles, for example, involve a slower onset and offset than deliberate smiles. Social bonding The Duchenne smile is an important signal in the formation and maintenance of social groups. Duchenne smiles are displayed more during sharing tasks and may serve to advertise altruistic intentions. Mutual Duchenne smiling reflects the shared goals and positive affect of the group. Additionally, moderate alcohol consumption is linked to increases in mutual Duchenne smiles among group members. Smiles with mouth opening are facilitated by alcohol intake and may have similar social cohesion functions, but less is known about this type of smiling in adults. Sex differences Women smile and laugh more than men, but these sex differences are moderated by social norms. For example, the tendency of women to smile more than men is most evident when individuals believe they are being observed, suggesting the role of gender expectations in shaping smile production. Women are encouraged to be emotionally expressive and affiliative, and smiling serves this gender norm. By contrast, men are socialized to exhibit dominance, and reduced smiling serves this gender norm. Consequently, sex differences in smiling are less evident when individuals are constrained by other social norms. For example, service providers are encouraged to be friendly, while funeral directors must appear somber. Such situational constraints reduce sex differences in smiling between women and men.

Conclusion Throughout the lifespan, smiles are a well-recognized facial signature of positive emotion. The form and timing of smiles changes rapidly early in life as infants become more active participants in positive social exchanges and in regulating their own emotions. In early childhood, smiling becomes an essential feature of developing peer play and relationships. By four years old, smiles reflect the social structure of peer interactions; by eight, smiles are expressed preferentially in the presence of another. In adulthood, smiling continues to index positive emotion but is strongly influenced by social context and roles. Katherine B. Martin and Daniel S. Messinger See also Infancy; Facial Expressions, Perception of; Social Development; Parent-Child Interactions; Emotion Expression; Happiness Further Readings Ruvolo, P., Messinger, D., & Movellan, J. (2015). Infants Time Their Smiles to Make Their Moms Smile. PloS one, 10(9), e0136492. Mattson, W. I., Cohn, J. F., Mahoor, M. H., Gangi, D. N., & Messinger, D. S. (2013). Darwin s Duchenne: Eye constriction during infant joy and distress. PLoS ONE, 8 (11). doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0080161.

Krumhuber, E. G., & Manstead, A. S. (2009). Can Duchenne smiles be feigned? New evidence on felt and false smiles. Emotion, 9(6), 807. Hess, U., Adams Jr, R., & Kleck, R. (2005). Who may frown and who should smile? Dominance, affiliation, and the display of happiness and anger. Cognition & Emotion, 19(4), 515-536. Holodynski, M. (2004). The miniaturization of expression in the development of emotional selfregulation. Developmental Psychology, 40(1), 16. Ambadar, Z., Cohn, J. F., & Reed, L. I. (2009). All smiles are not created equal: Morphology and timing of smiles perceived as amused, polite, and embarrassed/nervous. Journal of nonverbal behavior, 33(1), 17-34.