MULTIATTACHMENT AND CHAINS IN ROMANIAN GERUND CLAUSES

Similar documents
Approaches to control phenomena handout Obligatory control and morphological case: Icelandic and Basque

Pseudo-Passives as Adjectival Passives

ELD CELDT 5 EDGE Level C Curriculum Guide LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT VOCABULARY COMMON WRITING PROJECT. ToolKit

Basic Syntax. Doug Arnold We review some basic grammatical ideas and terminology, and look at some common constructions in English.

Introduction to HPSG. Introduction. Historical Overview. The HPSG architecture. Signature. Linguistic Objects. Descriptions.

Underlying and Surface Grammatical Relations in Greek consider

Theoretical Syntax Winter Answers to practice problems

Argument structure and theta roles

Korean ECM Constructions and Cyclic Linearization

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

1/20 idea. We ll spend an extra hour on 1/21. based on assigned readings. so you ll be ready to discuss them in class

Hindi Aspectual Verb Complexes

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Case government vs Case agreement: modelling Modern Greek case attraction phenomena in LFG

Developing Grammar in Context

Control and Boundedness

Advanced Grammar in Use

Update on Soar-based language processing

Words come in categories

The Structure of Relative Clauses in Maay Maay By Elly Zimmer

Multiple case assignment and the English pseudo-passive *

Written by: YULI AMRIA (RRA1B210085) ABSTRACT. Key words: ability, possessive pronouns, and possessive adjectives INTRODUCTION

Constraining X-Bar: Theta Theory

THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF HUMANITIES & SOCIAL STUDIES

Course Outline for Honors Spanish II Mrs. Sharon Koller

Universal Grammar 2. Universal Grammar 1. Forms and functions 1. Universal Grammar 3. Conceptual and surface structure of complex clauses

Today we examine the distribution of infinitival clauses, which can be

AN EXPERIMENTAL APPROACH TO NEW AND OLD INFORMATION IN TURKISH LOCATIVES AND EXISTENTIALS

Heads and history NIGEL VINCENT & KERSTI BÖRJARS The University of Manchester

Inleiding Taalkunde. Docent: Paola Monachesi. Blok 4, 2001/ Syntax 2. 2 Phrases and constituent structure 2. 3 A minigrammar of Italian 3

Progressive Aspect in Nigerian English

Construction Grammar. University of Jena.

The semantics of case *

SAMPLE. Chapter 1: Background. A. Basic Introduction. B. Why It s Important to Teach/Learn Grammar in the First Place

SOME MINIMAL NOTES ON MINIMALISM *

The subject of adjectives: Syntactic position and semantic interpretation

The optimal placement of up and ab A comparison 1

An Introduction to the Minimalist Program

Pye, C The Focus Antipassive in Quiché Mayan, Kansas University Working Papers in Linguistics.

Agree or Move? On Partial Control Anna Snarska, Adam Mickiewicz University

Tibor Kiss Reconstituting Grammar: Hagit Borer's Exoskeletal Syntax 1

The Acquisition of Person and Number Morphology Within the Verbal Domain in Early Greek

Citation for published version (APA): Veenstra, M. J. A. (1998). Formalizing the minimalist program Groningen: s.n.

Emmaus Lutheran School English Language Arts Curriculum

CS 598 Natural Language Processing

Intensive English Program Southwest College

Possessive have and (have) got in New Zealand English Heidi Quinn, University of Canterbury, New Zealand

Phenomena of gender attraction in Polish *

Course Syllabus Advanced-Intermediate Grammar ESOL 0352

Derivational and Inflectional Morphemes in Pak-Pak Language

Intervention in Tough Constructions * Jeremy Hartman. Massachusetts Institute of Technology

Direct and Indirect Passives in East Asian. C.-T. James Huang Harvard University

Chapter 3: Semi-lexical categories. nor truly functional. As Corver and van Riemsdijk rightly point out, There is more

California Department of Education English Language Development Standards for Grade 8

Derivations (MP) and Evaluations (OT) *

AN ANALYSIS OF GRAMMTICAL ERRORS MADE BY THE SECOND YEAR STUDENTS OF SMAN 5 PADANG IN WRITING PAST EXPERIENCES

Linguistic Variation across Sports Category of Press Reportage from British Newspapers: a Diachronic Multidimensional Analysis

Ch VI- SENTENCE PATTERNS.

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

The presence of interpretable but ungrammatical sentences corresponds to mismatches between interpretive and productive parsing.

LEXICAL COHESION ANALYSIS OF THE ARTICLE WHAT IS A GOOD RESEARCH PROJECT? BY BRIAN PALTRIDGE A JOURNAL ARTICLE

Proof Theory for Syntacticians

Backward Raising. Eric Potsdam and Maria Polinsky. automatically qualify as covert movement. We exclude such operations from consideration here.

GERM 3040 GERMAN GRAMMAR AND COMPOSITION SPRING 2017

Natural Language Processing. George Konidaris

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 154 ( 2014 )

Aspectual Classes of Verb Phrases

International Journal of Informative & Futuristic Research ISSN (Online):

Unaccusatives, Resultatives, and the Richness of Lexical Representations

Som and Optimality Theory

Chapter 4: Valence & Agreement CSLI Publications

The Acquisition of English Grammatical Morphemes: A Case of Iranian EFL Learners

Parsing of part-of-speech tagged Assamese Texts

Word Formation is Syntactic: Raising in Nominalizations

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

A construction analysis of [be done X] in Canadian English

Formulaic Language and Fluency: ESL Teaching Applications

Hindi-Urdu Phrase Structure Annotation

Pontificia Universidad Católica del Ecuador Facultad de Comunicación, Lingüística y Literatura Escuela de Lenguas Sección de Inglés

Developing a TT-MCTAG for German with an RCG-based Parser

Discourse markers and grammaticalization

On the Notion Determiner

Multiple Antipassives in Halkomelem Salish

On Labeling: Principle C and Head Movement

Switched Control and other 'uncontrolled' cases of obligatory control

The Syntax of Inner Aspect

How to become passive. Berit Gehrke and Nino Grillo

IS THERE A PASSIVE IN DHOLUO?

THE FU CTIO OF ACCUSATIVE CASE I MO GOLIA *

A First-Pass Approach for Evaluating Machine Translation Systems

Dependency, licensing and the nature of grammatical relations *

The Structure of Multiple Complements to V

a) analyse sentences, so you know what s going on and how to use that information to help you find the answer.

The building blocks of HPSG grammars. Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar (HPSG) HPSG grammars from a linguistic perspective

Compositional Semantics

Structure-Preserving Extraction without Traces

CHILDREN S POSSESSIVE STRUCTURES: A CASE STUDY 1. Andrew Radford and Joseph Galasso, University of Essex

How Does Physical Space Influence the Novices' and Experts' Algebraic Reasoning?

Enhancing Unlexicalized Parsing Performance using a Wide Coverage Lexicon, Fuzzy Tag-set Mapping, and EM-HMM-based Lexical Probabilities

PSIWORLD ª University of Bucharest, Bd. M. Kogalniceanu 36-46, Sector 5, Bucharest, , Romania

Transcription:

MULTIATTACHMENT AND CHAINS IN ROMANIAN GERUND CLAUSES ADRIANA TODEA * ABSTRACT. This paper uses the Relational Grammar account of clitic behaviour to identify functional characteristics of Romanian gerund forms. Such insights on how reflexive clitics are items of verb morphology that mark patterns of clause-internal functional operations are used to give a Generative Grammar account of the presence of the agreement-with-the-object phrase (AgroP) in the functional domain of gerund clauses. The paper explores as well the assumption that pronominal clitics, as they signal the -features of objects, are actually agreement heads. Key words: clitic behaviour, reflexive clitics, functional characteristic Examples [1] and [2] displays the Relational Grammar (RG) distinction between unaccusatives and unergatives: a speria [to frighten] is treated as an unaccusative with optional transitivity, whereas a teme [to fear] is an unergative with optional transitivity. In examples [2a,b] both verbs appear in pseudoreflexive constructions. According to RG, the argument of the unaccusative predicate undergoes retroherent advancemnent (or unaccusative advancement) in order to satisfy the Final 1 Law: Every clause must have a subject in the final stratum. The multiattachment displayed in a non-initial stratum is morphologically marked by the reflexive clitic. The argument of the unergative verb undergoes a retroherent antipassive, that is a retroherent demotion to the 2 (DO) relation. (Blake, pp 29-42) Babe-Bolyai University, Cluj-Napoca, Romania. E-mail: atodea@yahoo.com

ADRIANA TODEA The examples in [2] and [3] show that the Romanian Presumptive displays the (perfect) participle in Perfect Presumptive and the gerund (present participle) in Present Presumptive. The two forms create an aspectual contrast of the perfective vs. imperfective type. The RG analysis of [2] and [3] interprets both participle and gerund as chomeur predicates in predicate unions (La Fauci, pp. 4-5), which is also confirmed by the raising, irrespective of the syntactic type of the initial predicate, of the reflexive clitic se (Davis and Rosen 1988). [3a]Se vor fi temut ei de cîini, (dar au mers înainte.) cl-refl.acc.3 rd pl will be feared they by/of dogs, They might have feared the dogs, (but they went on) [3b]Se vor fi speriat ei de cîini, (dar au mers înainte.) cl-refl.acc.3 rd pl will be frightened they by/of dogs, They might have been afraid of the dogs, (but they went on) [3c]Se vor fi speriind ei uor de cîini, (dar de gini nu le e fric.) cl-refl.acc.3 rd pl will be frightening they easily by/of dogs, They might easily get scared of dogs, (but they are not afraid of hens) [3d]Se vor fi temînd ei de cîini, (dar vor merge înainte.) cl-refl.acc.3 rd pl will be fearing they of dogs, They might fear dogs, but they will go on. 210

MULTIATTACHMENT AND CHAINS IN ROMANIAN GERUND CLAUSES Gerunds complementing verbs of perception, as in the examples in [4], are not predicate chomeurs, but gerund clauses. As they cannot advance to the subject relation in passive constructions, we can assume that gerund clauses in such positions, although initial DO, are put into chomage by the ascension of their inner argument under the Relational Succession Law: A nominal promoted by an ascension rule assumes the grammatical relation borne by the host out of which it ascends (Blake, p. 94). From this point of view they are similar to Romanian supine clauses (Todea, 115-118). Gerund complementing verbs of perception have a clear imperfective aspectual feature, as the contrastive examples gerund vs. participle show and also the comparative finite object clauses constructions in [4] and [5]. An interesting issue is whether the participles in [5a,b,c] are predicates in chomeur participial clauses, or predicate chomeurs in predicate unions in the main clauses. As chomeur participial clauses are marked by the chomeur marker de in Romanian (see Todea), we prefer the predicate union analysis of [5 a,b,c] participial constructions. 211

ADRIANA TODEA Such gerunds as in [6c,d] have been interpreted as IOs in the main clause (Neamu, p.388), but by comparing them with the chomeur participle clauses in [6a,d] (initial subjects put into chomage by the retroherent unaccusative advancement of the arguments of the two unaccusative main clause predicates), they seem to be rather oblique clauses with an imperfective aspectual feature. 212

MULTIATTACHMENT AND CHAINS IN ROMANIAN GERUND CLAUSES Examples [7a,b] show a retroherent unaccusative advancement of the DO of an unaccusative-with-optional-transitivity predicate. 213

ADRIANA TODEA [7c,d] display imperfective gerund clauses complementing verbs of perception, which are also pseudoreflexives due to unaccusative retroherent advancement [7a], and respectively, the possessive reevaluated to IO relation in the higher clause as a result of nominal union (Blake, pp123-127). [7e,f] show perfective oblique gerund clauses with pseudoreflexivity produced by the same mechanisms which are at work in [7c,d]. 214 [7e] Vînzîndu-se florile, (florresele au plecat acas.) selling cl-refl.acc.3 rd sg?/pl? flowers-the, Having sold the flowers, (the flower girls went home.) [7f] Vînzîndu-i florile, (florresele au plecat acas.) selling cl-refl.dat.3 rd pl flowers-the, Having sold their flowers, (the flower girls went home.) [7g] Florile fiind vîndute, (florresele au plecat acas.) Flowers-the being sold, As their flowers had been sold, (the flower girls went home.) [7h] shows the topicalization of the predication in the main clause. The participle clause fails to display the pseudo reflexive clitic.

MULTIATTACHMENT AND CHAINS IN ROMANIAN GERUND CLAUSES Conclusions: RG analysis shows that the Romanian -ind, whether rendering the perfective or the imperfective aspect, in predicate unions or in gerund clauses, retains the pseudoreflexivity generated by the syntactic type of the initial predication (the lexical verb) the participle can appear in reflexive constructions only as a predicate chomeur in a predicate union. Chomeur participle clauses (the supine) lack reflexive clitics. The gerund clause must contain a functional feature that allows the realization of clitics, whereas participial clauses lack such a feature. 215

Powered by TCPDF (www.tcpdf.org) ADRIANA TODEA Proposal for a Generative Grammar interpretation of reflexivity (true or pseudo): The functional phrase proposed to host the reflexive clitic is AgroP. The Romanian gerund has been long thought to render some agreement feature, as in traditional Romanian analyses there has been a long debate on whether the gerund is a personal or a non-personal mood/ form (Hazi, 81-101). In [8] we propose two structures (one a true, the other a pseudo reflexive generated by an unaccusative verb) in which the VP shell complements the Agro functional head. We also propose a (Spec-vP, sister-of-v) Equi-NP Deletion type of chain for the structure displaying true reflexivity, and a chain generated by Spec-VP to Spec-vP NP-movement for the unaccusative structure (Radford, 240-246) We propose a merger between the lexical verb and the reflexive clitic under the Agro head, as the verb undergoes V-movement. The NP in spec-vp moves from the VP shell into the functional domain, of which the first landing position is spec-agrop. The clitic checks the -features [number, person, gender] of the nominal in spec-agrop. Assuming that the reflexive clitic has the [+anaphor] referential feature, Binding theory checks as the reflexive clitic is bound by the NP in Spec-AgroP in its local domain. From the fact that the gerund clause allows the realization of clitics, whereas participial clauses (supine ones) fail to preserve them, we can conclude that, in Romanian, gerund clauses display an agreement with the object phrase in their functional domain, whereas participial clauses do not. 216 BIBLIOGRAPHY 1. Avram, Larisa (2004) An Aspectual Analysis of Gerunds 2. Blake, Barry J. (1990) Relational Grammar, Routledge, London & New York 3. Davies, William and Carol Rosen (1988) Unions as Multi-predicate clauses, Language 64: 52-88 4. Gramatica Academiei (1963) Editura Academiei, Bucureti 5. Hazi, tefan (1997) Predicativitatea: Determinare contextual analitic, Editura Dacia, Cluj- Napoca 6. Neamu, G. G. (1999) Teoria i practica analizei gramaticale, Editura Excelsior, Cluj- Napoca 7. Perlmutter, David (1989) Multiattachment and the Unaccusative Hypothesis: The Perfect Auxiliary in Italian, Probus 1.1: 63-119 8. Perlmutter, David and Paul Postal (1984) Some Proposed Laws of Basic Clause Sructure, Studies in Relational Grammar 1, University of Chicago Press 9. Radford, Andrew (1999) Syntax. A minimalist introduction, Cambridge University Press 10. Rosen, Carol (1984) The Interface between Semantic Roles and Initial Grammatical Relations, in Perlmutter and Rosen (eds.), studies in Relational Grammar 2, Uchi Press 11. Todea, Adriana (2004) The Romanian Supine Complementing Adjectives, Adverbs, verbs, Modals, and Copulas in Studia Universitatis Babe-Bolyai. Philologia, no.4/2004, Babes-Bolyai University