EVIDENCE FOR A LEARNING BIAS AGAINST SALTATORY PHONOLOGICAL ALTERNATIONS IN ARTIFICIAL LANGUAGE LEARNING. James White, UCLA Department of Linguistics

Similar documents
Parallel Evaluation in Stratal OT * Adam Baker University of Arizona

Universal contrastive analysis as a learning principle in CAPT

Canadian raising with language-specific weighted constraints Joe Pater, University of Massachusetts Amherst

Revisiting the role of prosody in early language acquisition. Megha Sundara UCLA Phonetics Lab

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

Pobrane z czasopisma New Horizons in English Studies Data: 18/11/ :52:20. New Horizons in English Studies 1/2016

**Note: this is slightly different from the original (mainly in format). I would be happy to send you a hard copy.**

Manner assimilation in Uyghur

Acoustic correlates of stress and their use in diagnosing syllable fusion in Tongan. James White & Marc Garellek UCLA

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Linguistics 220 Phonology: distributions and the concept of the phoneme. John Alderete, Simon Fraser University

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

The Perception of Nasalized Vowels in American English: An Investigation of On-line Use of Vowel Nasalization in Lexical Access

Markedness and Complex Stops: Evidence from Simplification Processes 1. Nick Danis Rutgers University

Precedence Constraints and Opacity

The analysis starts with the phonetic vowel and consonant charts based on the dataset:

The phonological grammar is probabilistic: New evidence pitting abstract representation against analogy

To appear in the Proceedings of the 35th Meetings of the Chicago Linguistics Society. Post-vocalic spirantization: Typology and phonetic motivations

Language Acquisition by Identical vs. Fraternal SLI Twins * Karin Stromswold & Jay I. Rifkin

Underlying Representations

Contrastiveness and diachronic variation in Chinese nasal codas. Tsz-Him Tsui The Ohio State University

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Voiced-voiceless distinction in alaryngeal speech - acoustic and articula

Lexical phonology. Marc van Oostendorp. December 6, Until now, we have presented phonological theory as if it is a monolithic

Language Acquisition Fall 2010/Winter Lexical Categories. Afra Alishahi, Heiner Drenhaus

Dyslexia/dyslexic, 3, 9, 24, 97, 187, 189, 206, 217, , , 367, , , 397,

A. True B. False INVENTORY OF PROCESSES IN COLLEGE COMPOSITION

A Level Playing-Field: Perceptibility and Inflection in English Compounds. Robert Kirchner and Elena Nicoladis (U. Alberta)

Books Effective Literacy Y5-8 Learning Through Talk Y4-8 Switch onto Spelling Spelling Under Scrutiny

English for Life. B e g i n n e r. Lessons 1 4 Checklist Getting Started. Student s Book 3 Date. Workbook. MultiROM. Test 1 4

Learners Use Word-Level Statistics in Phonetic Category Acquisition

raıs Factors affecting word learning in adults: A comparison of L2 versus L1 acquisition /r/ /aı/ /s/ /r/ /aı/ /s/ = individual sound

SOME MINIMAL NOTES ON MINIMALISM *

SARDNET: A Self-Organizing Feature Map for Sequences

Approaches to control phenomena handout Obligatory control and morphological case: Icelandic and Basque

SOUND STRUCTURE REPRESENTATION, REPAIR AND WELL-FORMEDNESS: GRAMMAR IN SPOKEN LANGUAGE PRODUCTION. Adam B. Buchwald

Sounds of Infant-Directed Vocabulary: Learned from Infants Speech or Part of Linguistic Knowledge?

An Acoustic Phonetic Account of the Production of Word-Final /z/s in Central Minnesota English

CLASSIFICATION OF PROGRAM Critical Elements Analysis 1. High Priority Items Phonemic Awareness Instruction

Consonants: articulation and transcription

Consonant-Vowel Unity in Element Theory*

The presence of interpretable but ungrammatical sentences corresponds to mismatches between interpretive and productive parsing.

The Odd-Parity Parsing Problem 1 Brett Hyde Washington University May 2008

Speech Recognition using Acoustic Landmarks and Binary Phonetic Feature Classifiers

An argument from speech pathology

Guidelines for Writing an Internship Report

Stochastic Phonology Janet B. Pierrehumbert Department of Linguistics Northwestern University Evanston, IL Introduction

Different Task Type and the Perception of the English Interdental Fricatives

Introduction to HPSG. Introduction. Historical Overview. The HPSG architecture. Signature. Linguistic Objects. Descriptions.

have to be modeled) or isolated words. Output of the system is a grapheme-tophoneme conversion system which takes as its input the spelling of words,

Listener-oriented phonology

Infants learn phonotactic regularities from brief auditory experience

Probabilistic principles in unsupervised learning of visual structure: human data and a model

Spanish progressive aspect in stochastic OT

Module 12. Machine Learning. Version 2 CSE IIT, Kharagpur

PREP S SPEAKER LISTENER TECHNIQUE COACHING MANUAL

Radical CV Phonology: the locational gesture *

Achievement Level Descriptors for American Literature and Composition

A Neural Network GUI Tested on Text-To-Phoneme Mapping

Partial Class Behavior and Nasal Place Assimilation*

Developing Grammar in Context

Florida Reading Endorsement Alignment Matrix Competency 1

Essentials of Ability Testing. Joni Lakin Assistant Professor Educational Foundations, Leadership, and Technology

Laporan Penelitian Unggulan Prodi

Age Effects on Syntactic Control in. Second Language Learning

First Grade Curriculum Highlights: In alignment with the Common Core Standards

DOWNSTEP IN SUPYIRE* Robert Carlson Societe Internationale de Linguistique, Mali

PAGE(S) WHERE TAUGHT If sub mission ins not a book, cite appropriate location(s))

The Effect of Discourse Markers on the Speaking Production of EFL Students. Iman Moradimanesh

Year 4 National Curriculum requirements

Journal of Phonetics

1. REFLEXES: Ask questions about coughing, swallowing, of water as fast as possible (note! Not suitable for all

Learning Methods in Multilingual Speech Recognition

Sample Goals and Benchmarks

ELA/ELD Standards Correlation Matrix for ELD Materials Grade 1 Reading

Arizona s English Language Arts Standards th Grade ARIZONA DEPARTMENT OF EDUCATION HIGH ACADEMIC STANDARDS FOR STUDENTS

Linguistics Program Outcomes Assessment 2012

5. Margi (Chadic, Nigeria): H, L, R (Williams 1973, Hoffmann 1963)

Knowledge Transfer in Deep Convolutional Neural Nets

NAME: East Carolina University PSYC Developmental Psychology Dr. Eppler & Dr. Ironsmith

A Bootstrapping Model of Frequency and Context Effects in Word Learning

Preliminary Report Initiative for Investigation of Race Matters and Underrepresented Minority Faculty at MIT Revised Version Submitted July 12, 2007

The Acquisition of Person and Number Morphology Within the Verbal Domain in Early Greek

Acquiring Competence from Performance Data

Using a Native Language Reference Grammar as a Language Learning Tool

Phonological Processing for Urdu Text to Speech System

I propose an analysis of thorny patterns of reduplication in the unrelated languages Saisiyat

Joan Bybee, Phonology and Language Use. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2001,

The Strong Minimalist Thesis and Bounded Optimality

Tutoring First-Year Writing Students at UNM

Derivational and Inflectional Morphemes in Pak-Pak Language

1 st Quarter (September, October, November) August/September Strand Topic Standard Notes Reading for Literature

BSID-II-NL project. Heidelberg March Selma Ruiter, University of Groningen

The Journey to Vowelerria VOWEL ERRORS: THE LOST WORLD OF SPEECH INTERVENTION. Preparation: Education. Preparation: Education. Preparation: Education

Houghton Mifflin Reading Correlation to the Common Core Standards for English Language Arts (Grade1)

Som and Optimality Theory

Lexical Access during Sentence Comprehension (Re)Consideration of Context Effects

MGMT3403 Leadership Second Semester

AN EXPERIMENTAL APPROACH TO NEW AND OLD INFORMATION IN TURKISH LOCATIVES AND EXISTENTIALS

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

Transcription:

EVIDENCE FOR A LEARNING BIAS AGAINST SALTATORY PHONOLOGICAL ALTERNATIONS IN ARTIFICIAL LANGUAGE LEARNING James White, UCLA Department of Linguistics

Saltatory phonological alternations 2 Saltatory alternation = alternation in which an intervening sound is jumped over Example from Campidanian Sardinian 1 : p / V, but b remains unchanged /pani/ /binu/ [s:u βãi] the bread [s:u bĩu] the wine p b This is a productive process that occurs at the other places of articulation as well. 1. Bolognesi, 1998

More saltatory alternations 3 Some other examples: Colloquial Northern German 1 g x / #, k remains unchanged Polish 2 g ʒ / V +front, ʤ remains unchanged Suma (a tonal example) 3 L H / H # in associative construction, final M remains unchanged Note that these other cases are more limited in nature. 1. Ito & Mester, 2003 2. Lubowicz, 2002 3. Bradshaw, 1998

Research question 4 Thus, saltatory alternations are possible, but crosslinguistically rare (at least relative to non-saltatory ones). Question: Do learners have a bias against learning saltatory alternations? I will present 4 artificial language experiments with interesting results indicating that they do.

Overview (Experiments 1-4) 5 Artificial language learning (n = 20 for all experiments) Basic design strategy: Withhold certain information during exposure (ambiguous input), then test on the withheld cases to see which assumptions participants make 1 Same basic method for Exp 1-4, but types of items in training varies 3 phases: Exposure Verification of learning Generalization 1. E.g., see Wilson, 2006; Finley, 2008; and others

Experiment 1- Method 6 Artificial language learning (Auditory) Exposure phase: Train on p v, t ð / V V

Experiment 1 7 Artificial language learning (Auditory) Exposure phase: Train on p v, t ð / V V

Experiment 1 8 Artificial language learning (Auditory) Exposure phase: Train on p v, t ð / V V lanap

Experiment 1 9 Artificial language learning (Auditory) Exposure phase: Train on p v, t ð / V V

Experiment 1 10 Artificial language learning (Auditory) Exposure phase: Train on p v, t ð / V V lanavi

Experiment 1 11 Exposure phase: Train on p v, t ð / V V All singular words are CVCVC, sound inventory drawn from a subset of English phonemes 36 changing items ending in /p/ or /t/ lanap ~ lanavi (18 of this type) bunat ~ bunaði (18 of this type) 36 non-changing Filler items Ending in /m, n, l, r, s, ʃ/ Example: kasam ~ kasami Crucially, no words ending in intervening /b, d, f, /

Experiment 1 12 Verification phase: Did they learn the pattern? Task: Hear a previously heard singular form and choose the correct plural form 2-alternative forced choice test Choose between two options: changing and non-changing. 32 words from Exposure phase (8 p, 8 t, 16 fillers) Must get at least 80% to move on so that I know they have learned the pattern

Experiment 1 13 Verification phase: Did they learn the pattern? Task: Hear a previously heard singular form and choose the correct plural form

Experiment 1 14 Verification phase: Did they learn the pattern? Task: Hear a previously heard singular form and choose the correct plural form lanap

Experiment 1 15 Verification phase: Did they learn the pattern? Task: Hear a previously heard singular form and choose the correct plural form

Experiment 1 16 Verification phase: Did they learn the pattern? Task: Hear a previously heard singular form and choose the correct plural form lanapi... lanavi??????????????

Experiment 1 17 Verification phase: Did they learn the pattern? Task: Hear a previously heard singular form and choose the correct plural form Note: Changing option for fillers: /m, r, ʃ/ v (kasami... kasavi) /n, l, s/ ð lanapi... lanavi??????????????

Experiment 1 - Method 18 Generalization phase: Same task as verification phase, but with novel words. 24 words ending in /p, t/ 24 fillers But crucially also 24 words ending in the untrained, intervening sounds /b, d, f, /

Experiment 1 19 Input: p t v ð Possible interpretations of input: (Coronals analogous) p b p b p b p b f v f v f v f v Saltatory Partially saltatory Non-saltatory

Experiment 1 Results (all words are novel) 20 Trained sounds Untrained sounds 100 Mean % changing option chosen 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 p, t Fillers b, d f, Final sound of singular word

Experiment 1 Results (all words are novel) 21 Mean % changing option chosen 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 Trained sounds Untrained sounds E.g., for a word like lanap, how frequently did participants choose lanavi rather than lanapi? 0 p, t Fillers b, d f, Final sound of singular word

Experiment 1 Results (all words are novel) 22 Mean % changing option chosen 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 Trained sounds Untrained sounds Participants learned pattern and extended it to new forms of the same type. 0 p, t Fillers b, d f, Final sound of singular word

Experiment 1 Results (all words are novel) 23 Trained sounds Untrained sounds Mean % changing option chosen 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 Participants generalized to intervening sounds at a high rate, even with no evidence for such a change! 0 p, t Fillers b, d f, Final sound of singular word

Experiment 1 Results (all words are novel) 24 Trained sounds Untrained sounds Mean % changing option chosen 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 * * Participants generalized to intervening sounds at a high rate, even with no evidence for such a change. 0 p, t Fillers b, d f, Final sound of singular word

Experiment 1 Results (all words are novel) 25 Trained sounds Untrained sounds Mean % changing option chosen 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 * Greater change for intervening stops than for fricatives! 0 p, t Fillers b, d f, Final sound of singular word

Observations so far 26 Given ambiguous input, learners generalize to make learned alternations non-saltatory. There is a preference towards changing voiced stops more than voiceless fricatives. Binary abstract features cannot account for this difference Perhaps perceptual similarity is important Labials Coronals Sounds Confusability/ Similarity b ~ v.153 f ~ v.039 d ~ ð.103 ~ ð.029

Observations so far 27 Given ambiguous input, learners generalize to make learned alternations non-saltatory. There is a preference towards changing voiced stops more than voiceless fricatives. Binary abstract features cannot account for this difference Perhaps perceptual similarity is important Labials Coronals Sounds Confusability/ Similarity b ~ v.153 f ~ v.039 d ~ ð.103 ~ ð.029 = avg. of (rate that b is mistaken for v and rate that v is mistaken for b) (from confusion matrix data 1 ) 1. Wang & Bilger, 1973

Observations so far 28 Given ambiguous input, learners generalize to make learned alternations non-saltatory. There is a preference towards changing voiced stops more than voiceless fricatives. Binary abstract features cannot account for this difference Perhaps perceptual similarity is important Labials Coronals Sounds Confusability/ Similarity b ~ v.153 f ~ v.039 d ~ ð.103 ~ ð.029 Indeed, voiced stops [b, d] are more confusable with voiced fricative targets [v, ð] than voiceless fricatives [f, ].

Two alternate explanations 29 They just learned a more general rule: all stops become voiced fricatives between vowels Product-oriented responses: 1 large number of [-vi] and [-ði] plural endings resulted in a bias towards choosing those endings for new cases ½ of the plurals ended in [-vi] or [-ði] 1/12 ended in each of [-mi], [-ni], [-li], [-ri], [-si], [-ʃi] 2. Bybee & Slobin, 1982

Experiment 2 - Control 30 Train on b v and d ð, withhold p, t, f,. Designed to address alternate explanations: If learning a more general rule or responding based on product-oriented schema, then effect should remain. If it is really something about the intervening sound, then the effect should be greatly reduced.

Experiment 2 - Control 31 Input: b d Expected behavior: v ð (Coronals analogous) p b p b p b p b or f v f v f v f v More general rule (Similar to Exp 1) Product-oriented responses (Similar to Exp 1) Bias against saltations (Different from Exp 1)

Experiment 2 - Control 32 Input: b d Expected behavior: v ð Little generalization to other sounds p b p b p b p b or f v f v f v f v More general rule (Similar to Exp 1) Product-oriented responses (Similar to Exp 1) Bias against saltations (Different from Exp 1)

Experiment 2 Results 33 Trained sounds Untrained sounds Mean % changing option chosen 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 Trained stops Fillers Untrained stops Untrained fricatives Final sound of singular word Exp 1 Exp 2: Control

Experiment 2 Results 34 Trained sounds Untrained sounds Mean % changing option chosen 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 Exp 1 Exp 2: Control Learned trained pattern equally well 0 Trained stops Fillers Untrained stops Untrained fricatives Final sound of singular word

Experiment 2 Results 35 Trained sounds Untrained sounds Mean % changing option chosen 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 Exp 1 Exp 2: Control Untrained generalization enormously reduced! 0 Trained stops Fillers Untrained stops Untrained fricatives Final sound of singular word

Experiment 2 Results 36 Trained sounds Untrained sounds Mean % changing option chosen 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 Exp 1 Exp 2: Control * 2 x 2 ANOVA: Sig. main effect of Exp for untrained sounds (Type x Exp) 0 Trained stops Fillers Untrained stops Untrained fricatives Final sound of singular word

Experiment 2 Results 37 Trained sounds Untrained sounds Mean % changing option chosen 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 * * Exp 1 Exp 2: Control Trained stops Fillers Untrained stops Untrained fricatives Still sig. different than trained fillers can think of this as the basic effect of being trained vs. untrained Final sound of singular word

Observations so far 38 Given ambiguous input, learners generalize to make learned alternations non-saltatory. This effect cannot be explained by participants learning a general rule or by product-oriented responses. There is a preference towards changing voiced stops more than voiceless fricatives. Binary abstract features cannot account for this difference Perhaps perceptual similarity is important

Experiment 3 Blocked stops 39 Train participants on p v and t ð, but also that b and d do not change In training: 18 p v 18 t ð 18 non-changing b, d (9 of each) 18 non-changing fillers Nothing about f, Input: p b (Coronals analogous) f v

Experiment 3 Blocked stops 40 Train participants on p v and t ð, but also that b and d do not change In training: 18 p v 18 t ð 18 non-changing b, d (9 of each) 18 non-changing fillers Nothing about f, Input: p b Equal # of non-changing fillers and non-changing b, d (Coronals analogous) f v

Experiment 3 Blocked stops 41 Prediction: If there is bias against saltatory alternations % changing option for fricatives /f, / should remain high Input: p b (Coronals analogous) f v

Experiment 3 Results 42 Trained Sounds Untrained or Blocked Sounds Mean % changing option chosen 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 Trained Trained stops Fillers Untrained/ Blocked stops Final sound of singular word Exp 1 Exp 2: Control Exp 3: Blocked stops Untrained fricatives

Experiment 3 Results 43 Trained Sounds Untrained or Blocked Sounds Mean % changing option chosen 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 Trained Exp 1 Exp 2: Control Exp 3: Blocked stops Learned trained pattern equally well 0 Trained stops Fillers Untrained/ Blocked stops Untrained fricatives Final sound of singular word

Experiment 3 Results 44 Trained Sounds Untrained or Blocked Sounds Mean % changing option chosen 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 Trained Exp 1 Exp 2: Control Exp 3: Blocked stops No difference in generalization to untrained fricatives between Exp 1 and Exp 3 0 Trained stops Fillers Untrained/ Blocked stops Untrained fricatives Final sound of singular word

Experiment 3 Results 45 Trained Sounds Untrained or Blocked Sounds Mean % changing option chosen 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 * Trained Exp 1 Exp 2: Control Exp 3: Blocked stops Sig. more mistakes on blocked stops than on fillers despite being trained to not change stops! 0 Trained stops Fillers Untrained/ Blocked stops Untrained fricatives Final sound of singular word

Experiment 3 Results 46 Trained Sounds Untrained or Blocked Sounds Mean % changing option chosen 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 Trained stops Fillers Untrained/ Blocked stops * Trained Exp 1 Exp 2: Control Exp 3: Blocked stops Untrained fricatives Equal to Exp 2 even though Exp 3 is trained and Exp 2 is untrained! Final sound of singular word

Experiment 4 Blocked Fricatives 47 Same as Exp 3, but the fricatives are blocked instead of the stops Will we see the same pattern? Input: p b (Coronals analogous) f v

Experiment 4 Results 48 Trained Sounds Untrained or Blocked Sounds 100 Exp 1 Mean % changing option chosen 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 Trained Exp 2: Control Exp 3: Blocked stops Exp 4: Blocked frics Trained 0 Trained stops Fillers Untrained/ Blocked stops Untrained/ Blocked frics Final sound of singular word

Experiment 4 Results 49 Trained Sounds Untrained or Blocked Sounds 100 Exp 1 Mean % changing option chosen 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 Trained Exp 2: Control Exp 3: Blocked stops Exp 4: Blocked frics Trained Learned trained pattern equally well 0 Trained stops Fillers Untrained/ Blocked stops Untrained/ Blocked frics Final sound of singular word

Experiment 4 Results 50 Trained Sounds Untrained or Blocked Sounds 100 Exp 1 Mean % changing option chosen 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 Trained Exp 2: Control Exp 3: Blocked stops Exp 4: Blocked frics Trained Untrained stops equal to Exp 1 0 Trained stops Fillers Untrained/ Blocked stops Untrained/ Blocked frics Final sound of singular word

Experiment 4 Results 51 Trained Sounds Untrained or Blocked Sounds Mean % changing option chosen 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 * Trained Exp 1 Exp 2: Control Exp 3: Blocked stops Exp 4: Blocked frics Trained Sig. more errors on blocked frics than on fillers despite being trained to not change frics! 0 Trained stops Fillers Untrained/ Blocked stops Untrained/ Blocked frics Final sound of singular word

Experiment 4 Results 52 Trained Sounds Untrained or Blocked Sounds Mean % changing option chosen 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 Trained Exp 1 Exp 2: Control Exp 3: Blocked stops Exp 4: Blocked frics Trained Greater than Exp 2 even though Exp 4 is trained and Exp 2 is untrained! 0 Trained stops Fillers Untrained/ Blocked stops Untrained/ Blocked frics Final sound of singular word

Observations so far 53 Given ambiguous input, learners generalize to make learned alternations non-saltatory. This effect cannot be explained by participants learning a general rule or by product-oriented responses. There is a preference towards changing voiced stops more than voiceless fricatives. Binary abstract features cannot account for this difference Perhaps perceptual similarity is important Even when learners are trained that intervening sounds should not change, they have a tendency to change them to make the alternation non-saltatory.

Theoretical Implications 54 What do we know?

Theoretical Implications 55 What do we know? Natural languages exist with saltatory alternations. So phonological theory must be able to generate grammars that allow saltatory alternations. Even this is not totally straightforward (e.g., classical OT 1 cannot handle them). 1. Prince & Smolensky, 1993/2004

Theoretical Implications 56 What do we know? Natural languages exist with saltatory alternations. So phonological theory must be able to generate grammars that allow saltatory alternations. Even this is not totally straightforward (e.g., standard OT 1 cannot handle them). Saltatory alternations are relatively rare and I have shown that learners are biased against learning a system containing them. So our theories of phonological learning should account for why these alternations are dispreferred in learning 1. Prince & Smolensky, 1993/2004

Nature of the bias 57 Substantive bias 1 Steriade s P-map 2 principle seems to be a good basis for such a bias in this case (at least for a starting point) P(erceptual)-map Humans are aware of perceptual relationships between sounds (in a given context) and alternations should minimize perceptual changes Accounts for a preference for short distance changes over long distance changes Also accounts nicely for the preference in Exp 1 to change b v more than f v (b is more perceptually similar to v). 1. E.g., Wilson, 2006; Finley & Badecker, 2008; etc. 2. Steriade 2001/2008

Nature of the bias 58 Preliminary computational modeling looks promising for the P-map: Maximum Entropy grammar learning 1 with weighted constraints banning relevant alternating pairs (e.g., *p~v) Input/test items based on experiments With a prior (= bias) based on the P-map, the model does pretty well; the unbiased model fails Is P-map sufficient? Further experiments/modeling will help determine whether something else has a role (e.g., general dispreference for saltation that is more than just perceptual distance) 1. Goldwater & Johnson, 2003

Future directions 59 More computational modeling Will help explore what types of biases work and make predictions for additional experiments Open response/production experiments Infant study Do infants display a bias against saltation when learning phonological alternations? Will help determine if this bias is operational in child language acquisition

Conclusions 60 Learners are biased against learning saltatory alternations When trained on alternations that are (potentially) saltatory, they make assumptions/errors that make them not saltatory Perceptual similarity appears to play a role in this bias A substantive bias based on the P-map seems like a promising starting point for modeling the effect

Thank you! 61 Acknowledgments: For much helpful discussion: Bruce Hayes, Megha Sundara, Robert Daland, Kie Zuraw, Sharon Peperkamp, Marc Garellek, Karen Campbell UCLA Language Acquisition Lab managers: Kristi Hendrickson, Chad Vicenik My undergraduate RAs: Kelly Ryan, Kelly Nakawatase, Ariel Quist UCLA Language Acquisition Lab RAs UCLA Phonology seminar audiences Research funded in part by a UCLA Summer Research Mentorship Fellowship

References Bolognesi, R. (1998). The phonology of Campidanian Sardinian: A unitary account of self- organizing structure. The Hague: Holland Academics. Bradshaw, M. (1998). Tone alternation in the associative construction of Suma. In I. Maddieson & T. J. Hinnebusch (Eds.), Language History and Linguistic Description in Africa (pp. 117-125). Trenton: Africa World Press. Bybee, J. L., & Slobin, D. I. (1982). Rules and schemas in the development and use of the English past tense. Language, 58(2), 265-289. Finley, S., & Badecker, W. (2008). Substantive biases for vowel harmony languages. In J. Bishop (Ed.), Proceedings of the West Coast Conference on Formal Lingusitics 27, 168-176. Goldwater, S. J. and M. Johnson (2003). Learning OT constraint rankings using a maximum entropy model.in J. Spenader, A. Erkisson, and O. Dahl (Eds.), Proceedings of the Stockholm Workshop on Variation 16within Optimality Theory, pp. 111 120. Ito, J., & Mester, A. (2003). On the sources of opacity in OT: Coda processes in German. In C. Féry & R. van de Vijver (Eds.), The Syllable in Optimality Theory (pp. 271-303), Cambridge: Cambrige Univ. Press. Lubowicz, A. (2002). Derived environment effects in Optimality Theory. Lingua, 112, 243-280. Prince, A., & Smolensky, P. (1993/2004). Optimality theory: Constraint interaction in generative grammar. Cambridge: Blackwell. [Published in 2004. First circulated in 1993]. Moreton, E. (2008). Analytic bias and phonological typology. Phonology, 25(1), 83-127. Moreton, E. (2010). Constraint induction and and simplicity bias in phonotactic learning. Handout. Moreton, E., & Pater, J. (submitted). Learning artificial phonology: a review. Ms. Steriade, D. (2001/2008). The phonology of perceptibility effects: the P-map and its consequences for constraint organization. In S. Inkelas & K. Hanson (Eds.), The Nature of the Word: Studies in Honor of Paul Kiparsky (pp. 151-180). Cambridge: MIT Press. [Published in 2008. Originally circulated as ms. in 2001.] Wang, M. D., & Bilger, R. (1973). Consonant confusions in noise. JASA, 54, 1248-66. Wilson, C. (2006). Learning phonology with substantive bias: An experimental and computational study of velar palatalization. Cognitive Science, 30, 945-982. 62